Skip to main content

Neural Reuse and the Nature of Evolutionary Constraints

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neural Mechanisms

Part of the book series: Studies in Brain and Mind ((SIBM,volume 17))

Abstract

In humans, the reuse of neural structure is particularly pronounced at short, task-relevant timescales. Here, an argument is developed for the claim that facts about neural reuse at task-relevant timescales conflict with at least one characterization of neural reuse at an evolutionary timescale. It is then argued that, in order to resolve the conflict, we must conceptualize evolutionary-scale reuse more abstractly than has been generally recognized. The final section of the paper explores the relationship between neural reuse and human nature. It is argued that neural reuse is not well-described as a process that constrains our present cognitive capacities. Instead, it liberates those capacities from the ancestral tethers that might otherwise have constrained them.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    In this prosaic example, there is no deep truth about which functions are genuinely distinct, because the individuation conditions for the functions of a coffee mug are, presumably, a matter of convention rather than discovery.

  2. 2.

    To see this, consider how difficult it is to design an experiment that might serve to falsify the claim that “x is a representation,” where x is any pattern of neural activity you choose.

  3. 3.

    Although this claim has recently been disputed, in light of new data. See Kim et al. (2017).

  4. 4.

    Although see Coltheart (2014) for a somewhat deflationary interpretation of the degree of positional robustness that is actually licensed by the neuroimaging data.

  5. 5.

    The anthropological data Dehaene offers as evidence of neural reuse may be not as straightforward as he sometimes makes it sound. Max Coltheart has argued that the uniformity to which Dehaene refers is simply not there (Coltheart 2014). I am sympathetic to Coltheart’s concerns about the evidence, but would like to resist Dehaene’s account on different grounds altogether. I will therefore just assume the evidence says exactly what Dehaene says it does.

  6. 6.

    If you want to study the site at which the VWFA will appear in the brains of children who are currently pre-literate, you have to guess where it will appear in the future. Individual variability imposes a relatively low ceiling on the accuracy of such guesses. The Dehaene-Lambertz et al. (2018) study is the first to overcome this methodological difficulty.

References

  • Anderson, M. L. (2007). Massive redeployment, exaptation, and the functional integration of cognitive operations. Synthese, 159(3), 329–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. L., & Finlay, B. L. (2014). Allocating structure to function: the strong links between neuroplasticity and natural selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, M. L., Kinnison, J., & Pessoa, L. (2013). Describing functional diversity of brain regions and brain networks. Neuroimage, 73, 50–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arcaro, M. J., Schade, P. F., Vincent, J. L., Ponce, C. R., & Livingstone, M. (2017). Seeing faces is necessary for face-domain formation. Nature Neuroscience, 20, 1404–1412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barack, D. L. (2017). Cognitive recycling. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 70(1), 239–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergeron, V. (2010). Neural reuse and cognitive homology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(4), 268–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnston, D. C. (2016). A contextualist approach to functional localization in the brain. Biology and Philosophy, 31(4), 527–550.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Changizi, M. A., & Shimojo, S. (2005). Character complexity and redundancy in writing systems over human history. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1560), 267–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, P. D., Bradley, S. P., Haught, E. J., Riggs, K. E., Haffar, M. M., Daly, K. C., & Dacks, A. M. (2017). Co-option of a motor-to-sensory histaminergic circuit correlates with insect flight biomechanics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1859), 20170339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2014). The neuronal recycling hypothesis for reading and the question of reading universals. Mind & Language, 29(3), 255–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • d’Errico, F., & Colagè, I. (2018). Cultural exaptation and cultural neural reuse: A mechanism for the emergence of modern culture and behavior. Biological Theory, 13, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1877). On the various contrivances by which British and foreign orchids are fertilised by insects. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2008). Cerebral constraints in reading and arithmetic: Education as a “neuronal recycling” process. The educated brain: Essays in neuroeducation, pp. 232–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S. (2013). Inside the letterbox: how literacy transforms the human brain. In Cerebrum: the Dana forum on brain science, volume 2013. Dana Foundation, 2013, 7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, S., & Dehaene-Lambertz, G. (2016). Is the brain prewired for letters? Nature Neuroscience, 19(9), 1192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Monzalvo, K., & Dehaene, S. (2018). The emergence of the visual word form: Longitudinal evolution of category-specific ventral visual areas during reading acquisition. PLoS Biology, 16(3), e2004103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Disotell, T. R., & Tosi, A. J. (2007). The monkey’s perspective. Genome Biology, 8(9), 226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaillard, R., Naccache, L., Pinel, P., Clémenceau, S., Volle, E., Hasboun, D., Dupont, S., Baulac, M., Dehaene, S., Adam, C., et al. (2006). Direct intracranial, fmri, and lesion evidence for the causal role of left inferotemporal cortex in reading. Neuron, 50(2), 191–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2008). Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 317–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hannagan, T., Amedi, A., Cohen, L., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Dehaene, S. (2015). Origins of the specialization for letters and numbers in ventral occipitotemporal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(7), 374–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haueis, P. (2018). Beyond cognitive myopia: a patchwork approach to the concept of neural function. Synthese, 195(12), 5373–5402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., & Taoka, M. (2012). Triadic (ecological, neural, cognitive) niche construction: a scenario of human brain evolution extrapolating tool use and language from the control of reaching actions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 10–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(25), 11163–11170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, J. S., Kanjlia, S., Merabet, L. B., & Bedny, M. (2017). Development of the visual word form area requires visual experience: Evidence from blind braille readers. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(47), 11495–11504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luque, N. R., Naveros, F., Carrillo, R. R., Ros, E., & Arleo, A. (2019). Spike burst-pause dynamics of Purkinje cells regulate sensorimotor adaptation. PLoS Computational Biology, 15(3), e1006298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaffrey, J. B. (2015). The brain’s heterogeneous functional landscape. Philosophy of Science, 82(5), 1010–1022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naschev, P., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(11), 856–869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyama, S. (2000). The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution. Durham/London: Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Parkinson, C., & Wheatley, T. (2015). The repurposed social brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 133–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2013). The relation between finger gnosis and mathematical ability: Why redeployment of neural circuits best explains the finding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 877.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 576–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, J.-M., Tryba, A. K., & Pena, F. (2004). Pacemaker neurons and neuronal networks: an integrative view. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(6), 665–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rathkopf, C. (2013). Localization and intrinsic function. Philosophy of Science, 80(1), 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rathkopf, C. (2017). Neural information and the problem of objectivity. Biology and Philosophy, 32(3), 321–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reich, L., Szwed, M., Cohen, L., & Amedi, A. (2011). A ventral stream reading center independent of reading experience. Current Biology, 21, 363–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Cohen, L., Amemiya, E., Braga, L. W., & Dehaene, S. (2012). Learning to read improves the structure of the arcuate fasciculus. Cerebral Cortex, 24(4), 989–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zerilli, J. (2019). Neural reuse and the modularity of mind: Where to next for modularity? Biological Theory, 14(1), 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Matteo Colombo, Philipp Haueis, and Lena Kästner for insightful feedback on my Neural Mechanisms Online talk, which was my first attempt to work out the issues discussed in this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Charles Rathkopf .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Rathkopf, C. (2021). Neural Reuse and the Nature of Evolutionary Constraints. In: Calzavarini, F., Viola, M. (eds) Neural Mechanisms. Studies in Brain and Mind, vol 17. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-54092-0_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics