Introduction

Hymenoptera are a favored group for studies on mating systems because of the presumed effect of female mating frequency on the evolution of sociality (Hamilton, 1964; Boomsma, 2007; Hughes et al., 2008). The majority of female Hymenoptera are thought to mate only once or have an effective mate number very close to one (Boomsma and Ratnieks, 1996; Strassmann, 2001). Well known exceptions are the highly eusocial honey bees (Apini), attine ants and vespine wasps, some of which are highly polyandrous (Foster and Ratnieks, 2001; Villesen et al. 2002; Tarpy et al., 2004). Queens of the European honey bee (Apis mellifera) have an effective mate number of 12 on average (Strassmann, 2001). Honeybees belong to the monophyletic clade of corbiculate bees (Apinae) that also includes the eusocial stingless bees (Meliponini) and bumblebees (Bombini) (Michener, 2000), both of which generally exhibit monandry (Strassmann, 2001), and the strictly neotropical orchid bees (Euglossini). Information on the mating frequency of female orchid bees is lacking. Orchid bees are generally described as solitary (Zucchi et al., 1969), but in some cases two or several females share a nest or nest cavity. For some species there are reports of reproductive dominance of one female (Garofalo, 1985), providing a potential opportunity for the evolution of more complex sociality (Augusto and Garofalo, 2004; Cameron, 2004; Cocom Pech et al., 2008; Otero et al., 2008). However, why eusociality as an obligate condition has not evolved in orchid bees is an open question (Roubik et al., 1996).

Kin selection theory predicts that female mating frequency affects the likelihood that eusociality evolves because it elevates relatedness to 0.75 among female hymenopteran offspring, increasing their inclusive fitness when helping their mother or sisters to reproduce (Queller and Strassmann, 1998; Boomsma, 2007). Female multiple mating, on the other hand, decreases relatedness among a mothers’ daughters (to nearly 0.25), and their theoretical incentive to cooperate. Thus, polyandry may reduce the likelihood of advanced sociality to evolve. Its existence in honey bees, attine ants and vespine wasps is therefore considered to be derived and to postdate the evolution of sociality and permanent worker castes (Boomsma, 2007).

Other evolutionary factors may favor polyandry over monandry. For example, in social species polyandry provides an increase in intracolonial genetic variation (Crozier and Fjerdingstad, 2001) that has been shown to result in a better colony phenotype that, for example, minimizes the adverse effects of parasitism (Baer and Schmid-Hempel, 1999; Tarpy and Seeley, 2006; Seeley and Tarpy, 2007). Furthermore, polyandry in social and non-social insects is thought to provide compensation for difficulties in identifying the single best mate. The latter hypothesis predicts that multiple mating occurs in species where male quality is difficult to assess, thus constraining the evolution of choosiness in females (Strassmann, 2001).

Orchid bees have a highly unusual mating biology, aspects of which are still poorly understood. Males forage for volatile chemicals (fragrances) in a time-consuming and risky manner (Eltz et al., 1999). Flowers of orchids and other plants, as well as decaying wood or fruits, serve as natural sources of such fragrances, which consist mostly of terpenoids and aromatics. For a great number of neotropical orchid species, male orchid bees act as specific pollinators, giving rise to the Euglossine pollination syndrome (Dodson et al., 1969; Williams, 1982; Cameron, 2004). Collected volatiles are stored by euglossine males in voluminous leg pockets (Vogel, 1966), where a species-specific blend of chemicals accumulates (Eltz et al., 2005a; Zimmermann et al., 2006). During courtship display these blends are actively released and ventilated at courtship territories (Bembé, 2004; Eltz et al., 2005b), where females have been observed to mate (Kimsey, 1980; Stern, 1991; Eltz et al., 2003). Although the attraction of females to these odors has yet to be demonstrated in behavioral experiments, the male perfume is likely to function as a species specific chemical signal analogous to endogenous sex pheromones (Vogel, 1966; Zimmermann et al., 2006). In addition, the individual perfume of a male could represent a fitness indicator, giving an approaching female the possibility to evaluate a male’s quality and to choose her best mate, making potentially costly multiple mating unnecessary.

Thus, information on orchid bee female mating frequency is desirable both in the context of social evolution in corbiculate bees as well as for a better understanding of the significance of euglossine fragrance collection. The genus Euglossa represents the largest and most widely distributed genus of orchid bees (more than 100 described species) (Roubik and Hanson, 2004), with females of some species accepting artificial trap nests for their brood rearing. To analyze female mating frequency in orchid bees we genetically analyzed brood from nests of three species of EuglossaE. hemichlora, E. viridissima and E. sp. ‘2dentate’—using polymorphic microsatellites and calculated relatedness among brood, among cohabiting females and between brood and adults.

Materials and methods

Sample collection

We studied Euglossa hemichlora from Panama, Euglossa viridissima from Mexico and another so far undescribed species of Euglossa from Mexico, which is morphologically almost identical to E. viridissima, but males have two instead of three mandibular teeth. Population genetic analysis demonstrated that the lineage is reproductively isolated from E. viridissima (Eltz et al., 2008). For the purpose of this study, we refer to this species as Euglossa sp. ‘2dentate’.

We used wooden boxes (10 × 3 × 6 cm) as trap nests, which were placed around buildings in a private forest preserve in Santa Rita, Colon, Panama (8.39°N, 82.34°W) in October 2007 and at the campus of the Universidad Autónoma de Yucatán in Xmatkuil, Mexico (20.52°N, 87.37°W) in October 2006 and October 2007. The nest boxes were monitored over several months at each locality, and the number and condition of brood cells therein were recorded. Nests were collected when the boxes were populated with at least five (preferentially more) brood cells and at least one adult female was present (which was normally the case during afternoons). For microsatellite DNA analysis, adult females present in the nest boxes were immediately preserved in 99% ethanol and stored at +8°C. The brood cells were kept in a cabinet at 26°C and 70% humidity until the offspring emerged. We recorded the dates of eclosion of progeny, immediately preserved individuals in 99% ethanol and stored them at +8°C.

To obtain more precise estimates of allele frequencies, we also sampled males of all three species up to 3 km radius from the nest boxes. Males were attracted with fragrance baits (p-dimethoxy benzene for E. hemichlora, p-dimethoxy benzene, methyl cinnamate and eugenol for E. viridissima and E. sp. ‘2dentate’), caught and immediately preserved in 99% ethanol at +8°C.

DNA extraction and microsatellite DNA analysis

Microsatellite DNA analysis was conducted by using seven different microsatellite loci: ann02 (GenBank accession no. BV728898), ann04 (BV728900), ann08 (BV728902), ann24 (BV728906) (Paxton et al., 2009), Egc17 (EF451841), Egc18 (EF451842), and Egc37 (EF451846) (Souza et al., 2007). Not all of the samples were typed at all seven loci, but at the most polymorphic loci for the given species, with each individual being typed at an average of 3.36 loci (range 2–6). DNA was extracted from tissue from half of the thorax of each specimen using the method of Hunt and Page (Hunt and Page, 1995). After phenol/chloroform extraction, DNA pellets were dried at 37°C, resuspended in 40 μl of distilled water and stored at −20°C. All PCRs were performed as multiplex reactions with three loci 5′labeled with fluorescent dye (VIC, 6-FAM or NED; Applied Biosystems). Four μl of DNA template was used with 12.5 μl HotStar TaqTM Master Mix (Qiagen) in a final reaction volume of 25 μl, made up with RNase-free water (Qiagen). PCR reactions were performed in a Mastercycler gradient (Eppendorf), with the profile of an initial 95°C for 15 min (HotStar Taq Polymerase), followed by 22 cycles of 94°C for 30 s, 52°C for 90 s, 67°C for 90 s, and then a final extension step of 67°C for 10 min. Fragment analysis of PCR products was carried out with an ABI Prism 310TM Sequencer (PE Applied Biosystems) at the University of Düsseldorf (BMFZ). Allele lengths were assigned with the software GENEMARKER V1.71, using an internal standard run in every lane. Allele sizes were rounded to the nearest integer.

Data analysis

We tested for linkage disequilibrium between loci within each species with the program GENEPOP (Raymond and Rousset, 1995) (web version at http://genepop.curtin.edu.au/), using only the haploid male data because GENEPOP does not allow for a joint analysis of haploid and diploid data.

A standard suite of descriptive statistics was then calculated for each locus using Microsatellite Analyzer (MSA) version 4.05 (Dieringer and Schlotterer, 2003), which supports the option of jointly analyzing haploid and diploid data. To avoid pseudoreplication, only unrelated females were included in the calculation of population allele frequencies, which means one female per nest with one exception where a further unrelated female was present.

For each population of the three species we calculated the genetic non-detection error of the mating frequency, defined as the probability of an undetected second father among a female’s progeny, which occurs when two males have identical genotypes at all investigated loci (Boomsma and Ratnieks, 1996).

Estimation of mating frequency

Assignments of adult females as mothers of brood were first made by visual inspection of genotypes; all daughters of a mother had to carry one single maternal allele at all loci, and all sons had to carry one of the two maternal alleles at each locus. Under monandry, all female offspring of a mother should carry the same paternal allele at a locus. To support pedigree determination based on visual inspection of genotypes, we examined intra-nest relationships using the likelihood function of KINSHIP 1.3.1 (Goodnight and Queller, 1999), (http://www.gsoftnet.us/GSoft.html#Kinanchor), using the same data set as we used for the descriptive statistics. Female genotypes of each nest were analyzed separately in a group. In the first step we analyzed, whether one or more of the adult females present in a nest at the time of sampling were the mother of the brood. Specifically we tested whether her genotype allowed the rejection of the null hypothesis (no relationship to female brood; r = 0) in favor of the alternative hypothesis of a mother–daughter relationship (r = 0.5). In the second step, we tested whether the genotypes of female offspring in a nest allowed us to reject the null hypothesis that they were half sisters (r = 0.25 as expected with multiple mating of the mother) in favor of the alternative hypothesis that they were full sisters (r = 0.75 as expected by single mating). These tests were performed as pairwise comparisons calculated with 1,000 simulations between each female progeny pair within each nest.

With the relatedness function of KINSHIP 1.3.1 we performed pairwise relatedness (R) calculations between all female individuals to generate the mean value of relatedness between a mother and its female offspring and among all female offspring.

Results

In total, we collected 13 populated nest boxes from E. hemichlora (all in the year 2007), five from E. viridissima (two from 2006 and three from 2007) and 14 from Euglossa sp. ‘2dentate’ (eight from 2006 and six from 2007). Two types of nests were distinguished: newly founded nests (NFN) were in artificial boxes that had been colonized for the first time by a single female foundress, building one homogenous cell cluster. We found six NFNs for E. hemichlora, three for E. viridissima and 11 for E. sp. ‘2dentate’. The other nest type, older or re-used nests (RN), contained remains of previous brood (enclosed brood cells) and had obviously been re-used by other females, potentially by progeny of the original foundress. In RNs there was frequently more than one adult female and/or cell cluster, which made the assignment of mothers to progeny more difficult. For E. hemichlora we found four RNs, and for E. viridissima and E. sp. ‘2dentate’ we found two RNs each.

Three additional nests of E. hemichlora and one nest of E. sp. ‘2dentate’ were excluded from our analysis of mating frequency because they had both a low number of female progeny (n < 3) as well as more than one adult female present.

Microsatellite data analysis

No significant linkage disequilibrium between loci was detected in any of the three analysed species (n = 43 for E. hemichlora, n = 69 for E. sp. ‘2dentate’ and n = 73 for E. viridissima).

For the analysis of allelic variation at loci we genotyped males of each population (E. hemichlora n = 43, E. sp. ‘2dentate’ n = 69, E. viridissima n = 73) and pooled these data with the genotype data of unrelated females (see above; E. hemichlora n = 13, E. sp. ‘2dentate’ n = 15, E. viridissima n = 6). Genetic diversity estimates are given in Table 1. The expected heterozygosity (H e) of our markers ranged from 0.41 to 0.98 and we found between five and 49 alleles per locus. In every species H e was 0.8 at a minimum of 2 loci.

Table 1 Allelic diversity for each species, with the number of genotyped male and female individuals per locus

The extremely high variability of ann02 (H e = 0.98) in E. viridissima as well as the large mean allele size of 238 bp (minimum 150 bp and maximum 387 bp) led us to suspect non-specific PCR products. However, repeated independent processing (including DNA extraction) of the same samples confirmed fragment sizes in all cases (n = 14). Also, the marker was clearly inherited in a strictly Mendelian manner within families of bees.

As a result of the high variability of our markers, the population-wide probability of genetic non-detection of a second fathering male’s offspring among progeny genotypes was very small within each of the three species; the non-detection error (d p) varied from 0.002 to 0.00007.

Estimation of mating frequency

In 17 of the 20 analyzed NFNs, the single adult female present was clearly identified as mother of the whole nests female progeny, both by visual inspection of genotypes as well as by KINSHIP (Table 2). One exception was NFN 6 (E. hemichlora), where the adult female was unrelated to the offspring (r = −0.03 ± 0.09), but the female progeny still consisted solely of full-sisters (see below). In the second exception in NFN 24 (E. sp. ‘2dentate’), the single adult female was likely a daughter of the deceased foundress as its genotype was fully congruent with a full-sister relationship with all other female progeny (brood) of the nest. In the third exceptional case (NFN 25 in E. sp. ‘2dentate’) the adult female was identified as the mother of four of the emerging females, whereas the remaining four females (that emerged >21 days earlier) were unrelated to this mother.

Table 2 Summary of the KINSHIP likelihood analyses of mother-daughter and full-sister relationships among Euglossa females in newly founded nests (NFNs)

Genotypes of female progeny were consistent with a full-sister relationship in 19 out of 20 NFNs, and this finding was confirmed by KINSHIP tests (Table 2). The single exception was NFN 25 (E. sp. ‘2dentate’). Here, the first four emerged females formed a group of full-sisters that was likely descended from the adult female present in the nest at the time of sampling. The remaining four females that emerged later were not related to them (r = −0.09 ± 0.12), but were in a full-sister relationship amongst each other. For reasons of clarity the results of KINSHIP tests are given separately for these two groups of offspring (NFN 25a and NFN 25b in Table 2).

The haploid genotypes of male progeny of the NFNs were consistent with being derived from unfertilized eggs laid by the identified nest mothers. Numbers of emerged males per nest are given in Table 2. We did not find a single male offspring that was unrelated to the mother of the diploid brood.

Within the RNs we always found more than one adult female per nest, but only in RN 15 (E. viridissima) was an adult female present which was unrelated to the brood (r = 0.009 ± 0.17) in addition to the clearly identified nest mother. It had no offspring. In RN 20 (E. hemichlora) we found two unrelated pairs of full sisters present (r = 0.08 ± 0.22). In all other cases, relatedness estimates were consistent with adults being either sisters or in a mother-sister relationship (Table 3). The close relatedness between the adult females made an assignment of the offspring more difficult. Nonetheless, full-sister relationships among female offspring, as an indication for single mating, were found in five RNs (RN 1 in E. hemichlora, RN 6 and 26 in E. sp. ‘2dentate’ and RN 15 and 20 in E. viridissima). In these cases one adult female was clearly identified as the mother of the entire female progeny. The other related female(s) had definitely produced no female offspring, but definite exclusion of their contribution to male progeny was impossible based on the available data. One exception was RN 1 (E. hemichlora), where a likely sister of the major reproductive female appeared to be responsible for at least three of the nine male offspring. RN 18 (E. hemichlora) consisted only of female progeny in a full-sister relationship (including four adult females), but the mother was apparently no longer present. The female offspring of RN 4, RN 20 (E. hemichlora), and RN 21 (E. sp. ‘2dentate’) were not explainable with one single mother (even taking into account multiple mating), and a full-sister relationship between the female progeny was not detected. The genetically mixed brood must have been the offspring of more than one reproductive female. In RN 4 and RN 21 we could identify one main reproductive female; these two nests are explainable as derived from multiple, closely related mothers. In RN 20 we were not able to explain the origin of the progeny with the adult females present, which were two pairs of full sisters. This “nest” was located in an old stingless bee nest box (larger than the artificial trap-nests we employed for Euglossa) and contained three cell clusters. Two of them belonged to E. viridissima (RN 20) and one outlying cell cluster to E. sp. ‘2dentate’ (RN 26).

Table 3 Number of adult females and cell clusters in reused artificial trap-nests (RNs)

Altogether, we found strong evidence for single mating in 26 out of 29 nests. Even if multiple mating occurred among females that produced the three problematic nests, the average number of effective mates of the three Euglossa species would be close to one. Within the nine nests that were populated by multiple adult females, we found four cases with progeny that were definitely derived from more than one female. These contributing females were close relatives in three cases, most likely in a full sister relationship (N = 1) or in a mother-daughter relationship (N = 2).

Discussion

Our results represent strong evidence for a predominance of single mating in three Euglossa species and suggest that single mating is the rule in this genus and perhaps in the Euglossini as a whole. Single mating (monandry) within the Euglossini is consistent with the idea that females select a single best mate based on male fragrance phenotypes. However, as single mating is common among bees (Boomsma and Ratnieks, 1996; Roubik, 2006; Soro et al., 2009), and probably ancestral for corbiculate bees (Hughes et al., 2008), monandry in orchid bees is unlikely to be an adaptation resulting from fragrance-based mate selection by females. In any case, single mating is fully consistent with the rarity with which matings are observed at euglossine display sites. Males of most if not all species of orchid bees establish non-resource based display sites for fragrance signaling (Eltz et al., 2005b). These display sites are usually centered around perches (trunks of trees or tree lets, often in the forest understory), where males perform series of hovering flights during which they release their fragrances. Extensive studies of displaying males in the field have resulted in only a handful of observed matings (Kimsey, 1980; Stern, 1991). In most cases the female suddenly appeared and quickly landed on the perch, where it was mounted by the resident male. Such rare events may seem to be lucky strikes in the long lives of males, which otherwise appear to display unsuccessfully for weeks or more. Given single mating in females, the rarity of observed matings is understandable. Orchid bee males devote much of their life to fragrance collection, a behavior that requires specialized morphological features (Bembé, 2004), an intricate metabolic recycling mechanism (Eltz et al., 2007), and certainly a lot of energy and risk-taking in order to create their perfume. Although the composition of the male’s fragrance mixture is broadly species specific, there is substantial individual variation in quantity and complexity of the blends (Eltz et al., 1999; Zimmermann et al., 2006). It is, therefore, conceivable that females evaluate male fragrance phenotypes to obtain information on male suitability as a mate. If so, females may not need to mate more than once to obtain good genes for their offspring.

Our results fuel the discussion on how bee mating systems evolved (Paxton, 2005) and how they might affect the evolution of advanced sociality. The Euglossini were the last group of corbiculate bees for which information on female mating frequency was lacking. Our study confirms the prevalence of single mating among corbiculate bees: with the exception of honeybees, all other corbiculate clades (bumblebees, stingless bees, and orchid bees) have been demonstrated to be predominantly monandrous. Although the exact phylogenetic relationship among corbiculate bees is still controversial (Ascher et al., 2001; Kawakita et al., 2008), monandry in orchid bees confirms the view that single mating was the ancestral state in this clade. Correspondingly, polyandry in honeybees is likely derived (Hughes et al., 2008).

Kin selection theory predicts that single mating promotes the evolution of eusociality, because it increases the genetic relatedness among offspring and, accordingly, their incentive to cooperate in the care of brood (Trivers and Hare, 1975; Cole, 1983; Boomsma, 2007). This is suggested because non-reproductive individuals can gain greater inclusive fitness by functioning as helpers of close relatives (Hamilton, 1964; Queller and Strassmann, 1998). As orchid bees seem to be mostly singly mated, the mating system cannot account for the conspicuous lack of advanced sociality in this group. Female offspring were full sisters in most of the analyzed nests of this study, and adult females present in a given nest were almost always close relatives. This would seem to represent the ideal genetic background for reproductive division of labor and eusocial behavior to evolve. Indeed, some early stages of sociality and reproductive division of labor exist in orchid bees. Previous studies (Dressler, 1982; Santos and Garofalo, 1994) as well as our own observations here have provided evidence for the frequent occurrence of multi-female nests, which seem to result predominantly from nest re-use by bees of the next generation (Garofalo, 1985; Soucy et al., 2003; Augusto and Garofalo, 2004). In some species of Euglossa there is evidence for individual females gaining reproductive dominance over their nest mates that allocate more time to foraging (Michener, 1974; Garofalo, 1985; Cocom Pech et al., 2008; Otero et al., 2008). There is also a suggestion of reproductive conflict, e.g. evidenced by the frequent occurrence of oophagy, usually by the reproductively dominant female (Garofalo, 1985; Roubik and Hanson, 2004; Cocom Pech et al., 2008; Otero et al., 2008). In natural nest cavities, nest sharing may even be more common than in artificial trap-nests, because natural cavities are potentially larger and alternative nest sites perhaps more difficult to find. The benefits of nest sharing may include the avoidance of mortality risks associated with searching for new nest cavities and nest construction material. Furthermore, multi-female nests may be better protected against nest parasites, which regularly enter the nest while the female is out foraging (Soucy et al., 2003; Cocom Pech et al., 2008). However, while the presence of host females in the nest has been observed to deter attacking cleptoparasitic bees, such effects may not necessarily translate into better protection of multiple-female nests (Garofalo and Rozen, 2000).

Ecological factors such as limited nest sites and economy of nest material have already been hypothesized to favor nest sharing in studies about communal bees (Paxton et al., 1996), where females tolerate unrelated conspecifics as well. Nest reuse by succeeding generations would avoid the search for a suitable nesting place and allow the recycling of nest material from hatched brood cells (Dressler, 1982). Thus, given the right ecological conditions, females might benefit from cooperation with conspecifics or even heterospecifics. However, there is no evidence for highly eusocial behavior in orchid bees, or for the formation of long-lived colonies with highly skewed reproduction among individual females. This evolutionary avenue may be barred by several constraining factors: First, euglossine bees have relatively long generation times, with development from egg to adult taking at least 6 weeks and often longer (Roubik and Hanson, 2004). This probably reduces the amount of time that mother and offspring overlap in their adult lives and reduces the opportunities for daughters to help their mothers (but see (Cocom Pech et al., 2008). Second, female reproduction may be limited by physiological factors, even when help from related individuals is available. Possible physiological constraints include the number of sperm that can be stored in the female spermatheca (euglossine spermatozoa are exceptionally large, see (Zama et al., 2005)) or, more likely, the availability of protein for egg production.

In our study, we found little evidence for communal nesting of unrelated females. There were only three cases where an unrelated conspecific female was present in a nest box, and in two cases these had no own progeny. It is conceivable that the unrelated females were in search of nest cavities or even for nest building material, since for example E. viridissima females are known to glean resin from populated nests of stingless bees (J. Quezada-Euan, unpubl. data). Detailed behavioral observations of individually marked females coupled to genetic analysis of offspring will help resolve the uncertainty over the degree to which Euglossines exhibit social behavior.