Skip to main content
Log in

Quantitative Prediction and Molar Description of the Environment

  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Molecular explanations of behavior, based on momentary events and variables that can be measured each time an event occurs, can be contrasted with molar explanations, based on aggregates of events and variables that can be measured only over substantial periods of time. Molecular analyses cannot suffice for quantitative accounts of behavior, because the historical variables that determine behavior are inevitably molar. When molecular explanations are attempted, they always depend on hypothetical constructs that stand as surrogates for molar environmental variables. These constructs allow no quantitative predictions when they are vague, and when they are made precise, they become superfluous, because they can be replaced with molar measures. In contrast to molecular accounts of phenomena like higher responding on ratio schedules than interval schedules and free-operant avoidance, molar accounts tend to be simple and straightforward. Molar theory incorporates the notion that behavior produces consequences that in turn affect the behavior, the notion that behavior and environment together constitute a feedback system. A feedback function specifies the dependence of consequences on behavior, thereby describing properties of the environment. Feedback functions can be derived for simple schedules, complex schedules, and natural resources. A complete theory of behavior requires describing the environment’s feedback functions and the organism’s functional relations. Molar thinking, both in the laboratory and in the field, can allow quantitative prediction, the mark of a mature science.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anger, D. (1963). The role of temporal discriminations in the reinforcement of Sidman avoidance behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 477–506.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1973). The correlation-based law of effect. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 20, 137–153.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1981). Optimization and the matching law as accounts of instrumental behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 387–403.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (1986). Performance on ratio and interval schedules: Some data and some theory. Invited address at the convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee.

  • Baum, W. M. (1987). Random and systematic foraging, experimental studies of depletion, and schedules of reinforcement. In A. C. Kamil, J. R. Krebs, & H. R. Pulliam (Eds.), Foraging behavior (pp. 587–607). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review, 76, 49–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421–430.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Killeen, P. (1968). On the measurement of reinforcement frequency in the study of preference. Journal of the Experimental A nalysis of Behavior, 11, 263–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazur, J. E. (1986). Learning and behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H. (1976). Behavior and learning. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L., & Wynne, L. C. (1954). Traumatic avoidance learning: The principles of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility. Psychological Review, 61, 353–385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1973). On the notion of cause, with applications to behaviorism. Behaviorism, 1, 25–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1980). Optimality analyses of operant behavior and their relation to optimal foraging. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action (pp. 101–141). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, R. J. (1984). Predation. New York: Chapman and Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Baum, W.M. Quantitative Prediction and Molar Description of the Environment. BEHAV ANALYST 12, 167–176 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392493

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392493

Key words

Navigation