Keywords

1 Introduction

The placebo response, once considered a nuisance in clinical trials, is now being investigated in its own right as a way to enhance treatment effects endogenously. Conditions such as pain and depression, where the outcome measures are continuous, subjective and are based on self-reports, are most likely to be subject to manipulation by placebo (Hrobjartsson and Gotzsche 2010), but the placebo response has also been noted in less subjective disorders such as Parkinson’s disease (Colloca et al. 2004; De La Fuente-Fernandez et al. 2001) and asthma (Kaptchuk et al. 2008; Kemeny et al. 2007). The most studied of these conditions to manipulations by placebo is pain. Studies of placebo analgesia give us great insight into how psychological manipulations can cause physical changes in perception.

Placebo response rates are highly variable, ranging from no response to a full response. A placebo treatment will work if it has “meaning” to the individual receiving it and it is this “meaning” that is thought to cause the variance seen in placebo response rates (Moerman 2002).

Placebo response is highly dependent on prior expectation. Keeping expectations the same leads to a reproducible placebo response (Morton et al. 2009). Conversely, varying expectations, such as altering the name of the placebo, causes the response to become irreproducible (Whalley et al. 2008). Placebo response rates also vary inter-individually when the mode of treatment changes. For example, no relationship was found between subjects’ responses to placebo pills and sham acupuncture (Kong et al. 2013).

At their simplest, the expectations generated by a treatment cause a change in the interpretation of the sensory information which is used to represent and understand the environment. In the case of placebo analgesia this leads to the individual experiencing a decrease in pain perception. However, the variability of the placebo response suggests that the mechanisms behind these changes in perception are much more complex. Here we look at evidence demonstrating that the placebo response is brought about by an enduring cognitive change in information processing.

2 Prior Expectations

If perception is receiving information about your environment, cognition can be viewed as learning or knowing about your environment. Learning is fundamental to placebo responsiveness. For instance, regular use of paracetamol leads to associations with the tablets’ size, shape, colour, packaging and taste with pain relief. In order to learn, one must first receive information and it is this prior information that allows us to generate cognitive factors such as expectations and beliefs regarding future events. Expectations of analgesia are known to modify responses to analgesic treatment, a phenomena that is illustrated by Colloca et al 2004. In this study, patients unaware that they were receiving morphine via a computer-controlled infusion (hidden administration) experienced a significant reduction in analgesia compared to patients explicitly told they would be receiving morphine to help with their pain (open administration) (Colloca et al. 2004). The placebo component of the treatment is thought to be the difference between the open administration of the treatment and the hidden administration of treatment. The strength of these treatment expectations comprises an important component of the placebo response. Parkinson’s patients given varying expectations of receiving active medication when given a placebo only experienced a significant release of dopamine when they were informed that they had a 75 % probability of receiving active medication (Lidstone et al. 2005).

Learning about treatments comes not only through our own experience, but also from knowledge we have gained from others. Gaining information from observing other people can in itself generate expectations of treatment outcome possibly by establishing “a self-projection into the future outcome (pp 33)” (Colloca and Benedetti 2009).

How do we get from an expectation of treatment to an actual placebo response? In the case of experimental placebo analgesia, one would expect that once the subject is exposed to pain after the placebo administration, they would realise that their expectation of pain relief was incorrect and would not experience an analgesic effect. Of course in some instances this is true and is a reason for the variability in magnitude of placebo response. However, in placebo responders this doesn’t happen and may be explained by how our brains process sensory inputs.

3 Signal Detection Errors and Cognitive Bias

In order to quickly interpret the environment our brain constantly generates predictions about what our senses are telling us (Kveraga et al. 2007). These predictions use our past experiences and any prior information of the situation to create a picture of what is actually happening. Changes in our environment produce sensory information that can be incompatible with the model of the environment that has been generated in the brain (Yu and Dayan 2005). If the brain’s predictions (top-down) and the sensory representations (bottom-up) don’t match up, the two sets of information are thought to integrate through an error minimization pathway. A large error signal is then projected to a higher neural region where a new prediction refined by the error signal is generated (Friston 2005). Representing this as a computational model has shown how top-down inputs reduce the uncertainty of the stimulus representation when compared with bottom-up processing alone and leads to faster processing speeds (Siegel et al. 2000). In the placebo response, the expectation of treatment is thought to create uncertainty about incoming sensory information. Siegel’s model shows how top-down/bottom-up synchrony can lead to the biased processing of top-down information.

As individuals our level of cognitive flexibility to error varies (Allan and Siegel 2002). Therefore what is immediately noticeable to one person as violating their prior expectations may be totally overlooked by another. Because there is such variability in placebo response both intra- and inter-individually, researchers are interested in being able to predict placebo responses even before placebo is administered. Studies comparing personality and placebo response indicate that suggestibility (De Pascalis et al. 2002; Morton et al. 2010a, b) and optimism (Geers et al. 2010; Morton et al. 2009) may be important correlates of placebo magnitude. How placebo responders weigh perceptual information against prior expectations has been previously tested. Screening of subjects in a visual perceptual task resulted in an experimental population of which half had a tendency to rely heavily on prior expectations, and half who tended to rely on the current perceptual information (Morton et al. 2011). Individuals who used prior expectations when making perceptual decisions in both the perceptual task and the placebo manipulation were found to have greater magnitude of placebo response (Morton et al. 2010b). These results suggest that placebo responders “ignore” the incoming sensory information to base their decisions on their prior expectations, which creates a conflict between the incoming pain signals and cognitive control. In this scenario, the placebo response should have a direct influence on electrophysiological markers of error processing. This has been shown by Koban et al. (2012) when they hypothesised that placebo analgesia “may induce a transient change in the reactivity of cognitive control networks in order to adjust for the mismatch between predicted and experienced pain” (pp 7). The authors found that placebo analgesia was related to altered error monitoring processes in a go/nogo task. The go/nogo task was specifically designed to cause a large number of response errors and therefore a large event-related potential (ERP) on the EEG that corresponded to error processing and adjustments in behavioural control and error awareness. The error processing potential amplitude was significantly increased for placebo responders in the placebo condition compared to controls. Source reconstructions of the EEG recordings showed that this effect was probably caused by increased activation of specific medial frontal and lateral prefrontal regions, regions previously demonstrated to be vital in placebo analgesia (Krummenacher et al. 2010; Wager et al. 2004). Importantly these areas are also associated with adaptive control brain mechanisms (Botvinick et al. 2001; Ridderinkhof et al. 2004) and adjustments to expectations (Koban et al. 2012; Montague and Lohrenz 2007).

4 Anticipatory Responses

The anticipation of less pain during a placebo treatment has been suggested as an important component of placebo analgesia. Imaging a placebo conditioning procedure using fMRI showed activation in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, medial frontal cortex and the anterior mid-cingulate cortex. These same areas were also found to be modulated during the anticipation of placebo analgesia (Watson et al. 2009). Learnt analgesia can have a significant effect on future anticipatory responses to pain. In a repeated placebo paradigm, participants in the placebo group not only anticipated less pain than controls after the administration of placebo but also demonstrated lowered anticipatory responses to pain before placebo administration when the treatment was repeated 2–6 weeks later (Morton et al. 2010a). Using a penalised regression procedure (LASSO-PCR) to create a model of re-analysed data from an earlier experiment (Wager et al. 2004), Wager et al. (2011) were able to predict 12 % of the variance found in the magnitude of placebo analgesia. Large magnitude placebo analgesia was related to increases in anticipatory responses in the prefrontal cortex and correlated with prior expectations of analgesia, and reduced anticipatory responses in somatosensory area 2/temporal regions. The latter probably reflects the shifting of attention away from the painful stimulus (Coghill et al. 1999). Together, these results suggest that an enduring cognitive change in anticipatory pain processing can be produced by placebo analgesia, and the engagement of emotional appraisal pathways is responsible for some of the variation in placebo analgesia.

5 Opioids in Placebo Analgesia and Distraction

Many studies have associated placebo analgesia with the activation of the endogenous opioid system and with brain areas that include the prefrontal, limbic and brainstem regions (Wager et al. 2007; Zubieta et al. 2005). Changes in activity of these brain regions are related to reductions in the physical and emotional aspects of pain experience. Placebo response is most likely initiated in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex which is regarded as a cognitive-evaluative area. The placebo analgesic effect relies on enhanced functional coupling of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex with the hypothalamus, and brainstem areas such as the opioid receptor-rich periaqueductal grey and rostral ventral medulla (Amanzio and Benedetti 1999; Eippert et al. 2009; Wager et al. 2004, 2007), areas which have consistently shown expectancy-induced increases in relation to placebo analgesia (Atlas et al. 2010; Craggs et al. 2007; Eippert et al. 2009). The activity seen during placebo analgesia within all key regions of the descending pain modulatory system is significantly decreased with naloxone, an opioid antagonist (Amanzio and Benedetti 1999; Eippert et al. 2009; Levine et al. 1979; Zubieta et al. 2005). The placebo-dependent reduction of BOLD responses in fMRI and its reversal by naloxone is most evident in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (Eippert et al. 2009). Modulation of this region has been previously demonstrated in expectation manipulations (Keltner et al. 2006). During an fMRI study to image the spinal cord during pain, participants were required to do a continuous performance task (the N-back test) in order to distract them from the painful stimulus. The distraction task significantly reduced spinal responses to painful events whilst administration of naloxone during the task selectively blocked the distraction-induced reductions on reported pain (Sprenger et al. 2012). This indicates that opioids are at least partially required for both placebo responses and distraction effects. However, evidence shows placebo analgesia is not always mediated by opioids with some placebo responses being naloxone insensitive (Amanzio and Benedetti 1999; Vase et al. 2005). For example, Eippert et al. (2009) produced a blockade of placebo-induced decreases in BOLD responses, with naloxone, in regions associated with pain. However, the behavioural response was not completely blocked by naloxone as there was no significant increase in pain ratings after its administration. This suggests that pain self-reports due to placebo can be distinct from the physiological process of nociception which firmly implicates an additional non-opioidergic component to placebo analgesia.

During opioid analgesia and placebo analgesia there is consistent overlapping of brain regions involved in pain. In opioid analgesia there is more activation in the rostral anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula, whilst placebo analgesia generates greater responses in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. It is thought that this difference can be accounted for by the error signal generated by the discrepancy between actual pain and expectations of pain relief in placebo analgesia that is not present in opioid analgesia (Petrovic et al. 2010; Wager and Roy 2010). Colloca et al. (2004) open/hidden paradigm discussed earlier demonstrated that expectations of pain relief influence the magnitude of analgesia. Using this same paradigm to test the relationship between expectations and the opiate remifentanil, Atlas et al. (2012) showed that a hidden administration of remifentanil (no expectation of analgesia) influenced different brain regions when compared to an open administration of remifentanil (expectation of analgesia). Expectation of analgesia activated lateral and ventromedial prefrontal cortices and caused reduced responses in amygdala and pain-processing thalamic and somatosensory regions whereas analgesia caused by remifentanil without expectation of analgesia produced strong decreases in the anterior cingulate cortex and the weakest effects on somatosensory areas (S2/dorsal posterior insula). This suggests expectation operated independently but alongside remifentanil to reduce pain sensation. What these studies show us is that opioids, distraction, and placebo may have a common effect on pain, but they involve dissociable brain regions.

6 Altered Sensory Processing

As discussed in the previous section, the periaqueductal grey and the rostral ventral medulla are important in the production of the opioid-mediated placebo response. These same areas are also involved in the descending inhibition of pain by diffuse noxious inhibitory control (DNIC). DNIC was first described by LeBars et al. (1979) and is an endogenous pain-modulating system which includes descending inhibitory projections coordinated in the rostral ventral medulla. DNIC is a mechanism by which the response to painful stimulation by dorsal horn wide dynamic range neurons is inhibited by a second painful stimulus (counter-irritation). This response has been previously shown to reduce the amplitude of a spinal/nociceptive flexion reflex (RIII) (Willer et al. 1989, 1990). It has been suggested that the opioid-dependent placebo response may be attributed to, or work in parallel with, the inhibition of nociceptive processing in the dorsal horn of the spinal cord. Experimentally, expectations of hyperalgesia (nocebo) have been shown to block the normal decrease in both pain perception and the nociceptive reflex activity that is usually seen during counter-irritation (Goffaux et al. 2007). In contrast, fMRI imaging of the cervical spinal cord during painful heat together with placebo treatment significantly reduced spinal activity in response to heat compared to no treatment (Eippert et al. 2009). These findings suggest that the modulation of pain by placebo affects nociceptive signal processing at the earliest stage of the central nervous system.

7 Loss of Prefrontal Regions

The activation of opioid transmission has been seen in prefrontal brain areas (Eippert et al. 2009; Zubieta et al. 2005). In neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer’s disease, loss of prefrontal lobes can have severe implications for treatment effects. Benedetti et al. (2006) applied a local anaesthetic either openly or covertly to the skin of Alzheimer’s patients to reduce burning pain after venipuncture. In this paradigm, as in Colloca et al. (2004), the placebo component of the treatment was shown by the difference in analgesia after expected and unexpected application of the anaesthetic. Frontal lobe damage often seen in Alzheimer’s can be assessed using the frontal assessment battery, a series of simple tests which identifies impairments in cognition and motor behaviour. Patients with reduced frontal assessment battery scores showed a reduced placebo component of treatment and the reduction in placebo response was correlated with reduced cognitive status and the reduced functional connectivity of the frontal lobes to the rest of the brain. Losing the placebo component reduced the effectiveness of the treatment so much that a dose increase was needed to ensure sufficient analgesia.

Of particular interest in placebo analgesia is the involvement of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, an area known for cognitive and attention-related pain regulation (Lorenz et al. 2003; Miller and Cohen 2001; Peyron et al. 2000) that has been repeatedly identified in expectation-related placebo analgesia (Wager et al. 2004; Zubieta et al. 2005). Disruption of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has been shown to interfere with placebo analgesia. Krummenacher et al. (2010) used sham repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and an expectation of pain relief to induce an increase in pain threshold and pain tolerance indicative of a placebo response. Then using low-frequency rTMS, they artificially inhibited the function of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, which disrupted the placebo response and decreased pain threshold and pain tolerance. Previously, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has been related to the generation, maintenance and manipulation of cognitive representations (Miller and Cohen 2001; Pacheco-Lopez et al. 2006) and it has also been implicated in general attentional processes (Miller and Cohen 2001). The authors suggest that the loss of placebo analgesia after rTMS can be explained by the effects of disrupting the cognitive representation of analgesia and the directing of attention towards the painful stimulus.

Conclusion

If perception is the information we receive about a stimulus, cognition is how we have learnt to deal with that information. In the context of a placebo response, the stimulus information we receive is not variable but how we have learnt to deal with it using the expectations we have formed from our prior experiences is. To suggest that placebo response is due to a simple change in perception is to suggest that the placebo response is formed by a simple mechanism. Instead we see that a network of brain areas is responsible for the formation of a response, and that the frontal cortex, particularly the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, is the core area for the cognitive modulation of pain. With no prefrontal cortex there can be no cognitive input, and with no cognitive input there can be no placebo response (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The effect of cognition on placebo response using pain as an example. The placebo treatment causes an expectation of pain relief which is out of line with the incoming pain signal. Cognition uses prior experience (learnt information) to process the error signal leading to placebo analgesia