Skip to main content

Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adult Personality: Evaluating the Evidence

  • Chapter
Foundations of Personality

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 72))

Abstract

This paper is made up of two parts. In the first part we outline a frame of reference (the Big Five) for organizing the data from Behavior Genetic studies of adult personality and then briefly review methods (model fitting) used by behavior geneticists to analyze genetic and environmental influences on personality. We then proceed to summarize the major model fitting findings from twin, adoption, extended family, and twin reared apart studies. Part two critiques non-behavioral genetic approaches to the study of environmental influences on personality. With regard to part one we conclude that a) genetic factors account for about 40-50% of the variance in the Big Five personality traits as measured by standard instruments, and b) that non-additive variance contributes significantly to most of these traits (in the range.02 to.17). The hypothesis of a more modest heritability based on only additive variance (28-46%) and special monozygotic and sibling environments cannot, however, be completely ruled out. There are inconsistencies in the findings and failures to replicate the details of studies. We argue that this is due to the use of poor quality instruments, and that unlike scientists in other domains who push the limits of their instrumentation to test theoretical ideas, behavior geneticists have failed to implement the broad array advances in our understanding of the measurement of psychological traits that have occurred over the last forty years. With regard to part two we find that a) critiques of the twin method are largely unfounded, b) the methodologies and assumptions of the various environmental (non-behavior genetic) approaches to explaining variance in adult personality traits are largely untenable, c) the results of experimental studies have often been inappropriately extrapolated to domains to which they do not apply, and d) studies of a number of “so called” environmental variables (birth order, divorce, physical attractiveness, etc.) simply do not explain the phenomena they have been postulated to explain. We conclude that the non-behavior genetic approach to the study of environmental influences on personality has generated few if any credible findings. We further argue that studies of environmental influences on adult personality that do not impose genetic controls are uninterpretable with regard to explaining individual differences in adult personality.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bergeman, C.S., Chipuer, H.M., Plomin R., Pedersen, N.L., McClearn, G.E., Nesselroade, J.R., Costa, P.T., Jr. and McCrae, R.R. (in press). Genetic and environmental effects on openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness: An adoption/twin study. Journal of Personality.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., (1982a). Review of Identical twins reared apart: A reanalysis. Contemporary Psychology, 27, 190–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., (1982b). Review of The intelligence controversy. American Journal of Psychology, 95, 346–34

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., (1983). Do environmental similarities explain the similarity in intelligence of identical twins reared apart? Intelligence, 7, 175–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., (1987). The hereditarian research program: Triumphs and tribulations. In S. Modgil and C. Modgil (Eds.), Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy (pp. 55–75). London: Falmer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., (in press). The genetic architecture of human intelligence. In P.A. Vernon (Ed.), Biological approaches to the study of human intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Lykken, D.T., Tellegen, A. and McGue, M. (in press). Genes, drives, environment and experience: EPD theory revised. In C. Benbow and D. Lubinski (Eds.). From psychometrics to giftedness: Essays in honor of Julian C. Stanley. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Lykken, D.T., McGue M., Segal, N.L. and Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart. Science, 250, 223–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Lykken, D.T., McGue M., Segal, N.L. and Tellegen, A. (1991). IQ and heredity. Science, 252, 191–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr. and McGue, M. (1990). Genetic and rearing environmental influences on adult personality: An analysis of adopted twins reared apart. Journal of Personality, 58, 263–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr. and Segal, N. (1985). Environment and IQ. In B.J. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence: Theories, measurements, and applications. (pp. 391–464). New York: Wiley, 391-464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Segal, N.L. and Lykken, D.T. (1990). Genetic and environmental influences on special mental abilities in a sample of twins reared apart. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 39, 193–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–744.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burks, B.S. (1938). On the relative contributions of nature and nurture to average group differences in intelligence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 24, 276–282

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charlesworth, W.R. (1992). Darwin and developmental psychology: Past and present. Developmental Psychology, 28, 5–1

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherlin, A.J., Furstenberg, F.F. Jr., Chase-Lansdale, P.L., Kiernan, K.E., Robins, P.K., Morrison, D.R. and Teitler, J.O. (1991). Longitudinal studies of effects of divorce on children in Great Britain and the United States. Science, 252, 1386–1389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, P.T. Jr. and McCrae, R.R. (1992). Four ways five factors are basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 653–665.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L.J., Gleser, G.C., Nanda, H. and Rajaratnam, N. (1972). The dependability of behavioral measurements: Theory of generalizability of scores and profiles. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Digman, J.M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. In M.R. Rosenzweig and L.W. Porter (Eds.), Annual Review of Psychology. (Vol 41, pp. 417–440). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L.J., Eysenck, H.J. and Martin, N.G. (1989). Genes, culture and personality. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G.H. Jr. (1974). Children of the great depression. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elder, G.H. Jr., Caspi, A. and Nyguyen, T. (1986). Resourceful and vulnerable children: Family influence in hard times. In edR.K. Silbereisen, K, Eyferth and G. Rudinger(Eds.). Development as action in context. (pp. 167–186). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, C. and Angst, J. (1983). Birth order: Its influence on personality. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H.J. (1992). Four ways five factors are not basic. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 667–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farber, S.L. (1981). Identical twins reared apart: A reanalysis. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feingold, A. (1992). Good-looking people are not what we think. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 304–341.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Floderus-Myrhed B., Pedersen, N. and Rasmuson, I. (1980). Assessment of heritability for personality, based on a short-form of the Eysenck Personality Inventory. Behavior Genetics, 10, 153–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L.R. (1992). The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L.R. (in press). What the hell took so long? Donald Fiske and the Big-Five factor structure. In P.E. Shrout and S.T. Fiske (Eds.), Advances in personality researc? methods, and theory; A festschrift honoring Donald W. Fiske. New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H.G. (1968). An interpreter’s syllabus for the California Psychological Inventory. In P. McReynolds (Ed.), Advances in psychological assessment (Vol 1). Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H.G. (1987). California Psychological Inventory administrators guide. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, H. (1992). Personal communication to Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hartup, W.W. (1983). Peer relationships. In P.H. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality and social development (4th ed., pp. 102–196). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hase, H.D. and Goldberg, L. (1967). Comparative validity of different strategies of constructing personality inventory scales. Psychological Bulletin, 67, 231–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hewitt, J.K. (1984). Normal components of personality variation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 671–67

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, L.W. (1991). The influence of the family environment on personality: Accounting for sibling differences. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 187–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horn, J.M., Plomin, R. and Rosenman, R. (1976). Heritability of personality traits in adult male twins. Behavior Genetics, 6, 17–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hough, L.M. (1992). The “Big Five” personality variables-construct confusion: Description versus prediction. Human Performance, 5, 139–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, J.E. and Schmidt, F.L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: Correcting error and bias in research findings. Newbury Park: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jinks, J.L. and Fulker, D.W. (1970). Comparison of the biometrical genetical, MAVA, and classical approaches to the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 73, 311–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John, O.P. (1990). The “Big-Five” factor taxonomy: Dimensions of personality in the natural language and in questionnaires. In L.A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 66–100). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, L.M., Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Arvey, R.D., Segal, N.L. and Dawis, R.V. (1992). Work values: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 79–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamb, M.E. and Bornstein, M.H. (1986). Development in infancy (2nd ed.). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langinvainio H., Kaprio J., Koskenvuo, M. and Lonnqvist, J. (1984). Finnish twins reared apart. III: Personality factors. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 33, 259–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langlois, J.H. (1986). From the eye of the beholder to behavioral reality: Development of social behaviors and social relations as a function of physical attractiveness. In C.P. Herman, M.P. Zanna and E.T. Higgins (Eds.), Ontario symposium on personality and social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 23–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasko, J.K. (1954). Parent behavior toward first and second children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 49, 97–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lerner, R.M. (1987). A life-span perspective for early adolescence. In R.M. Lerner and T.T. Foch (Eds.), Biological-psychosocial interactions in early adolescence (pp. 9–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lernerz K., Kucher, J.S., East, P.L., Lerner, J.V. and Lerner, R.M. (1987). Early adolescents’ physical organismic characteristics and psychosocial functioning: Findings from the Pennsylvania early adolescent transition study (PEATS). In R.M. Lerner and T.T. Foch (Eds.), Biological-psychosocial interactions in early adolescence (pp. 225–248). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J.C. (1989). Partitioning environmental and genetic contributions to behavioral development. American Psychologist, 44, 1285–1292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J.C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loehlin, J.C. and Nichols, R.C. (1976). Heredity, environment, and personality. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lounsbury, M.L. and Bates, J.E. (1982). The cries of infants of differing levels of perceived temperamental difficulties: Acoustic properties and effects on listeners. Child Development, 53, 677–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski, D. and Dawis, R.V. (1992). Aptitudes, skills, and proficiencies. In M. Dunnette and L.M. Hough (Eds.). Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology (Vol 2). Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lubinski D., Tellegen, A. and Butcher, J.N. (1983). Masculinity, femininity, and androgyny: Viewed and assessed as distinct concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 428–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lubs, H.A. (1969). A marker X chromosome. American Journal of Human Genetics, 21, 231–244.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lykken, D.T., Bouchard, T.J. Jr., McGue, M. and Tellegen, A. (in press). How might genes determine interests? A twin study. Journal of Applied Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malatesta, C.Z., Culver, C, Tesman, J.R. and Shepard, B. (1989). The development of emotion expression during the first two years of life. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 54, (1-2, Serial No. 219).

