Keywords

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

1 Introduction

Social behavior in groups of marmosets and tamarins (the Callitrichidae) is characterized by the dominance of a single reproductive female or breeding pair (Abbott et al. 1993; French 1997), even when polygynous breeding is the norm (e.g., Dietz and Baker 1993; Digby 1995a; Coutinho and Corrêa 1996). The cooperative breeding system mediates tolerance of mature, non-reproductive helpers by breeding animals, but it also creates the potential for increased competition for both resources and breeding positions. However, intra-group agonism is normally rare in free-ranging callitrichids (Goldizen and Terborgh, 1989; Digby 1995a), and in captivity, it generally occurs between members of the same sex (e.g., Callithrix jacchus: Epple 1975; Rothe 1978; Evans 1983; Abbott 1984; Stevenson and Rylands 1988).

As only one or two helpers may make a significant contribution to a breeding female’s reproductive success (Goldizen 1987; Sánchez et al. 1999), it would seem reasonable to expect declining tolerance in groups with more than four adult members (Ferrari and Digby 1996). Many free-ranging marmoset (Callithrix) groups contain five or more adults, however, which suggests that such tolerance may depend on additional factors. The marmosets’ unique set of specializations for the dietary exploitation of plant exudates, which provides a stable resource base, and the relatedness of group members and the risks of dispersal is likely to help explain tolerance within large groups (Ferrari and Lopes 1989), as are the relatedness of group members and the risks of dispersal (Goldizen and Terborgh 1989; Ferrari and Digby 1996).

In the present study, agonistic behavior in a free-ranging group of buffy-headed marmosets, Callithrix flaviceps, is analyzed during a period when the group reached its maximum size (15 members) and four adult members emigrated. The results of the study point to a three-tiered, gender-based social hierarchy, but no evidence of any increase in agonism was found with decreasing resource availability, or in relation to the emigration of group members. Dispersal thus appeared to be triggered by external (availability of potential mates) rather than internal factors.

2 Methods

2.1 Study Site and Animals

The C. flaviceps study group was monitored at the Fazenda Montes Claros (19°50′S, 41°50′W) in the municipality of Caratinga, in Minas Gerais, Brazil, between 1985 and 1991 (see Ferrari 1988; Ferrari and Diego 1992). The observational records analyzed here cover the 7-month period between January and July, 1986, during which a pair of twins were born and four adult group members emigrated. During this period, group size varied between a maximum of 15 members (following the birth) and a minimum of 11, following emigrations (Table 8.1). The study subjects were fully habituated to the presence of human observers (observer-subject distances were often less than 1 m) and individually identifiable through differences in pelage markings.

Table 8.1 Composition of the Callithrix flaviceps study group during the study period

2.2 Behavioral Data

Quantitative behavioral records were collected using instantaneous scan sampling (Altmann 1974), with a scan of 1-min duration carried out at 5-min intervals throughout the daily activity period of the study group (sleep-tree to sleep-tree) during 10 days each month. The behavior of each group member located during the scan was recorded, allowing the compilation of activity budgets (Ferrari 1988, 1992). Behavior sampling (Martin and Bateson 1993) was also used for the collection of records of social behavior. In all cases, the identity of the animals involved and the sequence of events were recorded whenever possible. For the purposes of the present study, only records of agonistic (dominant) and submissive (subordinate) behavior categories (Table 8.2) were analyzed. The dominant member of a dyad was defined according to the ratio of agonistic to submissive behavior. The characteristics of the data proscribed the use of a more systematic, quantitative measure, such as Nishida’s (1988) index of reciprocity. Resource availability was monitored throughout the main study period (for details, see Ferrari 1988).

Table 8.2 Categories of social behavior used in the present study, adapted from Stevenson and Poole (1976), Ferrari (1988), and Digby (1995a)

As the composition of the group changed during the study period, comparisons between sexes and across months are based on the rate of a given behavior category per individual per standard observation month (10 days) rather than absolute values. Group composition only changed mid-month in June, at the end of the fifth day of observation. The data for this month are thus presented separately, with June 1 referring to the period preceding the emigration, and June 2 to that following the change in group composition. The distribution of events among individuals was tested using χ 2.

Standardized rates were calculated for each sex (Tables 8.3 and 8.4), and for the group as a whole (Fig. 8.1). For the χ 2 analyses, the individual values (number of events) were used, and compared to a homogeneous distribution.

Fig. 8.1
figure 1_8

Standardised monthly rates of agonistic (shaded) and submissive (unshaded) behaviour recorded in the Callithrix flaviceps study group. Emigration 1 Simão left group; Emigration 2 Cuba, Dida and Spock left group

Table 8.3 Records of agonistic behaviour categories among Callithrix flaviceps adults by sex and month
Table 8.4 Records of submissive behavior categories among Callithrix flaviceps adults by sex and month

3 Results

Infant care-giving behavior and changes in the composition of the C. flaviceps group during the present study period have been described elsewhere (Ferrari 1992; Ferrari and Diego 1992). Overall, there was little difference in the contribution of males and non-breeding females to the rearing of infants (carrying and food sharing). The adult male Simão left the study group at the end of May to join a neighboring group containing a single adult female and two adult males. In mid-June, the non-breeding females Cuba, Dida, and Spock left the study group to join the two males from the group into which Simão had immigrated for the formation of a new social unit of five adults; they were subsequently observed within the home range of the main study group on a regular basis.

During the 7 months considered here, social behavior, predominantly play, allo­grooming, and scent marking, accounted for 10.4% of scan sample records (n = 40713). Agonistic behavior accounted for no more than 0.2% of records in any one month. Even when behavioral sampling is included, agonism was observed at a maximum rate of 3.4 records per adult in any 1 month (March), and physical contact was extremely rare. The maximum monthly rate for submissive categories was similar (3.8), as might be expected given the complementary nature of these categories.

Agonistic interactions between males were virtually nonexistent, with a single record collected in January (Table 8.3). Interactions between females were more frequent, but account for a little over one tenth of the records, and almost invariably involved the breeding female (Maggie). Maggie was aggressor in all but one record of intra-female agonism, and received two-thirds of intra-female submissions (Table 8.4).

Intra-female agonism was concentrated in the first 3 months of the study period, when dependent infants were present in the group, and basically resulted from the breeding female’s monitoring of caregivers. When infant distress vocalizations were insistent, the breeding female normally reacted by approaching, threatening, and even attacking adult caregivers (both male and female). This occurred in the context of both infant carrying and the solicitation of food items (see Ferrari 1987). By contrast, no agonism between females was recorded in June, the month during which Cuba, Dida, and Spock emigrated.

Most of the inter-sexual agonism involving the breeding female was also related to the monitoring of caregivers. While males were aggressive towards Maggie on two occasions, she was never observed exhibiting submissive behavior towards individuals of either sex, which would appear to confirm her dominant position within the group. Similarly, a male was submissive towards a non-breeding female on only one occasion, in contrast with 170 records of non-breeding female→male submission. Males were also five times more aggressive towards non-breeding females than vice versa. Overall, then, the data indicate that males were socially dominant over non-breeding females.

While males as a whole were clearly dominant over non-breeding females, the almost total lack of intra-gender agonism obscures any possible linear hierarchy. There was also no clear tendency for a given male to be more aggressive than others (January to May: χ 2 = 4.96, df = 3, p > 0.20), nor for a given non-breeding female to be more submissive (χ 2 = 6.22, df = 3, p > 0.05).

Despite being able to exploit plant exudates systematically as a substitute source of carbohydrates during periods of fruit scarcity, marmosets are vulnerable to seasonal fluctuations in the abundance of arthropod prey, which declined progressively during the present study period, accompanying the transition from the wet to the dry season (Ferrari 1988). Over the year as a whole, feeding on animal material declined significantly with decreasing arthropod abundance, as did foraging efficiency and prey selectivity. However, the relative contribution of animal material to the C. flaviceps diet varied little between January (17.4% of feeding records) and June (19.8%), whereas it fell to 9.3% in July, that is, after the emigrations.

The exact consequences of such seasonal changes are unclear, but the fact that Maggie gave birth to a set of healthy twins towards the end of September appears to support the idea that nutritional stress was not a factor. While the observed changes do suggest that intra-specific competition increased during the course of the study period, there is no evidence to suggest that this resulted in any change in the relationships between group members (see Fig. 8.1), in particular with regard to the possibility that emigrations were stimulated by an increase in intra-group agonism. Inter-gender aggression was in fact recorded less frequently than expected (homogeneous distribution) in May (χ 2 = 3.83, df = 1, p > 0.05) prior to the emigration of Simão, and barely more than average in June 1 (χ 2 = 0.18, df = 1, p > 0.70).

4 Discussion

The social dominance of a single breeding female or male/female pair is a well-documented characteristic of callitrichid social organization, one which has a well-defined role in the reproductive biology of these primates (Abbott et al. 1993; French 1997). In general, relationships between other group members have received less attention, although tolerance is the norm (at least in the wild), even between unrelated group members or potentially reproductive adult males. Captivity often accentuates intra-group agonism (Epple 1975; Rothe 1978; Box and Morris 1980; Abbott 1984; Snowdon and Soini 1988; Moura 2003), which may result in the formation of a distinct, intra-sexual hierarchy (Rothe 1978; Fuchs et al. 1991). But up to now, little evidence has been found of social dominance between the sexes (Evans and Poole 1984; Sutcliffe and Poole 1984; Digby 1995a), as might be expected for such a sexually monomorphic mammal (Kleiman 1977).

The gender-based social hierarchy described here thus represents a somewhat novel interpretation of the social organization of a callitrichid species. Unfortunately, with the exception of data collected at the present study site (Ferrari and Diego 1992; Guimarães 1998), virtually nothing is known of the behavior of C. flaviceps, so it remains unclear whether the patterns reported here are typical of the species, and even less so, to what extent similar patterns might be expected in other members of the genus. In fact, with one exception (Digby 1995a), very little is known of intra-group relations in Callithrix species. Digby found some evidence of an age-based hierarchy in the common marmoset, Callithrix jacchus, although the social context was different from that in C. flaviceps, in particular the presence of two breeding females in each of the three study groups.

What does seem most likely, from these scant data, is that social structure and intra-group relations in marmoset species are as variable as their mating patterns (Ferrari and Digby 1996; Ferrari et al. 1996). As for mating patterns, group composition may play at least as important a role as ecological variables. Except for recruitment through births, the composition of the C. flaviceps study group remained unchanged for at least 18 months prior to the emigrations reported here, and it seems likely that most if not all group members were closely related (Ferrari and Diego 1992). All three C. jacchus groups appeared to be less stable, by contrast, suffering losses of both adult and immature members (Digby and Barreto 1993), in addition to at least one infanticide (Digby 1995b). The latter, in particular, appears to reflect more intense competition for reproductive success, as might be expected from the presence of two breeding females in each group.

The relatedness of group members may be a key factor in intra-group relations, and may account for much of the difference between captive and field data, given that the stable resource base provided by the marmosets’ gum-feeding specializations is common to all species (Soini 1982; Ferrari and Digby 1996; Ferrari et al. 1996; Passamani 1996; Veracini 1997, this volume Chapt. 12). Ideally, it would be valuable to compare systematically the social relations in groups of different composition, although up to now, there has been only one field study in which the genetic relatedness of all group members is known (see Faulkes et al. Chapt. 5 this volume). Systematic differences between marmosets and tamarins (Leontopithecus and Saguinus) might also be expected, given the ecological and demographic differences between these two groups (Ferrari and Lopes 1989).

The present study also reinforces the highly systematic nature of dispersal and group formation in this species, at least. The only pattern observed in the present study was an apparent increase in agonistic interactions between the breeding female and non-breeding females at the time of the birth. Certainly, there is no evidence to suggest that intra-group agonism was influenced in any way by resource abundance or intra-group competition, nor that emigrations were related to either factor. It thus seems more likely that the primary determinant of both emigrations was the availability of potential mates in neighboring groups. Timing may also have been important, given that Simão’s emigration coincided with the period during which Maggie would have conceived the litter born in late September.