Keywords

Introduction

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is the product of an environmental pathogen (i.e., a traumatic stressor) operating on a variety of individual diatheses or predispositions. These diatheses span the spectrum of human variation in vulnerability to psychopathology and result in extensive heterogeneity in the phenotypic expression of the disorder. One manifestation of this is a diverse pattern of diagnostic comorbidity associated with PTSD. Brown et al. (1) assessed the co-occurrence of current and lifetime Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV) anxiety and unipolar mood disorders in 1,126 community outpatients and found that, of all the disorders assessed, PTSD showed the most prevalent and varied pattern of comorbidity. Of individuals with a current diagnosis of PTSD, 92% met criteria for another current Axis I disorder, with the most frequent being major depressive disorder (77%), generalized anxiety disorder (38%), and alcohol abuse/dependence (31%). Similarly, in studies of veterans receiving clinical services, 82% of those with a current diagnosis of PTSD met criteria for another Axis I disorder (2), and the National Vietnam Veterans Readjustment Study showed that 50% of veterans with PTSD had an additional Axis I diagnosis (3 ).

Comorbidity presents significant problems to investigators engaged in the search for biomarkers of PTSD. It undermines the assumption that PTSD is a discrete syndrome that is qualitatively and mechanistically distinct from other DSM-IV disorders. It also means that multiple overlapping psychiatric phenotypes may be present in any PTSD sample, which can obscure the search for markers that reliably distinguish cases from controls. The aims of this chapter are to review research on an internalizing/externalizing model of the phenotypic heterogeneity (i.e., clinical presentations) among individuals with PTSD and to discuss the relevance of this model for the search for PTSD biomarkers. We review the literature on two measures that have been the focus of extensive PTSD biomarker research—basal cortisol levels and startle reflex amplitude—and illustrate how individual differences in internalizing and externalizing comorbidity may moderate associations between these measures and PTSD diagnostic status.

The Internalizing and Externalizing Model of Psychiatric Comorbidity

The internalizing/externalizing model of psychiatric comorbidity proposes that patterns of behavioral disturbance and psychiatric symptoms among common mental disorders cohere along two underlying, or latent, dimensions. It is rooted in over 30 years of research in the area of childhood behavior disorders (cf.4,5) and has recently come to the fore in the adult psychopathology literature as the result of a series of influential factor-analytic studies of the structure of adult mental illness (612 ). Krueger, Kendler, Kessler, and others have shown that comorbid disorders tend to cohere along these dimensions, with the alcohol and substance-related disorders and antisocial personality disorder loading on the externalizing dimension and the unipolar mood and anxiety disorders falling on the internalizing dimension. In several studies, the latter has been subdivided into correlated factors termed anxious-misery (defined by major depression, dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder) and fear (comprised of panic and phobic disorders; 8,1214). This structure has demonstrated invariance across genders and multiple samples drawn at random from a larger sample (6,8) and has also been shown to account for patterns of diagnostic comorbidity in combat veterans (15 ). Twin and adoption studies have implicated genetic factors in the etiology of the externalizing and internalizing latent factors (16,17 ), and the magnitude of these genetic effects increases with the severity of the behavior problems on a given dimension (18 ).

The externalizing/internalizing model is consistent with other major models of comorbidity that posit that the co-occurrence of disorders among broad classes (e.g., the anxiety and unipolar mood disorders) is due largely to the fact that they emerge from a common diathesis (e.g., 19,20). This concept is supported by evidence suggesting that whereas much overlap exists in terms of the predisposing factors within a given spectrum of psychopathology, the manifestations of these diatheses differ considerably as a function of exposure to various environmental factors (e.g., trauma exposure, other life stressors, or developmental experiences). In other words, the different manifestations of these shared vulnerability dimensions are represented by the various DSM-IV diagnoses. This concept is in accord with a leading theoretical explanation for the high rate of co-occurrence of disorders within a spectrum of psychopathology (i.e., “Disorders A and B co-occur because they are both influenced by another underlying or causal factor C”; cf. 21), although further empirical work is needed to explore the influence of other possibilities (e.g., disorder A predisposes or causes disorder B; disorders A and B are associated because they share overlapping definitional criteria; disorders A and B should not be considered comorbid because they can be subsumed into a larger category that has been artificially split by the classification system).

Internalizing and Externalizing Ptsd Subtypes

Recent studies suggest that the internalizing/externalizing model may also be relevant to the understanding of patterns of comorbidity (i.e., phenotypic heterogeneity) among individuals with PTSD and their links to underlying biologic mechanisms and biomarkers (15,2224 ). Specifically, through a series of cluster analytic studies of personality inventories completed by individuals with PTSD, Miller and colleagues found evidence of internalizing and externalizing subtypes of PTSD in both male and female samples totaling over 1,000 subjects. Summarizing across these three studies, Miller et al. found that one subtype, termed externalizing, was characterized by the tendency to express post-traumatic distress outwardly through antagonistic interactions with others and conflict with societal norms and values. Individuals in this subgroup had elevated rates of antisocial personality and substance-related disorders, endorsed high levels of anger and aggression, and produced personality inventory profiles defined by high disconstraint (i.e., impulsivity) coupled with high negative emotionality. They described themselves as easily upset, chronically stressed, and prone to act impulsively with little regard for the consequences of their actions. On measures of personality disorder features, they described themselves as tending toward exhibitionistic, manipulative, and unconventional behavior. In both studies of veterans in which data on premilitary characteristics were available, externalizers reported elevated rates of premilitary delinquency, suggesting that these characteristics may reflect the influence of externalizing personality traits that were present prior to the trauma.

In contrast, the internalizing subtype was characterized by tendencies to direct post-traumatic distress inwardly through shame, self-defeating/deprecating and anxious processes, avoidance, depression, and withdrawal. Across these three studies, individuals in this subtype were characterized by high rates of comorbid major depression and panic disorder, schizoid and avoidant personality disorder features, and personality profiles defined by high negative emotionality combined with low positive emotionality. Individuals in this subtype further described themselves as unenthusiastic, uninspired, easily fatigued, and lacking interests, but like externalizers, prone to experiencing frequent and intense negative emotions. They reported having few friends, feeling aloof and distant from others, and preferring to spend time alone. In contrast with the externalizers, internalizers reported that they tended to be self-effacing and humble and did not feel particularly special, admirable, or talented. They endorsed a restricted range of emotions in interpersonal settings and feelings of social inhibition, inadequacy, and hypersensitivity to negative evaluation.

These findings suggest that the internalizing/externalizing model of psychopathology, originally developed to account for covariation among broad classes of mental disorders (9; c.f.11), is relevant to the understanding of the heterogeneity of PTSD comorbidity. Moreover, similar patterns that can now be interpreted as reflecting individual differences in internalizing and externalizing processes were described in earlier cluster analytic studies of U.S. and Australian veterans with PTSD (2527 ). Taken together, these findings suggest that the internalizing/externalizing model may be a useful heuristic for studying the influence of comorbidity on the relationship between PTSD and candidate biomarkers.

Relationship Between Ptsd and Cortisol and the Moderating Influence of Internalizing and Externalizing Comorbidity

The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis is a key neurobiological substrate of the stress response, and abnormalities in its functioning have long been implicated in the pathophysiology of PTSD (for reviews, see Refs. 2830). Activity of this system is initiated by the release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) from the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. CRH acts as a neuropeptide in the production of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) in the pituitary and as a neurotransmitter in an elaborate network of interconnected neurons in the limbic system, brain stem, and cortex that are reactive to exogenous challenge and endogenous distress (for a review, see Ref. 31). Once CRH is released from the paraventricular nucleus into the hypophyseal portal blood of the median eminence, it is transported to the anterior lobe of the pituitary, where it stimulates the secretion of ACTH. ACTH in turn enters the general circulation and triggers the adrenal cortex to begin the synthesis of cortisol from low-density lipoprotein (LDL) cholesterol.

Cortisol has many peripheral effects on metabolic and immune processes. Centrally, it plays an important role in negative-feedback control of HPA axis activity by binding to glucocorticoid receptors of the hypothalamus and pituitary, which inhibits the release of CRH and ACTH, respectively (32,33 ). There is also evidence that adrenalectomy, which leads to a loss of negative feedback from peripheral cortisol, results in greatly increased numbers of CRH-containing storage vesicles in the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, suggesting that cortisol also exerts inhibitory effects on CRH production in a variety of extrahypothalamic regions (34 ).

Cortisol has been the most commonly studied component of the HPA axis as a potential biomarker for PTSD because it is easily sampled, can be measured in urine, plasma, or saliva, and is a well-established marker of the body's response to stress. Unfortunately, 20 years of clinical studies that have examined the relationship between cortisol levels and PTSD have yielded complicated and mixed results. Many investigations have found lower levels of cortisol under baseline or nonstressful conditions using both urinary (35,36 ) and plasma samples (3740 ) in individuals with PTSD compared to controls. Investigators have also reported evidence for a reduced rise in salivary cortisol on awakening in PTSD (41,42 ), and patients with PTSD have been found in many studies to respond to dexamethasone administration with enhanced cortisol suppression, suggesting a heightened sensitivity to cortisol in PTSD (38,41,43,44 ). These findings are not uniform, however. Other studies have found higher levels of cortisol in individuals with PTSD compared to controls in cerebral spinal fluid (CSF;45), urine (4648 ), plasma (49 ), and saliva (50 ). Still others have shown no significant differences in salivary cortisol between groups differing in PTSD diagnostic status (51,52).

In an effort to integrate these discrepant findings, investigators have focused primarily on demographic differences between samples (e.g., age, sex, chronicity of PTSD, type of trauma) and variability in the methods used to assess cortisol, such as the time of day of sample collection, the source of the sample (e.g., plasma, urine, saliva, or CSF), or the conditions under which the sample was collected (baseline vs. pre- or post-psychological or physical stress). These factors account for a large proportion of variation in cortisol levels, and a recent meta-analysis of over 100 studies linking stress to HPA axis function showed that such factors contribute significantly to differential findings across studies (53 ). Results of that analysis also showed that, across studies, PTSD was associated with significantly lower daily levels of cortisol (Cohen's d = −0.34) and lower levels of cortisol post-dexamethasone administration (Cohen's d = −0.25) compared to control subjects exposed to the same stress.

One explanation for variation in study findings that has received little direct attention to date is the hypothesis that internalizing and externalizing comorbidity moderates the association between PTSD and cortisol levels. In many studies, the influence of comorbid disorders has not been well assessed or examined in the data analyses. When comorbidity has been factored into study designs or analyses, the focus has generally been on major depressive disorder because it is highly comorbid with PTSD and in non-PTSD samples generally associated with hyper- as opposed to hypo-cortisolism. In the section that follows, we review the literature on the differential relations between internalizing and externalizing psychopathology and cortisol levels to illustrate the moderating influences that these dimensions of comorbid psychopathology may have on the relationship between cortisol and PTSD.

Evidence for the Link Between Internalizing Disorders and Elevated Cortisol

Research on childhood behavior disorders has been the source of considerable evidence for a link between internalizing symptoms and elevated cortisol. Internalizing symptoms, such as social withdrawal and anxiety, have been associated with greater salivary cortisol levels under baseline conditions (54 ) and in response to a social stressor in children and adolescents (55,56 ). Behavioral inhibition, a trait related to shyness, withdrawal, and anxiety, evidences a similar association (57,58 ), and infants whose mothers have panic disorder have been found to have higher salivary cortisol levels compared to controls (59). Greater salivary cortisol reactivity in response to a social stressor has also been shown to prospectively predict the development of subsequent anxiety disorders (60 ).

Studies with adult participants have paralleled these findings. Specifically, adults with panic disorder have been found to exhibit higher levels of baseline plasma (61,62) and urinary and salivary ( 63) cortisol compared to controls. Numerous studies have linked basal hypercortisolism and dexamethasone nonsuppression to major depression (for a review, see Ref.64), and Miller, Chen, and Zhou's (53) meta-analysis estimated that the size of the depressed versus control group difference in postdexamethasone cortisol levels to be large (d = 1.13). Similar effects have been observed in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (65,66). Taken together, these studies provide strong evidence for a link between disorders of the internalizing spectrum and elevated levels of cortisol.

Evidence for the Link Between Externalizing Disorders and Low Cortisol

A contrasting body of research suggests an association between disorders of the externalizing spectrum and abnormally low levels of cortisol. Theorists have long conjectured that the neural substrate for externalizing disorders involves diminished arousal (67–69), and since cortisol secretion is thought to partially reflect the arousal state of the organism, many investigators have focused on cortisol as a potential biomarker for this class of psychopathology. For example, Vanyukov et al., (70) found that young boys with antisocial fathers had lower levels of baseline salivary cortisol compared to controls, and their total number of conduct disorder symptoms was inversely correlated (r = −.20) with their cortisol levels. Similarly, investigators have reported associations between low basal cortisol concentrations and aggression toward peers (71), hostility to the teacher (72), and conduct disorder severity (70). Finally, salivary (73) and serum (74) cortisol has been shown to be inversely associated with psychopathic traits (i.e., callousness, lack of remorse) in older male adolescents/young adults.

Evidence for the Link Between Externalizing Disorders and Low Cortisol

A contrasting body of research suggests an association between disorders of the externalizing spectrum and abnormally low levels of cortisol. Theorists have long conjectured that the neural substrate for externalizing disorders involves diminished arousal (6769), and since cortisol secretion is thought to partially reflect the arousal state of the organism, many investigators have focused on cortisol as a potential biomarker for this class of psychopathology. For example, Vanyukov et al., (70) found that young boys with antisocial fathers had lower levels of baseline salivary cortisol compared to controls, and their total number of conduct disorder symptoms was inversely correlated (r = −.20) with their cortisol levels. Similarly, investigators have reported associations between low basal cortisol concentrations and aggression toward peers (71), hostility to the teacher (72), and conduct disorder severity (70). Finally, salivary (73) and serum (74) cortisol has been shown to be inversely associated with psychopathic traits (i.e., callousness, lack of remorse) in older male adolescents/young adults.

Similar effects have also been observed in research with adults with externalizing disorders. For example, King, Jones, Scheuer, Curtis, and Zarcone (75) found that inpatient adults with substance use diagnoses exhibited lower baseline levels of plasma cortisol than controls. Interestingly, in that sample no participants met criteria for major depressive disorder, while in a second study of substance users with comorbid depression, elevated serum cortisol levels were found (76). There is also evidence that trait impulsivity, which is the personality dimension thought to underlie propensities toward externalizing disorders, may be inversely associated with cortisol levels. For example, Wang et al. (77) found that novelty seeking (a personality construct closely related to impulsivity) was inversely associated with urinary cortisol (r = −.43) in male Vietnam veterans with PTSD. King et al. (75) reported similar findings in a community sample control group. In sum, considerable evidence suggests that children and adults with problems in the externalizing domain exhibit low basal cortisol, and these findings align with theories that emphasize the role of hypo-arousal in the etiology of disorders in this spectrum (e.g.,69). On the other hand, high levels of basal cortisol have often been found in association with disorders of the anxious-misery spectrum (e.g., major depression and generalized anxiety disorder) and problems in the domain of excessive behavioral inhibition.

The Startle Response

Like basal cortisol, the startle response has been the focus of extensive research as a possible biomarker for PTSD. Exaggerated startle responding has been recognized as a core symptom of PTSD since the earliest descriptions of combat soldiers suffering adverse effects of exposure to the stress of combat. Many individuals with PTSD report extreme reactions to startling events in the environment. They may report flinching or recoiling in response to an unexpected touch or springing awake in response to an unexpected noise in the night and being unable to fall back to sleep. Combat veterans describe “hitting the dirt” or “ducking for cover” at the sound of a car backfiring. In each instance, the initial startle response may be followed by a cascade of anxious arousal symptoms, with recovery taking minutes to hours.

From a psychophysiological perspective, the startle response is a constellation of reflexive motor movements, phasic autonomic responses, and voluntary orienting responses that occur in response to any sudden, intense change in stimulus intensity. The reflexive component of the reaction begins with an eyeblink between 20 and 50 ms after the onset of a startle-eliciting stimulus (e.g., car backfiring) and spreads distally throughout the body. In humans, it is measured via electromyography (EMG) recordings of the contraction of the orbicularis oculi muscle, which closes the eyelid. Startle-eliciting stimuli are typically loud noises presented over headphones, and the magnitude of the muscle contraction is the primary measure of interest. Secondary, longer latency autonomic responses include heart rate acceleration and skin conductance increases. These begin within a second after the onset of a startling stimulus and typically peak several seconds later.

The startle response has attracted considerable attention as a potential PTSD biomarker. Exaggerated startle is unique among the PTSD symptoms in terms of the degree of correspondence between the clinical symptom that occurs in humans and the behavioral analogue that can studied in the clinical psychophysiology lab or using animal models. Perhaps because of this, more is known about the neurocircuitry and neuromodulators of startle than for any other symptom of the disorder. Two primary systems in the brain have been implicated as possible mechanisms for the symptom of exaggerated startle in PTSD: the locus coeruleus/norepinephrine system and the hypothalamic/CRH system. Evidence for these links includes studies showing that lesions of the locus coeruleus and drugs that inhibit its activity decrease startle reactivity, whereas drugs that increase locus coeruleus activity have the opposite effect (78). Likewise, CRH administration produces a pronounced, dose-dependent enhancement of startle that can be blocked by pretreatment with a CRH receptor antagonist (e.g.,79,80).

Although these findings point to the promise of the startle response as a possible biomarker of PTSD, clinical laboratory studies have yielded mixed findings regarding the validity of exaggerated startle as a symptom of PTSD. Pole (81) recently conducted a meta-analysis of 20 studies that compared samples of individuals with and without PTSD on measures of startle responding and found that the effect size for the eyeblink reflex, weighted to control for sample size differences, was significant but modest (r =.13). Of those 20 studies, approximately half showed significant positive group differences in eyeblink startle amplitude, with some showing quite large effects (i.e., r >.50; 82,83). These findings suggest that there may be one or more important moderating variables that have not been consistently addressed by procedures used in past startle studies.

One variable that has received considerable research attention in studies of startle in PTSD is contextual anxiety, with the idea being that exaggerated startle in PTSD is a context- or state-dependent phenomenon related to anxiety processes (84,85). This hypothesis follows from the seminal research of Michael Davis and colleagues (e.g., 86,87) on the neurobiology of fear, anxiety, and startle, which showed that the amplitude of the startle response is potentiated by both exposure to contextual threat (i.e., anxiety; as in returning to the location of previous aversive conditioning) and explicit threat (i.e., fear; as in exposure to a conditioned stimulus signaling imminent shock). In line with this, a growing body of clinical research with PTSD patients suggests that differences between PTSD and non-PTSD groups are most reliably observed under test conditions involving the distal anticipation of an aversive stimulus and are not observed under conditions involving proximal threat (for a review, see Ref. 88). For example, Grillon et al. (85) examined startle responses in veterans with and without PTSD during an initial laboratory session that involved no aversive manipulation followed several days later by startle testing during an aversive conditioning procedure that involved anticipation of a mild shock. Significant group differences in baseline startle amplitude were observed only during session 2, suggesting that group effects were linked to the anxiogenic context in which the shock conditioning took place. Consistent with the possibility that exaggerated startle in PTSD is linked exclusively to the neurobiological system underlying contextual anxiety and not to the system underlying fear, no group differences in the fear response to presentation of conditioned threat cues (i.e., a CS+) were found.

Effects of Comorbidity on Baseline and Overall Startle Amplitude

Comorbidity may also serve a moderating role in the relationship between PTSD and amplitude of the startle response. Several studies have found exaggerated startle responding in samples of individuals with other anxiety disorders, which raises questions about the specificity of the measure as a possible biomarker of PTSD. For example, exaggerated baseline startle has been observed in several studies of patients with panic disorder (8993). Grillion and colleagues (94) examined startle as a possible vulnerability marker among offspring of parents with anxiety disorders or alcoholism and found that the magnitude of baseline startle was greater in children with a parental history of anxiety than in children without a parental history of anxiety. Similar findings were observed in a sample of individuals at risk for depression by virtue of their parents' depression diagnoses (95). In addition, Cuthbert et al. (96) reported that baseline startle responses were significantly larger among anxiety disorder patients with depression than for anxiety patients without depression, suggesting an additive effect of internalizing comorbidity on startle amplitude.

Other studies suggested that startle amplitude may tap individual differences in level of trait fearfulness. For example, rats that differ in fearfulness can be distinguished on the basis of their behavioral reactions to startling stimuli, with fearful rats exhibiting more extreme responses (97). In humans, individuals who score high on Cloninger's measure of harm avoidance (i.e., “a heritable tendency to respond intensely to aversive stimuli and to learn to avoid punishment, novelty and non-reward passively”; 98) exhibit larger overall startle response amplitudes compared to their counterparts who score low on the measure (99). Together, these findings suggest a link between exaggerated startle and psychopathology of the internalizing spectrum broadly and fear-related disorders more specifically.

Conclusions and Directions for Future Research

Comorbidity of PTSD presents problems on multiple levels for clinicians and researchers alike. In the clinical arena, individuals with comorbid Axis I or II disorders have been shown to have more severe PTSD symptoms (e.g., 100102) and poorer responses to treatment (e.g.,103,104). For researchers, PTSD comorbidity often represents a nuisance variable addressed through post hoc statistical controls or challenging (and arguably ill-advised) efforts to select “pure” PTSD cases from highly comorbid samples. The heterogeneity within PTSD also means that there is potential for multiple overlapping phenotypes to occur within any sample of individuals with the disorder. As the foregoing literature review suggests, the diagnostic composition of any sample and the proportion of cases with predominantly internalizing versus externalizing comorbidity can have a substantial influence on associations between candidate biomarkers and measures of PTSD.

PTSD is not alone among psychiatric disorders in this regard. Psychiatric epidemiology studies suggest that similar heterogeneity exists in some, but not all, mental disorders with the determining factor being the breadth of comorbidity associated with the index disorder. PTSD shows a particularly severe and diverse pattern of diagnostic comorbidity with frequently co-occurring conditions ranging from those of the internalizing spectrum to the externalizing spectrum. Certain other disorders, such as alcohol abuse/dependence, show similar patterns of comorbidity. Cloninger (98) emphasized this when he advanced a two-class typology of alcohol dependence. In his model, type I alcoholics (i.e., an internalizing type) were defined as individuals who use alcohol in association with heightened negative affectivity to avoid, dampen, or escape those states. In type II alcoholism (i.e., an externalizing type), alcohol problems were thought to stem from propensities toward sensation seeking and risky or uninhibited behavior. On the other hand, other psychiatric disorders with a narrower range of comorbidity—and more distinct and circumscribed phenotypes—rarely co-occur with each other. For example, Kessler and colleagues (7) reported that the internalizing spectrum disorders obsessive-compulsive disorder and separation anxiety disorder were negatively correlated with the externalizing syndromes drug dependence and conduct disorder. Generalizing from this, one would not expect to find large numbers of externalizers among samples composed of individuals with protoypic internalizing diagnoses, especially those related to pathological fear or vice versa (e.g., antisocial personality disorder is not a problem commonly found among individuals with simple phobia).

The foregoing literature review suggests that exaggerated startle, low basal cortisol, and perhaps any candidate biomarkers for PTSD are pleiotropic, meaning that the same marker may relate to the occurrence of several different diagnostic phenotypes. We know that psychiatric disorders do not co-occur by chance but rather cohere along latent dimensions of psychopathology. By extension, we should expect most psychiatric biomarkers to correlate with disorders within a spectrum of psychopathology rather than with individual diagnoses, as basal cortisol and startle appear to. In future studies, it may be advantageous to model associations between biomarkers and phenotypes using analytic approaches that better map onto the complex nature of these associations. Latent variable methods would be well suited for the study of pleiotropic biomarkers because they can be used to model the common factor underlying a broad class of disorders as well as the unique factors that predict individual disorders. This permits the investigator to examine separately (1) the strength of associations between the candidate biomarker and the common factor underlying a set of diagnostic indicators and (2) the strength of the association between the biomarker and individual diagnostic indicators. Furthermore, the latent factors underlying co-occurring diagnoses can be conceptualized as endophenotypic traits, which represent purer, error-free measures of the dimension underlying the diagnoses of interest. As a result, one can expect latent dimensions of psychopathology to map more directly and completely onto their biologic substrate and yield substantially increased predictive power for biomarker association analyses compared to analyses that focus on identifying biomarkers of individual disorders.