Skip to main content
Log in

How Is Physiology Relevant to Behavior Analysis?

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Physiology is an important biological science; but behavior analysis is not a biological science, and behavior analysts can safely ignore biological processes. However, ignoring products of biological processes might be a serious mistake. The important products include behavior, instinctive drift, behavior potentials, hunger, and many developmental milestones and events. Physiology deals with the sources of such products; behavior analysis can deal with how the products affect behavior, which can be understood without understanding their sources.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Begleiter, H., Porjesz, B., Yerre, C., & Kissin, B. (1973). Evoked potential correlates of expected stimulus intensity. Science, 179, 814–816.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W. (1979). Some clarifications on the meaning of a behavior analysis of child development. Psychological Record, 29, 3–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bijou, S. W., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Behavior analysis of child development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, L. R. (1967). The suppression of visualization by reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 19, 289–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. L. (1975). The development of memory: Knowing, knowing about knowing, and knowing how to know. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 10, pp. 103–152). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugelski, B. R. (1973). An introduction to the principles of psychology (2nd ed.). Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bugelski, B. R. (1982). Learning and imagery. Journal of Mental Imagery, 6(2), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J. T., Crites, S. L., Jr., Berntson, G. G., & Coles, M. G. H. (1993). If attitudes affect how stimuli are processed, should they not affect the event-related brain potential? Psychological Science, 4, 108–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Das, J. P., Kirby, J., & Jarman, R. F. (1975). Simultaneous and successive syntheses: An alternative model for cognitive abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 87–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1969). On certain similarities between the Philosophical Investigations of Ludwig Wittgenstein and the operationism of B. F. Skinner. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 489–506.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. (1976). Contemporary behaviorism and the concept of intention. In W. J. Arnold (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1975 (pp. 65–131). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1977). On Skinner’s treatment of the first-person, third-person psychological sentence distinction. Behaviorism, 5(1), 33–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. (1983). On the difference between radical and methodological behaviorism. Behaviorism, 11, 89–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, W. F. (1992). The concept of reinforcement-history and explanation in behaviorism. In S. Leigland (Ed.), Radical behaviorism: Willard Day on psychology and philosophy (pp. 117–122). Reno, NV: Context Press. (Paper originally presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Washington D.C., September 1976)

    Google Scholar 

  • Delprato, D. J. (1979). The interbehavioral alternative to brain-dogma. Psychological Record, 29, 409–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delprato, D. J. (1986). Response patterns. In H. W. Reese & L. J. Parrott (Eds.), Behavior analysis: Philosophical, methodological, and empirical advances (pp. 61–113). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1933). How we think: A restatement of the relation of reflective thinking to the educative process. Boston: Heath.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan-Johnson, C. C., & Kopell, B. S. (1981). The Stroop effect: Brain potentials localize the source of interference. Science, 214, 938–940.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ferster, D., & Spruston, N. (1995). Cracking the neuronal code. Science, 270, 756–757.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gevins, A. S., Morgan, N. H., Bressler, S. L., Cutillo, B. A., Whie, R. M., Illes, J., Greer, D. S., Doyle, J. C., & Zeitlin, G. M. (1987). Human neuroelectric patterns predict performance accuracy. Science, 235, 580–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Hayes, L. J., & Reese, H. W. (1988). Finding the philosophical core: A review of Stephen C. Pepper’s World Hypotheses: A Study in Evidence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 97–111.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Psychology in America: A historical survey. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. J. (1971). Second discussant’s comments: What’s left to say? In J. H. Flavell (Chair), What is memory development the development of? Human Development, 14, 279–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1922). The nervous system, psychological fact or fiction? Journal of Philosophy, 19, 38–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1923). The organismic vs. the mentalistic attitude toward the nervous system. Psychological Bulletin, 20, 684–692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1947). Problems of physiological psychology. Granville, OH: Principia Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1969). The scientific evolution of psychology (Vol. 2). Chicago: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1970). An analysis of the experimental analysis of behavior (TEAB). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 101–108.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kantor, J. R. (1977). Psychological linguistics. Chicago: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klahr, D., & Wallace, J. G. (1976). Cognitive development: An information-processing view. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1988). Aspects of a cognitive neuroscience of mental imagery. Science, 240, 1621–1626.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuo, Z.-Y. (1976). The dynamics of behavior development: An epigenetic view (enlarged ed. prepared by G. Gottlieb). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamal, P. A. (1983). A cogent critique of epistemology leaves radical behaviorism unscathed. Behaviorism, 11, 103–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loeb, J. (1964). The mechanistic conception of life (D. Fleming, Ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1912)

  • Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed., revised and expanded; B. Haigh, Trans.). New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • MacCorquodale, K., & Meehl, P. E. (1948). On a distinction between hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. Psychological Review, 55, 95–107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mapel, B. M. (1977). Philosophical criticism of behaviorism: An analysis. Behaviorism, 5(1), 17–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maunsell, J. H. R. (1995). The brain’s visual world: Representation of visual targets in cerebral cortex. Science, 270, 764–769.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCandless, B. R., & Evans, E. D. (1973). Children and youth: Psychosocial development. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (1981). On mentalism, methodological behaviorism, and radical behaviorism. Behaviorism, 9, 55–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, E. K., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (1982a). Comments on cognitive science in the experimental analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 109–125.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, E. K., Higgins, S. T., & Bickel, W. K. (1982b). The influence of Kantor’s interbehavioral psychology on behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 5, 159–173.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nesselroade, J. R., & McArdle, J. J. (in press). On the mismatching of levels of abstraction in mathematical-statistical model fitting. In H. W. Reese & M. D. Franzen (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Biological and neuropsychological mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

  • Palmer, D. C., & Donahoe, J. W. (1992). Essentialism and selectionism in cognitive science and behavior analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1344–1358.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parrott, L. J. (1983). Perspectives on knowing and knowledge. Psychological Record, 33, 171–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petri, H. L., & Mishkin, M. (1994). Behaviorism, cognitivism and the neuropsychology of memory. American Scientist, 82, 30–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1993). Seeing the mind. Science, 262, 673–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Petersen, S. E., Fox, P. T., & Raichle, M. E. (1988). Localization of cognitive operations in the human brain. Science, 240, 1627–1631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1982). On learning and neurologizing. Journal of Mental Imagery, 6(2), 55–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W. (1993). Comments about Morris’s paper. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 67–74.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, H. W., & Smyer, M. A. (1983). The dimensionalization of life events. In E. C. Callahan & K. A. McCluskey (Eds.), Life-span developmental psychology: Nonnormative life events (pp. 1–33). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rosales-Ruiz, J., & Baer, D. (in press). A behavior-analytic view of development. In E. Ribes & S. W. Bijou (Eds.), Recent approaches to behavior development. Reno, NV: Context Press.

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson’s University Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, O. (1995). Musical ability. Science, 268, 621. (Letter)

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schiaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1995a). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267, 699–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., & Steinmetz, H. (1995b). Response. Science, 268, 621–622. (Letter)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1972). On the development of the processor. In S. Farnham-Diggory (Ed.), Information processing in children (pp. 3–22). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, W. (1995). Development and plasticity of cortical processing architectures. Science, 270, 758–764.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1945). The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review, 52, 270–277, 291–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1978). Reflections on behaviorism and society. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1980). Notebooks (R. Epstein, Ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1984). The shame of American education. American Psychologist, 39, 947–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stemmer, N. (1987). The hypothesis of other minds: Is it the best explanation? Philosophical Studies, 51, 109–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, G. R. (1991). Behavior analysis for lasting change. Fort Worth, TX: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teyler, T. J. (1975). A primer of psychobiology: Brain and behavior. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, R. F. (1986). The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science, 233, 941–947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional brain imaging studies of cortical mechanisms for memory. Science, 270, 769–775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from the standpoint of a behaviorist. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weizenbaum, J. (1976). Computer power and human reason: From judgment to calculation. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, W. S. (1978). In defense of descriptive behaviorism, or theories of learning still aren’t necessary. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 82–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuriff, G. E. (1980). Radical behaviorist epistemology. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 337–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hayne W. Reese.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Reese, H.W. How Is Physiology Relevant to Behavior Analysis?. BEHAV ANALYST 19, 61–70 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392739

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392739

Key words

Navigation