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, N. and Jardine, R. (1986). Eysencks contribution to behaviour genetics. In S. Modgil and C. Modgil (Eds.), Hans Eysenck: Consensus and controversy. London: Falmer International.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T. Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGue, M. and Lykken, D.T. (in press). Genetic influence on risk of divorce. Psychological Science

    Google Scholar 

  • Meehl. P.E. (1978). Theoretical risks and tabular asterisks: Sir Karl, Sir Ronald, and the slow progress of soft psychology. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 806–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meehl, P.E. (1990). Why summaries of research on psychological theories are often uninterpretable. Psychological Reports, 66, 195–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1973). Towards a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moloney, D.P., Bouchard, T.J. Jr. and Segal, N.L. (1991). A genetic and environmental analysis of the vocational interests of monozygotic and dizygotic twins reared apart. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 39, 76–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myers, I.B. and McCaulley, M.H. (1985). Manual: A guide to the development and use of the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M.C., Cardon, L.R. and Associates (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, R.C. (1978). Twin studies of ability, personality and interests. Homo, 29, 158–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, N.L., McClearn, G.E., Plomin, R. and Friberg, L. (1985). Separated fraternal twins: Resemblance for cognitive abilities. Behavior Genetics, 15, 407–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, N.L., Plomin R., Nesselroade, J.R. and McClearn, G.E. (in press). A quantitative genetic analysis of cognitive abilities during the second half of the life span. Psychological Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedersen, N.L., Plomin R., McClearn, G.E. and Friberg, L. (1988). Neuroticism, extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 950–957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R. and Bergeman, C.S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic influences on “environmental” measures. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 14, 373–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R. and DeFries, J.C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado adoption project. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R. DeFries, J.C. and Fulker, D.W. (1988). Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R., DeFries, J.C. and Loehlin, J.C. (1977). Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 309–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin R., DeFries, J.C. and McClearn, G.E. (1990). Behavioral genetics: A primer (2nd ed). New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rennie, J. (1992). Mutable mutations. Scientific American, May, 34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Retherford, R.D. and Sewell, W.H. (1991). Birth order and intelligence: Further tests of the confluence model. American Sociological Review, 56, 141–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rose R., Kaprio, J. Williams, C.J., Viken, R. and Obremski, K. (1990). Social contact and sibling similarity: Facts, issues, and red herrings. Behavior Genetics, 20, 763–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S. (1985). Constructing psychology: Making facts and fables for our times. American Psychologist, 40, 499–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarr S., Webber, P.L., Weinberg, R.A. and Wittig, M.A. (1981). Personality resemblance among adolescents and their parents in biologically-related and adoptive families. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 885–898.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schofield, W. (1964). Psychotherapy: The purchase of friendship. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schooler, C. (1972). Birth order effects: Not here, not now! Psychological Bulletin, 78, 161–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorell, G. T. and Nowak, C.A. (1981). The role of physical attractiveness as a contributor to individual development. In R.M. Lerner and N.A. Bush-Rossnagle (Eds.), Individuals as producers of their development (pp. 389–446). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stagner, R. and Katzoff, E.T. (1936). Personality as related to birth order and family size. Journal of Applied Psychology, 20, 340–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H.F. (1980). The IQ game: A methodological inquiry into the heredity environment controversy. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tellegen, A. and Waller, N.G. (in press). Exploring personality through test construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire. In S.R. Briggs and J.M. Cheek (Eds.), Personality measures: Development and evaluation (Vol 1). Greenwich, CN: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomlinson-Keasey, C. and Little, T.D. (1990). Predicting educational attainment, occupational achievement, intellectual skill and personal adjustment among gifted men and women. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 442–455.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tupes, E.C. and Christal, R.E. (1961). Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings. Technical report ASD-TR-61-97. Lackland Air Force Base, TX: USA. Air Force.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turkheimer E., Lovett G., Robinette, C.D. and Gottesman, I.I. (1992). The heritability of divorce: New data and theoretical implications. Paper delivered to the Behavior Genetics Association Annual Meetings, Boulder Colorado, July 4, 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waller, N.G., Kojetin, B.A., Bouchard, T.J. Jr., Lykken, D.T. and Tellegen, A. (1990). Genetic and environmental influences on religious interests, attitudes, and values: A study of twins reared apart and together. Psychological Science, 1, 138–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waller, N.G., Lykken, D.T. and Tellegen, A. (in press). Occupational interests, leisure time interests, and personality: Three domains or one? Findings from the Minnesota Twin Registry. In R. Dawis, D. Lubinski (Eds.), Assessing individual differences in human behavior: New concepts and findings. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiesenfeld, A.R. and Malatesta, C.Z. (1982). Infant distress. In L.W. Hoffman, R.J. Gandelman and H.R. Shiffman (Eds.), Parenting, its causes and consequences (pp. 123–139). Hillsdale, N. J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiggins, J.S. (1966). Substantive dimensions of self-report in the MMPI item pool. Psychological Monographs, 80, (Whole No. 630).

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R.A. (1983). The Louisville twin study: Developmental synchronies in behavior. Child Development, 54, 298–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R.B. (1983). Validating the confluence model. Psychological Bulletin, 93, 457–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. (1992). What is a basic factor and which factors are basic? Turtles all the way down. Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 675–68

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1993 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bouchard, T.J. (1993). Genetic and Environmental Influences on Adult Personality: Evaluating the Evidence. In: Hettema, J., Deary, I.J. (eds) Foundations of Personality. NATO ASI Series, vol 72. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-1660-2_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-1660-2_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-4725-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-1660-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics