Introduction

In many social insects, especially highly eusocial species, queens are known to maintain reproductive monopoly by means of chemical signals or pheromones (Winston and Slessor, 1992; Katzav-Gozansky et al., 2002; D’Ettorre et al., 2004; Monnin, 2006). There is growing evidence for chemical signalling of reproductive status in primitively eusocial species as well (Bonavita-Cougourdan et al., 1991; Peeters et al., 1999; Liebig et al., 2000; Sledge et al., 2001; Dapporto et al., 2005; Sramkova et al., 2008). Queen pheromones have been proposed as honest signals of fertility by which a queen signals her fertility status to her workers (Keller and Nonacs, 1993; Heinze and D’Ettorre, 2009; van Zweden, 2010). Thus workers perceive the fertility of their queen through the queen pheromone and are selected to refrain from reproduction. When the fertility of a queen declines, she can be overthrown or even killed by her workers (Engels and Imperatriz-Fonseca, 1990). This implies that pheromone production should be correlated with the state of fertility of an individual.

Solitary foundresses in Ropalidia marginata, individuals who found small nests and raise brood alone, provide an interesting system to study the honesty of queen signalling. Since solitary foundresses do not have any nestmates, expressing a queen signal is redundant, since there is no one to receive the signal. But, if the pheromone is an honest signal arising as an inevitable byproduct of the state of fertility, i.e. inextricably linked to the fertility of an individual, expression of pheromone and pheromone composition should necessarily be correlated with the degree of fertility of the individual (Keller and Nonacs, 1993; Heinze and D’Ettorre, 2009; van Zweden, 2010). Such a signal (“index” or “constraint”) should be produced by any fertile individual, irrespective of the presence or absence or receivers of the signal (nestmates), by virtue of being physiologically linked to fertility (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1995; Searcy and Nowicki, 2005). A fertile individual cannot help but produce the signal due to this physiological constraint.

The R. marginata queen is remarkably docile and non-interactive, and thereby unlike queens of other primitively eusocial wasps, she cannot use dominance behaviour or other behavioural interactions to maintain reproductive monopoly (Gadagkar, 2001; Bhadra et al., 2007). The Dufour’s gland in R. marginata has been shown to be at least one source of the queen pheromone by which queens appear to maintain reproductive monopoly (Bhadra et al., 2010; Mitra et al., 2011), presumably by applying the Dufour’s gland contents on the nest surface (Bhadra et al., 2007). The Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition has also been shown to be correlated with ovarian activation status in queens (Mitra and Gadagkar, 2011). Here we compared the Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition and ovaries of solitary foundresses with those of queens and workers of normal post-emergence nests in an attempt to provide an independent line of evidence for the honesty of the queen signal. Our objective was to investigate whether the queen pheromone composition depends primarily on the state of ovarian activation, irrespective of the presence or absence of nestmates (receivers of the queen signal). We found that solitary foundresses were intermediate between queens and workers with respect to both their Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition and their ovaries, suggesting that queen pheromone composition in this species can be a byproduct of the state of fertility and hence may honestly signal fertility. This study specifically looks at fertility signalling by social insect queens in a naturally occurring solitary condition during the nest initiation stage and provides new evidence supporting the honest signal hypothesis.

Materials and methods

Eight solitary foundresses were used in this study. All of them had founded small nests naturally in and around the vespiary at the Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore (13°00′N and 77°32′E), India, and had laid some eggs. We compared these solitary foundresses with 12 queens and 36 workers from post-emergence colonies (colony sizes ranging from 7 to 20 individuals) collected from various localities in Bangalore. R. marginata has perennial colonies which can be founded and abandoned throughout the year, and the queen is the sole egg layer in a colony (Gadagkar, 2001).

Gas chromatographic analysis of Dufour’s gland

We analysed the Dufour’s glands by gas chromatography following Mitra et al. (2011). Identification of compounds had been done earlier by mass spectrometric analyses (Bhadra et al., 2010). We matched the retention times and pattern of peaks with those obtained in earlier analyses; for statistical analyses, we considered only those peaks that had been identified earlier.

For univariate analysis, we converted the area under each peak in an individual to percentage by dividing with area under all peaks in that individual. Kruskal–Wallis test was done followed by Mann–Whitney U tests to compare solitary foundresses, queens and workers with respect to percent area under peaks 1–4 pooled together (Heneicosane, 11-methylheneicosane, Tricosane and 11-methyltricosane, respectively), and for percent area under all remaining peaks pooled together, following Mitra and Gadagkar (2011).

For multivariate analysis, we used peaks present in at least 70% of all samples. As a result, peak # 20a (Hentriacontane) got eliminated from multivariate analysis. The area under each peak was square root transformed followed by log ratio transformation (Bhadra et al., 2010; Mitra et al., 2011) and the transformed areas subjected to discriminant analysis to see if solitary foundresses cluster with queens or with workers based on their Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition.

Ovarian measurements

After dissecting the Dufour’s gland, we dissected the ovaries of each wasp and measured them, calculated an ovarian index from the measurements, and considered the score of each individual wasp on the ovarian index as a proxy for fertility following Mitra and Gadagkar (2011). Comparisons of ovarian indices of solitary foundresses, queens and workers were done by Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Mann–Whitney U tests.

Statistical analyses used the software package StatistiXL 1.7.

Results

Gas chromatographic analysis showed that Dufour’s gland profiles of solitary foundresses were qualitatively similar to queens and workers (Online Resource 1). About 34 peaks were found in total. All 34 peaks had been identified earlier (Bhadra et al., 2010; Mitra et al., 2011) and were used here in statistical analyses.

Comparisons using compounds that differ significantly between queens and workers

Queens have been shown to have consistently lower percent area than workers for peaks 1, 2, 3 and 4, (Heneicosane, 11-methylheneicosane, Tricosane and 11-methyltricosane, respectively) and have consistently higher percent area than workers for the remaining peaks (Mitra and Gadagkar, 2011). The percent area under these peaks have also been found to be correlated with ovarian index of queens, implying that such differences may be important in defining the queen signal (Mitra and Gadagkar, 2011). For percent area under peaks 1, 2, 3 and 4, Kruskal–Wallis test showed that variation between groups was higher than variation within groups (F 2,52 = 29.664, P < 0.0001). Solitary foundresses were intermediate—lower than workers but higher than queens (Mann–Whitney U test, U = 210, P = 0.022 for comparison with workers and U = 92, P < 0.001 for comparison with queens, respectively). Queens were lower than workers, as found previously (U = 429, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1a). Similarly, for percent area under the remaining peaks, Kruskal–Wallis test showed that variation between groups was higher than variation within groups (F 2,52 = 29.664, P < 0.0001). Solitary foundresses were higher than workers and lower than queens (U = 210, P = 0.022 for comparison with workers and U = 92, P < 0.001 for comparison with queens, respectively). Queens were higher than workers, as found previously (U = 429, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1b).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Percent area under peaks 1, 2, 3 and 4, b percent area under the remaining peaks, and c ovarian index of workers (w), solitary foundresses (sf) and queens (q) of Ropalidia marginata. Different letters denote significant difference between distributions for each variable (n = 36 w, 12 q, 8 sf, Mann–Whitney U test: P < 0.05)

Comparisons using all compounds simultaneously

Solitary foundresses could be differentiated from queens and workers by discriminant analysis using the transformed areas under all peaks (Fig. 2a). Both discriminant functions 1 and 2 were significant and all individuals were classified correctly (function 1: Wilk’s λ = 0.014, P < 0.001; function 2: Wilk’s λ = 0.236, P = 0.010; classification analysis: 100% correct classification). Peak # 20a (Hentriacontane) was left out of Euclidean distance between points on the discriminant functions plot (Fig. 2a) showed that variation in distances between groups were higher than variation in distances among groups (Kruskal–Wallis test: F 2,812 = 347.466, P < 0.0001). Distances between solitary foundresses and queens were not different from distances between solitary foundresses and workers (Mann–Whitney U = 14,915, P = 0.247). Distances between queens and workers were higher than both distances between solitary foundresses and queens (U = 36,726, P < 0.001) as well as distances between solitary foundresses and workers (U = 111,308, P < 0.001) (Fig. 2b).

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Queens (q), solitary foundresses (sf) and workers (w) of Ropalidia marginata plotted on discriminant functions 1 and 2 (n = 12 q, 8 sf, 36 w). Both functions are significant (P < 0.05). Figures in parenthesis beside each function denote the percent variance explained by that function. b Distance on discriminant functions plot (a) between queens and workers (q–w), solitary foundresses and queens (sf–q), and solitary foundresses and workers (sf–w). Different letters denote significant difference between distributions (Mann–Whitney U test, P < 0.05)

Ovarian index

Kruskal–Wallis test showed that variation in ovarian index between groups was higher than variation within groups (F 2,52 = 68.791, P < 0.0001). Ovarian index of solitary foundresses was intermediate between queens and workers—it was higher than that of workers (Mann–Whitney U = 288, P < 0.001) but lower than that of queens (U = 96, P < 0.001). Ovarian index of workers was lower than that of queens as expected (U = 432, P < 0.001) (Fig. 1c).

Discussion

Dufour’s gland profiles of solitary foundresses were not similar to established queens of post-emergence nests, and neither they were similar to workers, but seemed to occupy a position in between queens and workers. Analysis of pair-wise distance between points on the discriminant functions plot showed that queens and workers were relatively well separated from each other, while solitary foundresses were equidistant from queens and workers. Solitary foundresses were in an intermediate position for percent area under peaks 1, 2, 3 and 4, and for percent area under the remaining peaks also. Although all solitary foundresses had laid eggs, they had lower fecundity than queens. Their state of ovarian activation was intermediate between queens and workers, and this could be a consequence of energy consuming tasks like foraging and nest maintenance that they have to perform. Queens of established post-emergence nests never go out to forage and do almost no work for colony maintenance, and this could be a reason as to why their ovaries are better activated than those of solitary foundresses.

Since solitary foundresses occupy an intermediate position with respect to both Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition and ovary activation, this suggests that their intermediate position may be attributed to their intermediate fecundity level, lending support to the honest signal hypothesis (Keller and Nonacs, 1993). In spite of there being no immediate need for signalling their fertility status, owing to the absence of receivers of the signal, solitary foundresses are expressing a profile that is different from that of workers and closer to that of queens. This is presumably because their state of ovary activation is also higher than workers and closer to queens. The correlation of Dufour’s gland composition with state of ovarian activation might hint at underlying physiological mechanisms. It could be possible that factors involved in the ovarian activation pathway influence the biosynthesis of Dufour’s gland compounds. These results suggest that Dufour’s gland composition in R. marginata may arise as an inevitable byproduct of the ovarian activation status of an individual and hence may function as an honest signal that portrays the fertility of an individual.

It has been proposed that for any signal to originate and be incorporated into a communication system the signal should be reliable or honest (Zahavi, 1975; Maynard Smith and Harper, 1995; Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997; Searcy and Nowicki, 2005). This helps eliminate cheaters and thus makes it possible for the receiver to rely on it. Unless a signal is honest on average, both signaller and receiver will not benefit from it and hence the signal will not evolve (Johnstone and Grafen, 1993; Kokko, 1997). There may be several ways in which a signal can achieve honesty (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1995; Searcy and Nowicki 2005). Since in R. marginata, the queen signal appears to be inextricably linked to the state of fertility, this suggests that queen pheromone in this species is likely to be a signal that can be classified as “index” or “constraint”, i.e. a signal that is forced to be reliable because of physiological constraints on signal production (Maynard Smith and Harper, 1995; Searcy and Nowicki 2005). Since R. marginata workers appear to detect the presence of their queen by means of the queen’s Dufour’s gland compounds that she applies on the nest surface, and react accordingly (Bhadra et al., 2007; Bhadra et al., 2010; Mitra et al., 2011), this has important implications for the mechanism of maintenance of eusociality in this species.

Previous work has shown that Dufour’s gland hydrocarbon composition in R. marginata is correlated with ovarian activation status of queens, suggesting that Dufour’s gland hydrocarbons may act as honest signals of fertility (Mitra and Gadagkar, 2011). This study adds to the evidence for the link between fertility of individuals and chemicals likely to be involved in fertility signalling (Sledge et al., 2001; Monnin, 2006; Izzo et al., 2010; Mitra and Gadagkar, 2011), and reinforces the idea that a signal that arises as a byproduct of fertility is honest and hence can be incorporated in a communication system for fertility signalling.