Skip to main content

Relevance of Family Psychosocial Environment in the Language Development of Mexican Children

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Accardo, P. J., & Capute, A. J. (2005). The capute scales: Cognitive Adaptive Test/Clinical Linguistic, & Auditory Milestone Scale (CAT/CLAMS). Baltimore: Brookes Pub.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acuña, X., & Sentis, F. (2004). Desarrollo pragmático en el habla infantil. Revista Onomázein. Facultad de Letras, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile. http://www.onomazein.net/10/desarrollo.pdf

  • Bates, E., Thal, D., & Janowsky, J. S. (1992). Early language development and its neural correlates. Handbook of Neuropsychology, 7, 69–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolaños, M. C., Márquez, A., De la Riva, M., Sánchez, G. C., Gutiérrez, G. O., & Elorza, P. H. (2006). Validez de correlación del perfil de conductas del desarrollo con la escala de desarrollo infantil Bayley II. Acta Pediátrica de México, 27(4), 190–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., Caldwell, B. M., Rock, S. L., Ramey, C. T., Barnard, K. E., Gray, C., et al. (1989). HOME environment and cognitive development in the first 3 years of life: A collaborative study involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America. Developmental Psychology, 25(2), 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, R. H., Mundfrom, D. J., Whiteside, L., Barrett, K., & Casey, P. H. (1994). A factor analytic study of the infant-toddler and early childhood versions of the HOME inventory administered to white, black and Hispanic American parents of children born preterm. Child Development, 65(3), 880–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to language—A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Zeisel, S. A., & Rowley, S. J. (2008). Social risk and protective factors for African American children’s academic achievement and adjustment during the transition to middle school. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 286–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, B. M., & Bradley, R. H. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME). University of Arkansas at little Rock Little Rock.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. (2009). The processing of audiovisual speech: Empirical and neural base. In B.C. Moore, L. Tyler, & W. Marslen-Wilson. (Eds.), The perception of speech: From sound to meaning (pp. 133–146). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capute, A. J., & Accardo, P. J. (1996). The infant neurodevelopmental assessment: A clinical interpretive manual for CAT-CLAMS in the first two years of life, part 2. Current Problems in Pediatrics, 26(8), 279–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capute, A. J., Palmer, F. B., Shapiro, B. K., Wachtel, R. C., Schmidt, S., & Ross, A. (1986). Clinical linguistic and auditory milestone scale: Prediction of cognition in infancy. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 28(6), 762–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cipriani, P., & Bruni, G. (1986). Relations between sensorimotor attainments and development of language comprehension in retarded children. Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 5(Suppl), 133–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damían, D. M. (2007). El desarrollo de la comunicación y de la interacción social durante el primer año de vida. In D. M. Damían (Ed.), Desarrollo del lenguaje y la comunicación en la primera infancia (p. 214). Mexico: Trillas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enesco, I. (2004). El desarrollo del bebé. Cognición, emoción y afectividad. Madrid: Alianza Editorial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., Pethick, S. J., et al. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial 242, 59(5), 1–173. discussion 174-85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1999). The social world of children: Learning to talk. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E., & Tian, C. (2005). Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. Journal of Communication Disorders, 38(4), 271–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., & Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29(1), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., & Luce, P. A. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(5), 630–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff, K., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Karmiloff, K. (2009). Pathways to language: From fetus to adolescent. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Early language acquisition: Phonetic and word learning, neural substrates and a theoretical model. In B. Moore, L. Tyler, & W. Marslen-Wilson (Eds.), The perception of speech from sound to meaning (pp. 103–125). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M., Thomas, M., & Westermann, G. (2007). Neuroconstructivism-I: How the brain constructs cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2007). Intersubjectivity before language: Three windows on preverbal sharing. Advances in Consciousness Research, 68, 149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, B. (2009). Basic auditory processes involved in the analysis of speech sounds. In B. Moore, L. Tyler, & W. Marslen-Wilson (Eds.), The perception of speech from sound to meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olds, D. L., Robinson, J., Pettitt, L., Luckey, D. W., Holmberg, J., Ng, R. K., et al. (2004). Effects of home visits by paraprofessionals and by nurses: Age 4 follow-up results of a randomized trial. Pediatrics, 114(6), 1560–1568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., Wieman, L. A., Doyle, W. J., & Ross, C. (1976). Infant babbling and speech. Journal of Child Language, 3(01), 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (1998). Late onset canonical babbling: A possible early marker of abnormal development. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 103(3), 249–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ontiveros-Mendoza, E., Sánchez, C., Corral-Guille, I., & Barragan-Mejia, G. (2011). Desarrollo del periodo sensorimotriz en lactantes de riesgo bajo. In C. Sánchez & R. Rivera-González (Eds.), El desarrollo de la inteligencia sensorimotora de lactantes en seguimiento del neurodesarrollo (pp. 69–84). México: Universum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1981). El papel de la imitación en la formación de la representación. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 4(2), 229–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poeppel, D., Idsardi, W. J., & van Wassenhove, V. (2009). Speech perception at the interface of neurobiology and linguistics. In B. Moore, L. Tyler, & W. Marslen-Wilson (Eds.), The perception of speech from sound to meaning (pp. 249–274). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robb, M. P., & Saxman, J. H. (1990). Syllable durations of preword and early word vocalizations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 33(3), 583–593.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children’s language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schietecatte, I., Roeyers, H., & Warreyn, P. (2012). Exploring the nature of joint attention impairments in young children with autism spectrum disorder: Associated social and cognitive skills. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(1), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R. G. (2009). Handbook of child language disorders. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serra, M., Serrat, E., Solé, R., Bel, A., & Aparici, M. (2008). La adquisición del lenguaje. Barcelona: Ariel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R. E. (1980). Stages of speech development in the first year of life. Child Phonology, 1, 73–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R. E., Bernstein, L. E., & Demorest, M. E. (1993). Vocal communication in the first 18 months of life. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(3), 548–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009a). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2009b). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2005). First things first: Infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 31(1), 91–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D. (1989). The nature of the physical microenvironment: An expanded classification system. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 35(4), 399–419.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rolando Rivera-González .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Izazola-Ezquerro, S. et al. (2017). Relevance of Family Psychosocial Environment in the Language Development of Mexican Children. In: Auza Benavides, A., Schwartz, R. (eds) Language Development and Disorders in Spanish-speaking Children. Literacy Studies, vol 14. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-53646-0_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-53645-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-53646-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics