Skip to main content

Monolingual-Bilingual (Non-)convergence in L3 Rhythm

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Speech Rhythm in Learner and Second Language Varieties of English

Part of the book series: Prosody, Phonology and Phonetics ((PRPHPH))

Abstract

This study examines the production of speech rhythm in Turkish-German bilinguals and German monolinguals in their L3 English and their L1/L2 German. A variety of durational and pitch-based rhythm metrics were calculated from read speech produced by the participants in both English and German. A comparison of rhythm metrics across groups and languages revealed that (a) the productions of bilinguals and monolinguals differ both in their English and in their German, (b) the groups vary more in their English than in their German productions, and (c) the source of variation cannot always be clearly attributed to CLI from the bilinguals’ L1 Turkish since unexpected results emerged in the L3 English. We take these findings as further evidence for a combined multilingual language system in which all background languages are interconnected and may be the source of CLI, which is not only conditioned by both universal and language-specific factors, but also manifested as property-by-property and bit-by-bit transfer.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    One reviewer rightly noted that the subjects may have been speakers of the German multiethnolect Kiezdeutsch which is, among other things, associated with Turkish immigrant identity. This cannot be ruled out since their Turkish was not examined for accentedness in this case. We would then be dealing with an English accent associated with the variety of German that is spoken by the participants rather than with their HL. See Sect. 6 for more details.

  2. 2.

    Usually, only PVI-V is normalized for speech rate in the field, but not PVI-C, since speech rate has only been found to correlate with the vocalic and not the consonantal variables. Since it is difficult to justify this practice in crosslinguistic studies involving languages with different vocalic and consonantal inventories as well as diverging phonotactic structures, we will use the normalized PVIs of both variables (i.e., nPVI-V as well as nPVI-C) to allow for a more objective measure of pairwise variability in speech.

  3. 3.

    Note that there are several other rhythm metrics, often slight mutations of each other, that are rarely used and are thus not mentioned here. See Fuchs (2016) for a review.

  4. 4.

    But, see Gut (2010) for a case study on four trilingual speakers with different background languages that show that these speakers produced rhythmic structures that were distinct from those of native speakers.

  5. 5.

    Although age of acquisition in German differs in both groups, due to early immersion, the bilingual group is generally perceived as native-like, with presumably pervasive nuances that may emerge in highly scrutinized contexts such as laboratory settings.

  6. 6.

    See Arvaniti (2012), who showed that different types of rhythm can be elicited from one and the same language if the prosodic structure of words used in sentences is manipulated in a way to foster a specific type of rhythm.

  7. 7.

    While we used a categorization into obstruents and sonorants, we will consider obstruents to be equivalent to consonantal material and sonorants equivalent to vocalic material. Thus, measures based on obstruents will be referred to as consonantal metrics (e.g., VarcoC instead of VarcoO), while those based on sonorants will be referred to as vocalic ones (e.g., VarcoV instead of VarcoS).

References

  • Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of general phonetics. Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arvaniti, A. (2012). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40(3), 351–373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bardel, C., & Falk, Y. (2012). The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, S. Flynn, & J. Rothman (Eds.), Studies in bilingualism. Third language acquisition in adulthood (Vol. 46, pp. 61–78). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.46.06bar

  • Bardel, C., & Lindqvist, C. (2007). The role of proficiency and psychotypology in lexical cross-linguistic influence: A study of a multilingual learner of Italian L3. Atti del VI Congresso Internazionale dell’Associazione Italiana di Linguistica Applicata, 123–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, C., Nota, A., Sprenger, S. A., & Schmid, M. S. (2016). L2 immersion causes non-native-like L1 pronunciation in German attriters. Journal of Phonetics, 58, 71–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2016.07.001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birdsong, D., Gertken, L. M., & Amengual, M. (2012). Bilingual language profile: An easy-to-use instrument to assess bilingualism. COERLL, University of Texas at Austin. Web. 20 Jan 2012. https://sites.la.utexas.edu/bilingual/

  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.40. Retrieved May 11, 2018, from http://www.praat.org/

  • Boukrina, O., & Marian, V. (2006). Integrated phonological processing in bilinguals: Evidence from spoken word recognition. In Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabrelli Amaro, J. (2017). Testing the phonological permeability hypothesis: L3 phonological effects on L1 versus L2 systems. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 698–717. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916637287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, P. M. (2005). Quantifying rhythmic differences between Spanish, English, and Hispanic English. In R. S. Gess & E. J. Rubin (Eds.), Amsterdam studies in the theory and history of linguistic science Series 4, Current issues in linguistic theory: Vol. 272. Theoretical and experimental approaches to Romance linguistics: Selected papers from the 34th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages (LSRL), Salt Lake City, March 2004 (Vol. 272, pp. 63–75). Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.272.05car

  • Dasher, R., & Bolinger, D. (1982). On pre-accentual lengthening. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 12, 58–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalysed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dellwo, V. (2006). Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for deltaC. In P. Karnowski & I. Szigeti (Eds.), Language and language-processing (pp. 231–241). Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dellwo, V., & Wagner, P. (2003). Relationships between rhythm and speech rate. Presented at the 15th International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, August 3–9, 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Domahs, U., Genç, S., Knaus, J., Wiese, R. & Kabak, B. (2012). Processing (un)-predictable word stress: ERP evidence from Turkish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Domene Moreno, C. (2021). Beyond transfer? The acquisition of an L3 phonology by Turkish-German bilinguals. Doctoral thesis, University of Würzburg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Féry, C. (2010). The intonation of Indian languages: An areal phenomenon. In Imtiaz Hasnain and Shreesh Chaudhury (Eds.), Festschrift in honour of Ramakant Agnihotri.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, S., Foley, C., & Vinnitskaya, I. (2004). The cumulative-enhancement model for language acquisition: Comparing adults’ and children’s patterns of development in first, second and third language acquisition of relative clauses. International Journal of Multilingualism, 1(1), 3–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790710408668175

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, R. (2016). Speech rhythm in varieties of English: Evidence from educated Indian English and British English. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel, C., & Rusca-Ruths, E. (2015). Der Sprachrhythmus bei deutsch-türkischen L3-Spanischlernern: Positiver Transfer aus der Herkunftssprache? In S. Witzigmann & J. Rymarczyk (Eds.), Mehrsprachigkeit als Chance (pp. 185–203). Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabe, E., Post, B., & Watson, I. (1999). The acquisition of rhythmic patterns in English and French. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 1201–1204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grabe, E., & Low, E. L. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In C. Gussenhoven & N. Warner (Eds.), Laboratory phonology VII (pp. 515–546). Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gut, U. (2010). Cross-linguistic influence in L3 phonological acquisition. International Journal of Multilingualism, 7(1), 19–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gut, U. (2012). Rhythm in L2 speech. In D. Gibbon, D. Hirst, & N. Campbell (Eds.), Rhythm, melody and harmony in speech: Studies in honour of Wiktor Jassem, special edition of speech and language technology (pp. 83–94). Poznan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gut, U. (2002). Prosodic Aspects of Standard Nigerian English. In Gut Ulrike, Gibbon Dafydd (Eds.), Typology of African prosodic systems (pp. 167–178). Bielefeld. Handel, S., & Lawson, G. R. (1983). The contextual nature of rhythmic interpretation. Perception & Psychophysics, 34(2), 103–120. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211335

  • Güneş, G. (2013). Limits of prosody in Turkish. In E. E. Taylan (Ed.), Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi (the Journal of Linguistics Research), special issue “Updates in Turkish Phonology”, 133–169. Boğaziçi University Press, Istanbul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Güneş, G. (2015). Deriving prosodic structures. Doctoral Dissertation. University of Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Handel, S., & Lawson, G. R. (1983). The contextual nature of rhythmic interpretation. Perception & Psychophysics, 34(2), 103–120. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211335

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Kroll, J. F. (2001). Do bilinguals activate phonological representations in one or both of their languages when naming words? Journal of Memory and Language, 44(1), 2–31. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2747

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabak, B., & Vogel, I. (2001). The phonological word and stress assignment in Turkish. Phonology, 18, 315–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabak, B. (2014). Pervasive syllables. In J. C. Reina & R. Szczepaniak (Eds.), Syllable and word languages (pp. 112–139). Linguae & Litterae Series. DeGruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabak, B. (2016). Refin(d)ing Turkish stress as a multifaceted phenomenon. Paper delivered at Second Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL-2), October 7–9, 2016, Indiana University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamali, B. (2011). Topics at the PF interface of Turkish. Doctoral thesis, Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kisler, T., Reichel, U. D., & Schiel, F. (2017). Multilingual processing of speech via web services. Computer Speech & Language, 45, 326–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kopečková, R. (2016). The bilingual advantage in L3 learning: A developmental study of rhotic sounds. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 410–425. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790718.2016.1217605

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kopečková, R., Marecka, M., Wrembel, M., & Gut, U. (2016). Interactions between three phonological subsystems of young multilinguals: The influence of language status. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 426–443. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790718.2016.1217603

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, J.-P. & Jang, T.-Y. (2004). A comparative study on the production of inter-stress intervals of English speech by English native speakers and Korean speakers. Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, 1245-8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi, S. V. (2005). Acoustic correlates of lexical accent in Turkish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 35, 73–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, A., & Post, B. (2014). L2 acquisition of prosodic properties of speech rhythmL2: Evidence from L1 Mandarin and German Learners of English. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36(02), 223–255. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263113000752

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ling, L. E., Grabe, E., & Nolan, F. (2000). Quantitative characterizations of speech rhythm: Syllable-timing in Singapore English. Language and Speech, 43(Pt 4), 377–401. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309000430040301

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Llama, R., & López-Morelos, L. P. (2016). VOT production by Spanish heritage speakers in a trilingual context. International Journal of Multilingualism, 13(4), 444–458. https://doi.org/10.1080/14790718.2016.1217602

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lloyd-Smith, A., Gyllstad, H., & Kupisch, T. (2017). Transfer into L3 English. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(2), 131–162. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.15013.llo

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louriz, N. (2007). Alignment in L3 phonology. Langues Et Linguistique, 18(19), 129–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., Kovack-Lesh, K. A., Goodwin, D., & McEchron, W. (2013). Infant directed speech and the development of speech perception: Enhancing development or an unintended consequence? Cognition, 129(2), 362–378. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mok, P. P. K. (2011). The acquisition of speech rhythm by three-year-old bilingual and monolingual children: Cantonese and English. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(04), 458–472. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000453

  • Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–766. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.3.756

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nespor, M., Shukla, M., & Mehler, J. (2011). Stress-timed vs. syllable-timed languages. In M. Van Oostendorp, C. J. Ewen, E. V. Hume, K. Rice (Eds.). The Blackwell companion to phonology, Vol. II. (pp. 1147–1157). Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nokes, J., & Hay, J. (2012). Acoustic correlates of rhythm in New Zealand English: A diachronic study. Language Variation and Change, 24(01), 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394512000051

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polyanskaya, L., & Ordin, M. (2015). Acquisition of speech rhythm in first language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(3), EL199–204. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4929616

  • Pycha, A. (2006). A duration-based solution to the problem of stress realization in Turkish. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report, 141–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73(3), 265–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00058-X

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roach, P. (1982) On the distinction between ‘stress-timed’ and ‘syllable-timed’ languages. In D. Crystal (Ed.) Linguistic controversies (pp. 73–79). Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothman, J. (2010). L3 syntactic transfer selectivity and typological determinacy: The typological primacy model. Second Language Research, 27(1), 107–127. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658310386439

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sánchez, L. (2017). An inquiry into the role of L3 proficiency on crosslinguistic influence in third language acquisition. ODISEA. Revista de estudios ingleses. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.25115/odisea.v0i15.282

  • Schmid, M. S., & Dusseldorp, E. (2010). Quantitative analyses in a multivariate study of language attrition. Second Language Research, 26(1), 125–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Setter, J. (2006). Speech rhythm in world Englishes: The case of Hong Kong. TESOL Quarterly, 40(4), 763. https://doi.org/10.2307/40264307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabakova, R. (2016). The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 651–665. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916655413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The MathWorks. (2018). MATLAB user's guide. The MathWorks, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgersen, E. N., & Szakay, A. (2012). An investigation of speech rhythm in London English. Lingua, 122(7), 822–840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.01.004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valdés, G. (2005). Bilingualism, heritage language learners, and SLA research: Opportunities lost or seized? The Modern Language Journal, 89(3), 410–426. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2005.00314.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Geert, P. (2008). The dynamic systems approach in the study of L1 and L2 acquisition: An introduction. The Modern Language Journal, 92(2), 179–199. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2008.00713.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vicenik, C., & Sundara, M. (2013). The role of intonation in language and dialect discrimination by adults. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 297–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westergaard, M., Mitrofanova, N., Mykhaylyk, R., & Rodina, Y. (2016). Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The linguistic proximity model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 21(6), 666–682. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006916648859

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitworth, N. (2002). Speech rhythm production in three German-English bilingual families. Leeds Working Papers in Linguistics and Phonetics, 9, 175–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiese, H. (2012). Kiezdeutsch. Ein neuer Dialekt entsteht. C.H. Beck.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrembel M. (2011). Cross-linguistic influence in third language acquisition of voice onset time. In W.-S. Lee & E. Zee (Eds.). Proceedings of the 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. 17–21 August 2011. Hong Kong (pp. 2157–2160). City University of Hong Kong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zora, H., Heldner, M., & Schwarz, I.-C. (2016). Perceptual correlates of Turkish word stress and their contribution to automatic lexical access: Evidence from early ERP components. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2016.00007

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Marlene Keßler for her assistance in the coding of the data, as well as the two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. Needless to say, all errors are our own.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christina Domene Moreno .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Appendices

Appendix 1: Reading Text English

One day Catherine and Rose, two recent Caltech graduates who had been friends ever since they had been little children, decided to go on a three-week hiking trip to the Laprig hills together. They met at Rose’s place to make travel plans: to book their hotel room, decide on various places they wanted to visit for their breakfasts, lunches and dinners and find out about the rangers’ warnings they had to heed. Rose had prepared the most wonderful meal—potato soup, cooked plums with root vegetables and cranberry sauce, and apple ice cream with sunflower seeds for dessert—and she had put a full bottle of expensive red wine into the fridge. Rose was extremely excited about their holiday and wanted this evening of planning to be perfect. They used to go to the theater together every Thursday, but the two young women hadn’t really spent quality time with each other for the last three years since they both had extremely busy schedules. At six o’clock sharp Catherine rang the doorbell and Rose opened her front door. After they had shooed away a rook from the front porch, the two friends hugged happily and went inside, where Catherine hung up her bag on the coat rack. Rose asked her to take a seat. Doing as she had been bid, Catherine sat down at the clean wooden table and Rose went into the kitchen to fetch drinks. While Catherine was sitting in the living room on her own contemplating the neat yellow pattern on Rose’s wallpaper she heard the strangest noise. It sounded as if a panther cub was trying to roar, and she could not place it anywhere: It might have originated outside the window just as well as right under the plush sofa that she could just see from where she was sitting. For a moment Catherine was worried, but then Rose came back with two crystal glasses and the wine, so Catherine forgot about the incident.

Appendix 2: Reading Text “Der Nordwind und die Sonne”

Einst stritten sich Nordwind und Sonne, wer von ihnen beiden wohl der Stärkere wäre, als ein Wanderer, der in einen warmen Mantel gehüllt war, des Weges daherkam. Sie wurden einig, dass derjenige für den Stärkeren gelten sollte, der den Wanderer zwingen würde, seinen Mantel abzunehmen. Der Nordwind blies mit aller Macht, aber je mehr er blies, desto fester hüllte sich der Wanderer in seinen Mantel ein. Endlich gab der Nordwind den Kampf auf. Nun erwärmte die Sonne die Luft mit ihren freundlichen Strahlen, und schon nach wenigen Augenblicken zog der Wanderer seinen Mantel aus. Da musste der Nordwind zugeben, dass die Sonne von ihnen beiden der Stärkere war.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Domene Moreno, C., Kabak, B. (2023). Monolingual-Bilingual (Non-)convergence in L3 Rhythm. In: Fuchs, R. (eds) Speech Rhythm in Learner and Second Language Varieties of English. Prosody, Phonology and Phonetics. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-19-8940-7_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics