Skip to main content
Log in

The Effect of an Irrelevant Premise on Temporal and Spatial Reasoning

Zum Einfluß irrelevanter Prämissen auf das zeitliche und räumliche Schließen

  • Published:
Kognitionswissenschaft

Zusammenfassung

Gemäß der Theorie mentaler Modelle ziehen Individuen Schlußfolgerungen, indem sie Modelle der Situation konstruieren, die in den Prämissen beschrieben wird. Eine Aufgabe ist umso schwieriger, je mehr Modelle dabei konstruiert werden müssen. Byrne und Johnson-Laird (1989) bestätigten diese Vorhersage für räumliche Probleme, und Schaeken, Johnson-Laird und d’Ydewalle (1996 a, 1996 b) für zeitliche. Allerdings existiert in all diesen Arbeiten das folgende Problem: Alle Mehr-Modell-Aufgaben und einige der Ein-Modell-Aufgaben enthielten eine irrelevante Prämisse. Wie Rips (1994) anmerkte, ist es möglich, daß eine irrelevante Prämisse die Suche nach einer Ableitung behindert. In dieser Arbeit wird über ein Experiment berichtet, in dem explizit die Präsentation einer solchen irrelevanten Prämisse sowohl in Ein-Modellals auch in Mehr-Modell-Fällen variiert wurde. Die Ergebnisse stützen die Vorhersagen der Theorie mentaler Modelle.

Abstract

The mental model theory of reasoning postulates that individuals reason by constructing models of the situation described by premises. The more models reasoners have to build, the harder a problem will be. Byrne and Johnson-Laird (1989) confirmed this prediction with spatial problems and Schaeken et al. (1996 a, b) with temporal ones. There is, however, a problem with these studies. All the multiple-model problems and some of the one-model problems contained an irrelevant premise. As Rips (1994) argues, it is possible that an irrelevant premise would complicate the search for a derivation. The present paper reports an experiment which explicitly manipulated the presence of such an irrelevant premise in both one-model and multiple-model problems. The results corroborate the predictions of the mental model theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Explore related subjects

Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.

References

  • Braine, M.D. S., Reiser, B. J. & Rumain, B. (1984). Some empirical justification for a theory of natural propositional logic. The psychology of learning and motivation (vol. 18, pp. 313–371). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, R.M.J. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1989). Spatial reasoning. Journal of Memory and Language 28, 564–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, C. & Santamaria, C. (1997). Reasoning about relations: spatial and nonspatial problems. Thinking and Reasoning 3, 191–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hagert, G. (1985). Modeling mental models: experiments in cognitive modeling of spatial reasoning. In: T. O’Shea (ed.), Advances’ in artificial intelligence (pp. 179–188). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1983). Mental models: towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P.N. & Byrne, R.M.J. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macnamara, J. (1986). A border dispute: the place of logic in psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D. (1975). Logic and models of logical thinking. In: R.J. Falmagne (ed.), Reasoning: representation and process in children and adults (pp. 81–91). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollock, J. (1989). How to build a person: a prolegomenon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. J. (1983). Cognitive processes in propositional reasoning. Psychological Review 90, 38–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rips, L. J. (1994). The psychology of proof. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeken, W. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1995). How do subjects reason about temporal relations. In: J.D. Moore & J.F. Lehman (eds.), Proceedings of the Seventeenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 725–730). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaeken, W., Johnson-Laird, P.N. & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996a). Mental models and temporal reasoning. Cognition 60, 205–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeken, W., Johnson-Laird P.N. & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996b). Tense, aspect, and temporal reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning 2, 309–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vandierendonck, A. & De Vooght, G. (1996). A comparison of reasoning with time and space concepts. Thinking and Reasoning 2, 249–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schaeken, W., Girotto, V. & Johnson-Laird, P.N. The Effect of an Irrelevant Premise on Temporal and Spatial Reasoning. Kognit. Wiss. 7, 27–32 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03354960

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03354960

Keywords

Navigation