Keywords

Introduction

The great Colombian author Gabriel García Márquez reified the cyclical nature of history in his Nobel Prize-winning 1967 book, One Hundred Years of Solitude. Eugen Bleuler’s less-famous book Dementia Præcox or the Group of Schizophrenias [1], originally published in 1911, saw first use of the term “autism”, a form of solitude manifest by Bleuler as withdrawal from reality in schizophrenia. This neologism, about to celebrate its centenary, epitomizes an astonishing cycle of reification and change in nosology, a cycle only now coming into clear view as molecular-genetic data confronts the traditional, age-old categories of psychiatric classification.

The purpose of this article is to review and evaluate the relationship of schizophrenia with autism, to help illuminate the causes of both conditions. To do so, I integrate data from recent studies of genomics, neurodevelopment, psychology, and psychiatry, in the context of alternative hypotheses for how schizophrenia and autism are related to one another, genetically, developmentally, psychologically, and with regard to the evolution of social-cognitive and social-affective phenotypes in humans.

I first provide a brief history of the term “autism”, in the context of schizophrenia, Kanner’s autism, and diagnostic methods. This background is necessary for understanding how “autism” and “autistic” are currently conceived and applied in the literature.

Second, I review evidence regarding the phenotypes and phenotypic structure of autism, and schizophrenia, and how they relate to diagnostic classifications. I introduce the concept of “positive” symptoms as manifestations of autism, to help in contrasting autism with schizophrenia psychiatrically, psychologically, and with regard to underlying neurodevelopment, physiology, and brain morphology and function.

Third, I describe alternative models for how schizophrenia-spectrum conditions and autism are related to one another psychologically, developmentally, neurologically, and with regard to evolution of the human social brain. Each of these models makes specific predictions that can be tested in a strong-inference framework, using data on causes, correlates and consequences of variation in neurodevelopment.

Finally, I discuss the primary implications of this review for practical applications, including pharmacological therapy, strategies for uncovering genetic risk factors, integration of etiological studies across causal levels from genes to psychiatric phenotypes, and conceptualization of psychiatric conditions in relation to normative development.

“Autism” from Bleuler to the DSM

One of the most important symptoms of schizophrenia is the preponderance of inner life with an active turning-away from the external world. The most severe cases withdraw completely and live in a dream world; the milder cases withdraw to a lesser degree. I call this symptom “autism.” [1, p. 397]

The outstanding, “pathognomonic”, fundamental disorder is the children’s inability to relate themselves in the ordinary way to people and situations from the beginning of life. … This is not, as in schizophrenic children or adults, a departure from an initially present relationship; it is not a “withdrawal” from formerly existing participation. From the start there is an extreme autistic aloneness. [2]

Kanner [2] struggled to establish “autism” as a condition separate from schizophrenia, especially given the tendency among psychiatrists to consider schizophrenia as some function of Bleuler’s “four As” of autism, association, affect, and ambivalence [3]. Kanner’s attempts to conceptually-extract autism from schizophrenia initially failed, and throughout much of the 1950s, 1960s and 1970s, autism was considered as a manifestation or subtype of schizophrenia in children (“childhood-onset” schizophrenia) [4, 5]. Eventually, documentation by Kolvin [6] of a bimodal onset or diagnosis of autism (in early childhood) and schizophrenia (in adolescence), and comprehensive reviews by Rutter [79] contrasting the correlates and phenotypes of autism and schizophrenia, validated autism as a separate and distinct condition. This condition was formalized by DSM-III in the context of “infantile autism” as a subgroup of “pervasive developmental disorder” [10]. The initial diagnostic criteria for “infantile autism” included: (1) lack of responsiveness to others, (2) language absence or abnormalities, (3) resistance to change or attachment to objects, (4) the absence of schizophrenic features, and (5) onset before 30 months. A more-flexible DSM-III-R, based in large part on work by Rutter [9], saw the first formalization, in “autistic disorder”, of the familiar triad of impairments: (1) reciprocal social interaction; (2) verbal and non-verbal communication; (3) restricted repertoire of activities and interests; with diagnostic thresholds based on minimum numbers of specific behaviors in each category, and overall.

DSM-IV was fundamentally similar to DSM-III, but with the addition, among other diagnoses, of PDD-NOS (pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified). PDD-NOS is defined as “severe and pervasive impairment in the development of reciprocal social interaction or verbal and nonverbal communication skills, or when stereotyped behavior, interests, and activities are present”, when the full criteria are not met for autistic disorder or other conditions (including schizophrenia and schizotypal personality disorder), due to late age of “onset” (after age 3), or atypical or sub-threshold symptoms.

Finally, nascent DSM-V simplifies diagnostics by subsuming the full set of closely-related idiopathic autism spectrum conditions (autistic disorder, Asperger’s syndrome, and PDD-NOS) under a single category, “autistic disorder”, with three criteria: (1) persistent deficits in social communication, reciprocity, and peer interactions, (2) restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interests and activities, and (3) symptoms present in early childhood but possibly “not fully manifest until social demands exceed limited capacities”.

Most modern research on autism spectrum conditions, and their relationship to schizophrenia, has been conducted under the rubrics of DSM-IV, which was published in 1994. Under these criteria, PDD-NOS, considered as an autism spectrum condition, applies to any child with social-behavorial impairments salient but sub-threshold to autistic disorder. The PDD-NOS category has been applied very extensively, with diagnostic prevalence comparable of that of autism and Asperger’s syndrome combined in some populations (e.g. [11]), probably due in part to its lack of specificity [12]. About half of PDD-NOS cases involve clinical social and communication deficits, but sub-threshold impairments in repetitive behavior and restricted interests [13].

With regard to diagnostic accuracy, and conceptualization of how autism is related to schizophrenia from a neurodevelopmental perspective, two key questions follow from work conducted under DSM-IV: how often premorbidity to schizophrenia is sufficiently severe to result in autism spectrum diagnoses during childhood, and how often schizophrenia that lacks prominent positive symptoms has been diagnosed as autism spectrum or considered “autistic”. These questions will be addressed below, in the contexts of information on manifestations of premorbidity to schizophrenia, and genetic data on the causes of schizophrenia and autism.

Despite the clear separation of schizophrenia from autism provided by all versions of the DSM, Bleuler’s concept of “autistic” deficits in schizophrenia has persisted in the literature, probably due to the prominent impairments in social reciprocity, language, and theory of mind found in most schizophrenic individuals [1418]. Such impairments are, moreover, frequently combined with elements of obsessive or compulsive behaviors [19], as is autism [20]. These phenotypes of schizophrenia correspond closely, of course, with the tried of traits used to describe and diagnose autism. Conflation of schizophrenia with autism can usually be avoided by the frequent presence of prominent “positive” symptoms, such as hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorder, characteristic of schizophrenia and exclusionary of autism in the DSM. However, such positive symptoms are also highly variable and episodic, and patients with schizophrenia or schizotypy typified by predominantly negative and disorganized symptoms overlap in their profiles of psychological and behavioral symptoms with some autistics [2125]; such individuals are also, like autistics, most-commonly male [26].

These considerations suggest that many schizophrenics and individuals with schizotypal personality disorder are likely to score in the “autism” or “autism spectrum” (including PDD-NOS) ranges on DSM-based or other metrics of “autistic” symptoms, as demonstrated by several studies (e.g., [22, 27, 28]). Of such metrics, the most inclusive is represented by the “broad autism phenotype” [29], a set of language and personality traits that are similar to, but milder than, those found in autism. Comparison of the broad autism phenotype questionnaire [29] to its equivalent in the study of schizophrenia, the schizotypal personality questionnaire [3032], indicates considerable similarity between measures of the broad autism phenotype and the interpersonal and disorganized dimensions of schizotypy. Such resemblances are validated by scores above the broad autism phenotype cutoff for schizophrenic and schizotypal individuals in one study [33], and supported by similarities between questionnaire measures of Asperger’s syndrome and schizotypy [23]. These similarities and possible “false-positive” diagnoses apparently reflect the central roles of deficits in social cognition and behavior, and unusual patterns of interests and compulsions, among both the autism and schizophrenia spectra, regardless of their underlying causes.

Deficits in sociality and language, and the presence of repetitive behavior and restricted interests, usually remain idiopathic, but are also not uncommon in a variety of neurogenetic conditions, such as Velocardiofacial syndrome, Klinefelter syndrome and Prader-Willi syndrome, that involve neurological phenotypes of schizophrenia in a large subset of individuals, and a high incidence of schizophrenia and schizotypy in adults [3445]. Individuals with such syndromes – which are usually recognized in childhood – have often been diagnosed as “autistic” if they meet all three autism criteria, or “autism spectrum” (usually PDD-NOS) if the fit between phenotypes and criteria is less complete. “Autism spectrum” diagnoses are thus usually applied, in both idiopathic and syndromic cases, to children who are under the usual age (in the late teens) at which schizophrenia is expressed. As a result, individuals who are “premorbid” for schizophrenia are not diagnosed as such (due to a lack of specific, predictive criteria for later development of schizophrenia), but are categorized as “autism spectrum” if their social, language, and/or repetitive-behavioral restricted-interest impairments are sufficiently severe. Social, language, and other developmental deficits and delays are indeed common among children – especially boys – who later develop schizophrenia [27, 4654]. How often do such phenotypes motivate childhood diagnoses of “autism spectrum”?

The frequency of “false-positive” autism spectrum diagnoses [55] among pre-schizophrenic or schizotypal children remains unclear, especially for individuals without identified genetic or genomic risk factors. However, the tendencies for genomic copy number variants to exert severe effects on diverse aspects of early neurodevelopment [56], and for schizophrenia of earlier onset to exhibit a higher male sex-ratio bias and a stronger tendency to be associated with copy-number variants rather than other factors [5, 57] suggests a substantial risk for false-positive diagnoses of autistic spectrum conditions [4, 28, 33, 49, 55] at least for individuals with genomic or genetic risk factors of high penetrance. The most direct evidence for diagnostic conflation of the autism spectrum with premorbidity to schizophrenia, schizotypy, or other schizophrenia-spectrum conditions comes from diagnoses of autism spectrum conditions in children with deletions at 15q11.2, 15q13.3, and 22q11.21, and duplications of 16p11.2, copy number variants for which schizophrenia risk has been well-established from studies of adults [36, 49, 58, 59, 6062]. By contrast, autism-associated copy number variants, such as deletions at 16p11.2 [63], or duplications at 22q11.21 [64] have seldom also been reported in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia and have not been established as risk factors.

Differentiating autism spectrum conditions from false-positive diagnoses of premorbidity to schizophrenia requires judicious use of intermediate, biologically-based phenotypes including “positive” symptoms of autism (as described below), adoption of relatively-new diagnostic categories such as Multiple Complex Developmental Disorder [65, 66], and use of data from genomic risk factors in developmental, longitudinal perspectives. In particular, children with diagnoses of autism spectrum conditions who harbour deletions at 15q11.2, 15q13.3, or 22q11.21, duplications at 16p11.2, supernumary X chromosomes (Klinefelter syndrome in males), paternal deletions of 15q11–13 or maternal uniparental disomy for chromosome 15 (Prader-Willi syndrome), or other well-validated schizophrenia-spectrum genomic risk factors, might usefully be monitored for the development of schizophrenia spectrum conditions after the onset of adolescence, and studied for the presence of biologically-based schizophrenia-spectrum phenotypes (e.g., [43, 67]; see also [44, 62]). To the degree that false-positive diagnoses of autism spectrum conditions in childhood are not uncommon, and autism and schizophrenia are underlain by different genetically-based risk factors, inclusion of children premorbid for schizophrenia in studies to uncover novel genetic or genomic risk factors for autism will dilute the probability of detecting significant results.

Consideration of autism from the general perspective of impaired social interaction, due to some combination of deficits in reciprocity and language, and repetitive, restrictive interests and behavior, cycles us back to Blueler’s original incarnation. The extensive cognitive-affective and behavioral variation among children with Kanner’s autism has motivated, justifiably, a wide diagnostic net – but such a net, especially when structured mainly by the presence of deficits, may capture individuals with impairments due to highly diverse causes. Ultimately, the diagnostic bases of autism and schizophrenia should presumably be founded on models of causation, models that can subsume the diversity of developmental, physiological, morphological, behavioral and psychological phenotypes found in each condition under a relatively small set of convergent frameworks. Only in this way can the diagnostic categorizations of autism and schizophrenia each be reconciled with their genetic and neurodevelopmental foundations, and nosology be connected with proximate and ultimate causes [68]. These are fundamentally important issues because, as described below, many researchers consider autism and schizophrenia to be similar conditions based on the centrality of social deficits in both, while others consider them to be separate and independent (as in the DSM), or diametric.

Phenotypic Structure of Autism and Schizophrenia Spectrum

Reification of autism and schizophrenia as “real” categories, rather than pragmatic human constructs, may be a natural consequence of their fundamental, long-term historical role in structuring 100 years of thought and research into causes of human psychiatric conditions. The usefulness to society of these constructs has been extensively demonstrated, but evaluation of their status as dimensional versus categorical, and causally divisible into components versus unitary, has required data on the genetic bases of the conditions, their sets of diagnostic criteria, and the relationships between their component phenotypes. Such information has become available and begun to solidify mainly within the past few years, allowing the first explicit comparisons between the phenotypic structures of schizophrenia and autism.

First, schizophrenia (and related conditions) and autism each encompass sets of dissociable but more or less related phenotypes. Schizophrenia is not clearly separable from bipolar disorder and major depression; these conditions overlap broadly in core phenotypes, exhibit joint familial aggregation even when underlain by single, segregating penetrant risk factors such as DISC1 loss of function, and are mediated by genetic risk factors that may influence liability to pairs or trios of the conditions, or specific symptoms such as aspects of psychosis (e.g., [6972]). Considered together, these conditions are thus referred to as the “schizophrenia spectrum”, or “psychotic-affective spectrum”, and considered as a set of genetically and phenotypically related conditions comprising a mosaic of partially-overlapping but dissociable psychotic, manic, affective and other symptoms. Any given individual can be conceptualized as occupying a zone in three-dimensional space defined by degree of expression of psychotic (including positive, negative and disorganized), manic and affective phenotypes, or, more specifically by a multidimensional space that comprises the constellation of phenotypes found in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depression. Van Os [73] provides a useful depiction of such a space, in the context of “salience syndrome” as a unified framework for understanding phenotypes associated with psychosis. Phenotypes of the schizophrenia spectrum are dissociable, in that specific symptoms can and do occur in isolation, and components are relatively independent genetically (e.g., [74, 75]) – but they often occur together, presumably due to pleiotropy and shared neurodevelopmental causes. However, the relationship of psychotic, manic, affective, and other schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms to one another remains poorly understood.

The trio of phenotypes diagnostic of idiopathic autism has been demonstrated to be partially if not largely dissociable, in that the phenotypic and genetic correlations between measures of impairments in social reciprocity, language, and restrictive, repetitive behavior and interests are modest [12, 76, 77]. Autism can thus be considered as a set of social and behavioral traits that are genetically and phenotypically related in that they occur together somewhat more often than expected by chance, or interact additively or in synergy to produce a recognizable pattern, but are also separable, in that any specific component of the three can and does occur in isolation. Any given individual can thus be conceptualized as occupying some area in a multi-dimensional space with axes defined by the diagnostic triad and their severities [12]. The relationship of social with non-social phenotypes remains poorly understood [78], and the degree to which the three core components of autism would emerge from factor analysis of a much wider range of childhood behavioral phenotypes than previously included (e.g., [77]) has yet to be ascertained.

Unlike the schizophrenia spectrum, the autistic spectrum is defined predominantly by the presence of deficits and impairments, rather than by “positive” symptoms that represent novel patterns of behavior, cognition and affect that are reasonably specific to the condition or its subtypes. A suite of “positive” symptoms can be described for autism (Table 7.1), although each of them applies more or less strongly across subsets of autistic individuals. The primary usefulness of “positive” autistic phenotypes rests in their use for deliniation of discrete sets of causes that convergently mediate autism, and in their ability to facilitate differential diagnosis of autism from other conditions characterized by social and communicative impairments, such as relatively-severe premorbidity to schizophrenia.

Table 7.1 Psychological, behavioral, neurological and physiological traits that can be considered as “positive” symptoms of autism

Second, studies of the genetic basis of schizophrenia in relation to schizotypy, the prevalence of autistic and schizotypal traits in non-clinial populations, familial aggregation of phenotypes considered as autistic or schizotypal, quantitative-genetic studies of autistic phenotypes, and studies of genetic variation in specific genes subject to either large-scale or small-scale alterations, provide convergent evidence that schizophrenia-spectrum traits, and autistic-spectrum traits, each grades more or less smoothly from severe impairment into normality [98100]. In accordance with such continuity, autism and schizophrenia have each been demonstrated to be strongly mediated by effects from de novo and segregating genetic variation of small and large effects, with high heritabilities, but also with high levels of genetic heterogeneity, such that a large number of genetic risk factors, separately and/or in combination, convergently mediates the risk of each condition [101103]. A primary implication of the continuity between these clinical disorders and normality is that the phenotypes characteristic of schizophrenia and autism spectrum conditions can each be considered as extreme manifestations of normal human variation in cognitive and affective phenotypes (e.g., [73, 104, 105]) rather that pathology disembodied from human adaptation.

For both the schizophrenia and autism spectrum, the neurocognitive adaptations most relevant to the disorders are components of the human “social brain” – the integrated, distributed set of neural systems that subserves the acquisition, processing and deployment of social information [44, 106108] (Fig. 7.1). To the extent that these psychiatric conditions are mediated by dysregulated development and function of the social brain, understanding the ontogeny, modularity, integrated ultimate and proximate functions, and genetic bases of social-brain components becomes fundamental to dissecting the etiologies of autism and schizophrenia, and ultimately, to defining disorders of human development based on biological criteria. A simple, testable model for conceptualizing the adaptive significance of social-brain phenotypes, in relation to their maladaptive dysregulation in schizophrenia spectrum conditions, is provided in Fig. 7.2. This model can be tested and refined by evaluating the normal adaptive functions of specific cognitive-affective phenotypes (e.g., [109]) in relation to their forms of dysregulation in more or less severe psychiatric conditions, and by systematically elucidating the cognitive-affective correlates of validated schizophrenia or autism “risk” alleles in non-clinical populations (e.g., [110, 111]).

Fig. 7.1
figure 7_1_215985_1_En

Functional partitioning of components of the human “social brain”, mainly from imaging and lesion studies. A = amygdala; aCC = anterior cingulate cortex; FG = fusiform gyrus; iOG = inferior orbital gyrus; latPC = lateral parietal cortex; mPFC = medial prefrontal cortex; oPFC = orbital prefrontal cortex; pCC = posterior cingulate cortex; pIFG = posterior inferior frontal gyrus; pSTG = posterior superior temporal gyrus; PT = planum temporale; STS = superior temporal sulcus. The cerebellum (“C”) also plays important roles in social-brain cognition and affect. Neuroanatomical positions are approximate

Fig. 7.2
figure 7_2_215985_1_En

The social, cognitive-affective context of schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes, considered as dysregulated manifestations of neurological-psychological traits that are conditionally adaptive

Despite the continuity of the autism spectrum and the schizophrenia spectrum with normality, the multidimensional distributions of autistic and schizophrenic-spectrum phenotypes in populations of clinical and non-clinical individuals considered together remain unclear, in part because they are some function of the spectrum of salient genetic and environmental variation present in populations. Thus, if the bulk of variation in such phenotypes is mediated by relatively common, segregating alleles each of small effect, normal distributions are expected – as for any polygenic trait. By contrast, rare, commonly de novo mutations of large effect, such as highly-penetrant copy number variants and monogenic causes of autism, should tend to produce discontinuities, with “spikes” in the distributions at the clinical ends of the spectra and their components. Each type of genetic risk factor has now been well documented, especially for schizophrenia, but the spectrum of effect sizes, and risk allele frequencies, remain to be fully elucidated, with much of the heritability for each set of conditions yet to be discovered [112].

The confrontation of genetic and genomic data with data on diagnosis-related phenotypes represents one of the most important ongoing developments in understanding the etiologies of autism and schizophrenia. A useful set of models relating genes to psychiatric conditions and their diagnostic phenotypes is described by Fanous and Kendler [70]: allelic variation may affect risk of the condition itself, risk of some subset of clinical features of the condition, or expression of clinical features but not risk. Genes “for” autism or schizophrenia [68] have thus far been evaluated in genome-wide association, copy number variant, and deep-sequencing studies predominantly under models of causation that involve genetic factors mediating risk of the full diagnoses as threshold traits, rather than mediating phenotypic expression, qualitative or quantitative, of the component phenotypes. To the extent that diagnostic phenotypes are genetically as well as phenotypically dissociable, and to the extent that neurodevelopmental disorders are mediated by sets of distinct genetic causes that convergently yield particular diagnostic phenotypes, this approach will be notably inefficient. Much of the heritability of schizophrenia and autism may thus be presently “missing” due to the phenotypic and genetic structures of the disorders, and not the limitations of genomic technology. Despite such considerations, the observation of a proportion of cases of autism or schizophrenia that appears attributable to losses of function or dosage alterations in single genes (e.g. AUTS2, FMR1, DISC1, and NRXN1, among others) indicates that single genes may pleiotropically mediate expression of the full suite of symptoms sufficient for diagnosis, perhaps due to extensive pleiotropy (e.g., [113]). Such data provide evidence for coherence of autism and schizophrenia as biologically-based conditions, with shared causal linkages between their components.

The primary question that follows from these considerations is whether or not the phenotypic and genetic structures of the autism and schizophrenia spectra – their partially-dissociable phenotypes, continuity with normality, highly-heritable nature, and notable clinical and genetic heterogeneity – can be reconciled such that the two conditions are causally and functionally related in some manner that can be evaluated statistically. If so, then the criteria for defining the two sets of conditions can, in principle, be grounded within a common non-arbitrary, biologically-based, non-historical, yet pragmatic causal framework. Addressing this question involves consideration of explicit alternative models for how the schizophrenia spectrum and autism spectrum are related to one another, and how strong inference can be used to evaluate which model or models best fits the available data.

Alternative Models for the Relationship of Schizophrenia with Autism

Differentiating between alternative models for the relationships of major human psychiatric conditions with one another has important implications for diagnoses, pharmacological and psychological treatment, and strategies for dissection of etiology at all levels from genes to psychology. Recent studies [114117] have demonstrated how genetic data can be deployed to evaluate explicit alternative hypotheses for the relationship of schizophrenia with bipolar disorder, originally described as a dichotomy in work that followed from the pioneering studies of Emil Kraepelin but now viewed in terms of partial overlap, based on the presence of shared genetic risk factors mediating shared phenotypes. Such analyses have only recently become feasible with the discovery of robustly replicated genetic risk factors, and they allow objective criteria to be applied to nosological frameworks that have thus far been difficult to evaluate rigorously.

Studies by Craddock and colleagues [114117] have provided strong evidence for continuity between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, because specific genetic risk factors – both highly-penetrant alterations and common polymorphisms of small effect – predispose to either one or both of the conditions. As a result, despite the common presence of patients with “pure” psychotic or mood symptoms, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder grade together phenotypically, apparently due to a continuum of shared and separate genetic risk factors. Such structure should, eventually, allow clinical heterogeneity to be connected with, and mapped onto, genetic heterogeneity, in the additional context of environmentally-based risk.

The relationship of schizophrenia with autism remains much less clear [44, 62, 118120]. For some years, Kanner [121] considered autism as a distinct, early-expressed subtype of schizophrenia, similar in general form to negative-symptom schizophrenia. As noted above, this viewpoint persisted for several decades in the context of autism as a manifestation of childhood-onset schizophrenia, and as a disorder characterized predominantly by social deficits similar to those found in negative-symptom schizophrenics [122]. Under this model (Fig. 7.3a), autism should be mediated by a subset of negative-symptom schizophrenia genetic-risk factors – presumably those with early onset or high severity.

Fig. 7.3
figure 7_3_215985_1_En

Alternative models for the relationship of autism with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder, in comparison to normality. (a) Subsumed [121, 122], (b) Separate [123, 79; DSM III–V]; (c) Diametric [44, 62]; (d) Overlapping I [129]; (e) Overlapping II [117]

Kanner [123] later renounced the idea of autism as a subtype of schizophrenia, in favor of a view, also supported by Rutter [7, 8], with the conditions as distinct, separate and unrelated to one another (Fig. 7.3b). By this model, autism and schizophrenia should not overlap in genetic risk factors, unless some risk alleles are sufficiently general to underlie a broad swath of cognitive-affective dysfunctions.

Under each of these two hypotheses, autism and schizophrenia each grades independently into normality. Under a third model (Fig. 7.3c), schizophrenia and autism have been considered as diametric (opposite) sets of conditions along a spectrum of social-brain phenotypes, from hypo-development in autism, to normality, to hyper-development in schizophrenia [44, 124]. The idea of diametric disorders is novel to psychiatric classification, but can be conceptualized as analogous to neurodevelopmental pathways that can be perturbed in two opposite directions, or signaling pathways can become under- versus over-activated. Schizophrenia spectrum conditions may thus develop in part due to losses of function in negative, homeostatic regulators of social-cognitive-affective phenotypes (phenotypes that in autism never matured), which leads to differential deficits in social skills that may appear similar to those in autism but develop by fundamentally-different means (Fig. 7.4). Examples of such losses in negative regulation may include over-activity of the mid-brain dopaminergic systems leading to inappropriate assignment of motivational salience to irrelevant objects, people and actions [125], “jumping to conclusions” as a risk factor for the formation of delusions [126] and impairments of corollary discharge systems mediating aspects of hallucinations and passivity phenomenon (e.g., [127, 128]). In a developmental context, social skills are impaired during childhood in autistics due to hypo-development of social brain phenotypes, and, in some cases, childhood social skills are also reduced in individuals premorbid for schizophrenia, though for other reasons.

Fig. 7.4
figure 7_4_215985_1_En

The development of autism and schizophrenia, with regard to the timing, causes and expression of social-cognitive-affective impairments

Diametric etiology of the autism and psychotic-affective spectra, in the context of the normally-developing social brain, takes account of their fractionable natures in that corresponding hypo- and hyper-developed social-brain phenotypes can be described for the two conditions, each of which may be underlain by some subset of the genetic risk factors that underly the full spectra. Each phenotype of the autism and psychotic-affective spectra can also be directly related to normal social-brain functions that became highly-developed along the human lineage (Fig. 7.5), such that expression of these psychiatric phenotypes becomes explicable in terms of the recent expansion and elaboration of the human social brain [44, 107, 108]. By contrast, other models of autism in relation to the psychotic-affective spectrum remain disconnected from human evolution and the cognitive-affective adaptations that characterize our lineage.

Fig. 7.5
figure 7_5_215985_1_En

The relationships of autism spectrum and psychotic-affective spectrum phenotypes to social-behavioral-cognitive phenotypes that have become highly developed and elaborated along the human lineage

By a fourth model, autism overlaps broadly yet partially with schizophrenia, sharing some risk factors and phenotypes but not others (Fig. 7.3d). This model has been motivated by recent genetic evidence for shared loci and pathways mediating both autism and schizophrenia risk (e.g. [129, 130]), as well as by work describing social deficits as central to both Kanner’s autism and the “autistic” symptoms of Blueler’s schizophrenia, as described above. Under this hypothesis, autism and schizophrenia are expected to share a subset of risk alleles, which underlie shared social-deficit phenotypes, but each condition should also be mediated by unshared risk alleles that mediate unique phenotypes.

Finally, Craddock and Owen [117] have proposed a variant of the overlapping model, under which schizophrenia overlaps partially with autism along a spectrum of increasing cognitive impairment, with autism grading into mental retardation (Fig. 7.3e). By this hypothesis, autism and schizophrenia should be mediated by a partially-overlapping set of genetic risk factors, with autism the result of more-severe alterations to the genetic bases of neurodevelopment. This model can help to explain evidence for: (1) high comorbidity of autism with intellectual disability; (2) the presence of cognitive impairments but not mood-disorder symptoms in schizophrenia, autism and intellectual disability; and (3) data showing overlap of risk loci between schizophrenia and autism, as well as between autism and intellectual disability [62, 73, 131, 132].

The primary assumptions involved in using strong inference to evaluate the predictions of these alternative hypotheses include: (1) the absence of ascertainment biases, which have been proposed to explain reports of relatively low IQ in autism spectrum conditions, given that lower-IQ individuals are more likely to be brought to the attention of clinicians ([133]; see also [84, 85]); (2) accuracy of diagnoses, especially with regard to “false positive” diagnoses of relatively-severe premorbidity to schizophrenia as autism spectrum, as described above; (3) validity of the associations between genetic risk factors and diagnosed conditions, especially for autism, which has been subject to considerably less well-replicated, large-sample genetic study than schizophrenia; and (4) the possibility that the hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, due to the presence of genetic variants and polymorphisms that increase risk of any psychiatric condition non-specifically, or due to relationships between autism and schizophrenia that are more complex than the models depicted here. These assumptions are unlikely to be met, but each can be addressed with regard to future studies and the nature of the potential biases and inaccuracies that they introduce.

Crespi et al. [62] used copy number variant and genetic association data to evaluate the former four hypotheses described above. They reported evidence of statistically significant increased sharing of risk genes, from genetic association studies, between autism and schizophrenia. This sharing of risk genes was generated by two patterns in the relationships of alleles and genotypes to the conditions: (a) for some loci, the same allele mediated risk of both conditions; and (b) for other loci, autism was associated with one allele or genotype and schizophrenia was associated with the other; other loci could not be directly compared due to heterogeneity of markers or complexity of the findings.

For copy number variant data, Crespi et al. [62] found diametric patterns at three (16p11.2, 22q11.2, 22q13.3) of the seven copy number variant loci with sufficient data for analysis: deletions were statistically associated with one condition from case-control studies, and duplications were statistically associated with the other. By contrast, one of the loci (16p13.1) showed duplications associated with both conditions, and for 1q21.1, deletions were associated with both autism and schizophrenia (though with only two autism cases reported, compared to 15 cases for schizophrenia), whereas duplications were associated only with autism.

Taken together, these results from genetic-association and copy-number variant data can be interpreted as apparent falsification of the hypotheses that autism is subsumed within schizophrenia (Fig. 7.3a), or that schizophrenia and autism are genetically independent of one another (Fig. 7.3b). The findings of diametric patterns for several copy number variant loci, and some genetic-association loci, are compatible with the diametric model (Fig. 7.3c) but the presence of some unambiguously-shared risk factors support a model of partially-shared risk and some degree of overlap, or a substantial degree of false-positive diagnoses of premorbidity to schizophrenia as autism (or negative-symptom schizophrenia and schizotypy as autism). These questions require further, focused study in hypothesis-testing frameworks, to assess the prevalence of false positives, and jointly evaluate the genetic underpinnings of autism and schizophrenia using genome-wide approaches with the same suites of markers.

One possible reconciliation of diametric and overlap models is that some set of loci exerts diametric genetic effects, while at other loci, some alleles mediate mild impairments of social cognition that increase risk of both autism and schizophrenia. For example, in autism such alleles may contribute to hypo-development of social brain phenotypes, whereas in schizophrenia they increase the likelihood of social-brain dysregulation by other means such as deficits in theory of mind, after the social brain has developed to maturity. Alleles with relatively general effects on social-cognitive development, which would be analogous to intellectual-disability genes but differentially affect social cognition, should also contribute to premorbid phenotypes of schizophrenia involving impairments in sociality and language.

Ultimately, robust evaluation of alternative hypotheses for the relationship of autism with schizophrenia will require in-depth analyses of the neurodevelopmental and neuronal-function effects of different alterations to the genes that mediate social-brain function and dysfunction, and integrative data from diverse disciplines other than genetics, especially the neurosciences and psychology.

The primary impediments to further testing of the models are likely to be reified conceptualizations of autism and the schizophrenia spectrum divorced from social brain adaptations, and perspectives from psychology that focus on deficits rather than their causes. Indeed, substantive progress in understanding these neurodevelopmental conditions is especially likely to come from analyses that span and connect levels of analysis, from genes to intermediate neurological and behavioral phenotypes, and from such phenotypes to psychiatric conditions [117, 134137]. Head size provides a paradigmatic case in point, as relatively large head size or brain size have been reported in idiopathic autism, syndromic autism, and autism associated with specific copy number variants, and causes of large head size in autism have been ascertained in terms of growth-signaling pathway dysregulation (reviewed in [44, 62]). Large head size has also been demonstrated to mediate enhanced local compared to global information processing [138], a phenotype characteristic of autism that may connect directly to social and non-social impairments as well as explain enhancements of some sensory and cognitive skills [81].

An important practical implication of understanding the relationship between schizophrenia and autism is its potential usefulness in developing pharmacological therapies based on similar, independent, or diametric alterations to neurological systems. For example, antagonists of mGLUR5 signaling have recently been deployed for treatment of the autistic syndrome Fragile X [139], while agonists of this pathway are under development for schizophrenia [140]; similar considerations may apply to treatments for autism and schizophrenia that target the cholinergic system [141, 142]. The presence and nature of similarities and differences between autism and schizophrenia may thus reciprocally illuminate the causes and potential treatments for both conditions, and motivate psychiatric classifications and associated therapies based on biology rather than symptoms [143].

Conclusions and Future Directions

Recent accelerating progress in genetics, molecular biology, and neuroscience have brought psychiatry into a new era, facing the first realistic hopes of comprehensively characterizing the genetic underpinnings of autism and schizophrenia spectrum conditions and developing rational therapies based on alterations to sets of specific biological pathways [144]. Psychiatric classification, and psychological methods based on categorizing and correlating deficits, have lagged behind such advances, and indeed may impede progress to the extent that they conflate disparate conditions, combine individuals across highly-fractionable DSM diagnoses, deny the reality and effects of false-positive diagnoses, ignore the adaptive significance of the cognitive-affective mechanisms disrupted by autism and schizophrenia spectrum phenotypes, and echo Blueler’s century-old conceptualization of autism. Indeed, until interdisciplinary studies that integrate across levels from genes to neurodevelopment to psychological phenotypes are systematically deployed, in hypothesis-testing frameworks that use strong inference [62, 145], we should expect to remain, as penned by García Márquez, in “permanent alternation between excitement and disappointment, doubt and revelation, to such an extreme that no one knows for certain where the limits of reality lay” – for yet another 100 years.