Skip to main content

The influence of music tuned to 440 Hz & 432 Hz on the perceived arousal

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Musik & Marken

Abstract

The aim of this article is to investigate whether music tuned to A4 = 432 Hz leads to less perceived arousal than music tuned to A4 = 440 Hz.

To answer this question, an experiment with 54 subjects without any musical training was conducted. The participants heard six music excerpts (three pop and three classical) for 30 s each and tuned to both frequencies. Participants were asked to rate the songs in terms of hypoarousal and hyperarousal.

The results of the experiment showed that music tuned to A4 = 432 Hz does not result in lower, but in higher levels of perceived arousal. The data revealed a significant difference between both tunings. The results show that the frequency music is tuned to has an influence on perceived arousal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 74.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ansorge, U., & Leder, H. (2017). Wahrnehmung und Aufmerksamkeit (2. Auflage). Wiesbaden: Springer Fachmedien.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory?. Trends in cognitive sciences4(11), 417–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nature reviews neuroscience4(10), 829–839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berz, W. L. (1995). Working memory in music: A theoretical model. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal12(3), 353–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnel, A. M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., & Besson, M. (2001). Divided attention between lyrics and tunes of operatic songs: Evidence for independent processing. Perception & psychophysics, 63(7), 1201–1213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasborough, M. T., Sinclair, M., Russell-Bennett, R., & Tombs, A. (2008). Measuring emotion: Methodological issues and alternatives. In N. M. Ashkanasy & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), New horizons in management. Research companion to emotion in organizations (pp. 197–208). Edward Elgar Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamantopoulos, A., Sarstedt, M., Fuchs, C., Wilczynski, P., & Kaiser, S. (2012). Guidelines for choosing between multi-item and single-item scales for construct measurement: a predictive validity perspective. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40(3), 434–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Nasso, L., Nizzardo, A., Pace, R., Pierleoni, F., Pagavino, G., & Giuliani, V. (2016). Influences of 432 Hz music on the perception of anxiety during endodontic treatment: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Journal of endodontics42(9), 1338–1343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, E. (1934). Emotion: an example of the need for reorientation in psychology. Psychological Review41(2), 184–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fastl, H., Patsouras, C., & Rader, T. (2003). Klingt ein Flügel bei 432 Hz-Stimmung besser als bei 440 Hz-Stimmung?. In Tagungsband Fortschritte der Akustik-DAGA #2003 (pp. 528–529).

    Google Scholar 

  • Geringer, J. M., & Madsen, C. K. (1984). Pitch and tempo discrimination in recorded orchestral music among musicians and nonmusicians. Journal of Research in Music Education32(3), 195–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hansen, M., Wallentin, M., & Vuust, P. (2013). Working memory and musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 41(6), 779–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haynes, B. (2002). A history of performing pitch: the story of ‘A’. Oxford, United Kingdom: Scarecrow Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hébert, S., & Peretz, I. (1997). Recognition of music in long-term memory: Are melodic and temporal patterns equal partners?. Memory & cognition25(4), 518–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herbert, C., Junghofer, M., & Kissler, J. (2008). Event related potentials to emotional adjectives during reading. Psychophysiology, 45(3), 487–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, S., Lee, L. H. L., Gaab, N., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Cerebellar volume of musicians. Cerebral cortex13(9), 943–949.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kabuto, M., Kageyama, T., & Nitta, H. (1993). EEG power spectrum changes due to listening to pleasant musics and their relation to relaxation effects. Nippon Eiseigaku Zasshi (Japanese Journal of Hygiene)48(4), 807–818.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluger, A. N., Lewinsohn, S., & Aiello, J. R. (1994). The influence of feedback on mood: Linear effects on pleasantness and curvilinear effects on arousal. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60(2), 276–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelletier, C. L. (2004). The effect of music on decreasing arousal due to stress: A meta-analysis. Journal of music therapy41(3), 192–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pereira, C. S., Teixeira, J., Figueiredo, P., Xavier, J., Castro, S. L., & Brattico, E. (2011). Music and emotions in the brain: familiarity matters. PloS One6(11), e27241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peretz, I., Gaudreau, D., & Bonnel, A. M. (1998). Exposure effects on music preference and recognition. Memory & Cognition26(5), 884–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Petsche, H., Pockberger, H., & Rappelsberger, P. (1985). Musikrezeption, EEG und musikalische Vorbildung. EEG-EMG Zeitschrift für Elektroenzephalographie, Elektromyographie und verwandte Gebiete16(4), 183–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plantinga, J., & Trainor, L. J. (2005). Memory for melody: Infants use a relative pitch code. Cognition98(1), 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollmann, S. (2008). Allgemeine Psychologie. München: Ernst Reinhardt Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sammler, D., Baird, A., Valabrègue, R., Clément, S., Dupont, S., Belin, P., & Samson, S. (2010). The relationship of lyrics and tunes in the processing of unfamiliar songs: a functional magnetic resonance adaptation study. Journal of Neuroscience30(10), 3572–3578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawilowsky, S. S., & Blair, R. C. (1992). A more realistic look at the robustness and Type II error properties of the t test to departures from population normality. Psychological bulletin, 111(2), 352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schellenberg, E. G., Peretz, I., & Vieillard, S. (2008). Liking for happy-and sad-sounding music: Effects of exposure. Cognition & Emotion22(2), 218–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlaug, G., Jäncke, L., Huang, Y., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). Increased corpus callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia33(8), 1047–1055.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, P., Scherg, M., Dosch, H. G., Specht, H. J., Gutschalk, A., & Rupp, A. (2002). Morphology of Heschl's gyrus reflects enhanced activation in the auditory cortex of musicians. Nature neuroscience5(7), 688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, L. (1966). Habituation and stimulus novelty: A model based on classical conditioning. Psychological Review73(4), 352–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroebele, N., & de Castro, J. M. (2006). Influence of physiological and subjective arousal on food intake in humans. Nutrition, 22(10), 996–1004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Pol, B., & Addink, C. C. J. (1939). The pitch of musical instruments and orchestras. Philips Tech. Rev.4(7), 205–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vorwerk, M. [Marten]. (2017, May 24). #Vorwerk #TipOfTheWeek #157 440 Hz vs 432 Hz. Facebook. https://www.facebook.com/VorwerkMaarten/posts/1785936141432784. [Accessed 02.03.2020].

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marvin Kopka .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 Der/die Autor(en), exklusiv lizenziert durch Springer Fachmedien Wiesbaden GmbH, ein Teil von Springer Nature

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Kopka, M. (2022). The influence of music tuned to 440 Hz & 432 Hz on the perceived arousal. In: Grünewald-Schukalla, L., Jóri, A., Schwetter, H. (eds) Musik & Marken. Jahrbuch für Musikwirtschafts- und Musikkulturforschung . Springer VS, Wiesbaden. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-36472-4_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-36472-4_10

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer VS, Wiesbaden

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-658-36471-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-658-36472-4

  • eBook Packages: Social Science and Law (German Language)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics