Introduction

Longer life expectancy has led to the growth of the older population, and older adults might account for nearly 16% of the world’s population by 2050 (1). Disorders of aging, especially neurodegenerative changes, which eventually result in dementia, has become an increasing concern, in recent years (2). With a lack of curative treatments for cognitive impairment and dementia, many studies have focused on identifying risk factors at the prodromal and preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (3). As an early pathological manifestation of brain aging, subjective cognitive decline (SCD), has become a research hotspot (4).

An international working group called the Subjective Cognitive Decline Initiative (SCD-I) focusing on advances in related research has been established (5). SCD could be defined as a self-experienced persistent decline in cognitive capacity in comparison with a previously normal status, which is unrelated to an acute event. Moreover, normal age-, sex- and education-adjusted performance on standardized cognitive tests is used to classify mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (6, 7). SCD has several alternative names, including subjective cognitive complaints (SCC) (8, 9), subjective memory decline (SMD) (10) and subjective memory complaints (SMC) (11). A previous systematic analysis provided evidence for the prognostic validity of memory complaints to predict the risk for subsequent dementia and cognitive impairment (12), while it might ignore the baseline cognitive status of included individuals. Besides, healthy controls without memory complaints should be taken into the consideration as the reference group to ensure the preciseness of analysis. Therefore, we conducted this meta-analysis in healthy population with more strict inclusion criteria. Our aim was to explore the association of SCD with the combined risk of cognitive impairment and dementia in longitudinal studies.

Methods

Search Strategy

This meta-analysis was conducted following the guidelines of the Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) (13) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (14). PubMed, Embase and Cochrane databases were searched with the same strategy ‘(subjective memory decline OR concern* OR complaint* OR SCD OR SMC OR SMD OR SCC) AND (risk OR association) AND (dementia OR cogniti* OR alzheimer* OR MCI OR mild cognitive impairment)’ from January 1st, 1970 to June 4, 2020. Conference abstracts and unpublished studies were also reviewed. Additional studies were identified by screening related reviews and reference lists of studies. If full texts were unavailable, we contacted corresponding authors.

Study Selection

The study selection process was described in Fig.1. There were 28,895 studies from three databases, after deleting duplicates in the EndNote. Studies which met the following criteria were eligible: (1) studies investigating the association of subjective memory complaints with cognitive impairment or dementia (all-cause dementia [ACD] or vascular dementia [VaD] or AD); (2) prospective longitudinal studies with a follow-up of at least 6 months; (3) studies including cognitively normal participants at baseline who were divided into an exposure group with subjective cognitive concerns (assessed by various questionnaires) and a reference group without complaints; (4) studies using recognized diagnostic criteria for objective cognitive performance (including cognitive impairment or dementia) as an end point of the study, such as the criteria made by National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA). We did not place language restrictions upon the eligibility criteria of included studies. Randomized clinical trials were excluded, as therapies, psychological suggestions and interventions provided may influence the associations of subjective memory complaints with cognitive impairment and incident dementia. Moreover, people with psychoactive medication use, neurological disease (e.g. Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, and multiple sclerosis), history of brain lesion (e.g. infection and infarction), head trauma or other systematic diseases of sufficient severity to adversely affect cognition were also excluded. First, after screening the titles and abstracts, we excluded the articles unconcerned with our topic, and only included topic related ones (n=165) for further selection. We then read full texts of those potential eligible articles, searched bibliographies of relevant reviews or meta-analyses, and finally selected 21 articles based on the criteria mentioned above.

Figure 1
figure 1

The flowchart of study selection

*: A study contains two cohorts, &: two studies with specific data on both cognitive impairment and dementia; #: Only three of five studies with specific data of both Alzheimer’s disease and non-Alzheimer’s disease.

Data Extraction

We extracted authors, year of publication, study period, country, language, sample size, inclusion or exclusion criteria, source of participants, age, numbers of male and female individuals, education, follow-up time, methods of diagnosis, count data, unadjusted and adjusted estimates of odds ratio (OR), relative risk (RR), hazard risk (HR) and their 95% confidence intervals (CI) for cognitive impairment or incident dementia. As for we encountered some studies from same cohorts, we chose the study with the largest number of included participants at the baseline. Among effective values reported in the studies, we chose the maximally adjusted estimates. If effective values were not available directly, we used RR calculated by the ratio comparing the of incident rates of cognitive impairment or dementia between exposed and reference groups. Information was first extracted by one investigator, and then checked independently by another two authors. Discrepancies were resolved by discussion. When the data we required were not available in the article, we contacted the corresponding authors for original information.

Quality Assessment

The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) has been used to assess the quality of published non-randomized studies in meta-analyses (15, 16). The NOS contains eight items which can be categorized into three dimensions (selection, comparability and outcome). A star system is employed to allow a semi-quantitative assessment of study quality, with a maximum of one star for each item except the comparability item which allows the assignment of two stars (17). The highest quality studies could be awarded a maximum of nine stars.

Statistical Analysis

We mainly analyzed the pooled RR, showing whether individuals with SCD at baseline were more likely than those without to develop cognitive impairment and dementia during follow-up in our study. Given that ORs tend to overestimate the effect sizes compared with RRs/HRs particularly when the incidence is not low, we transformed ORs into RRs using the following algorithm:(18)

RRadjusted = ORadjusted /[(1 − P0) + (P0 × ORadjusted]

P0 indicates the incidence of endpoint (dementia or cognitive decline) in the non-exposed group of the cohort. When P0 is not available, the incidence rate of total sample was used as a proxy.(18) HR, compared with RR, additionally considering the factor of time, might be approximately equal to RR at a point in time. Effective values across studies were combined to provide overall estimates and their 95% CIs using random-effects DerSimonian-Laird models (19). Participants with cognitive disorders were additionally stratified into cognitive impairment and dementia groups. Further subgroup analyses (stratified by different age, sex, year of education, follow-up time and source) were also conducted to investigate whether other factors would change the results using the same models. When calculating RR and 95%CI in subgroup analyses, participants without SCD in each subgroup were considered as the reference group. Each subgroup of basic characteristics might include at least three studies to ensure the reliability of subgroup analyses.

Heterogeneity between studies was assessed by I2 test statistics for each analysis. An I2 of less than 25% is considered as no statistical heterogeneity, 25% to 50% as low statistical heterogeneity, 50% to 75% as medium statistical heterogeneity, and more than 75% as high statistical heterogeneity (20). Meta-regression analyses (n≥10) were also conducted with robust variance estimation, assessing the potentially important covariates that might exert a substantial impact on between-study heterogeneity. Sensitivity analyses were additionally carried out to explore the source of heterogeneity by excluding one study at a time.

We also evaluated the potential publication bias with funnel plots for the outcomes, the symmetry of which was detected by Egger’s test. Egger’s test, also known as linear regression method, uses standard normal deviate and precision of included studies to establish regression equation (21). Moreover, if statistically significant publication bias was detected, the trim-and fill method was used to adjust for bias. A two-tailed P values <0.05 is considered as statistically significant. Statistical analyses were conducted in R (R programming).

Results

Basic characteristics of included studies

A total of 21 studies (2242) were selected for our metaanalyses (Fig.1). In a study conducted by Snitz et.al (36), both the individuals from communities and clinics were divided into SCD and non-SCD groups. We consider this study as two independent cohorts to include in our meta-analysis. Therefore, 22 cohorts were ultimately included in our meta-analyses. The basic characteristics of included studies are presented in Table 1. A total of 47,805 individuals from the 22 studies were included at baseline. The median of the mean age was 72.70 years old (ranging from 62.8 to 83.31 years old), except one study (25) which did not report its mean age. The female proportion of included studies ranged from 46.46% to 100% and the average years of education was more than 9 years. The average length of follow-up across studies ranged from 2 to 18 years (mean: 5.4; standard deviation [SD]: 3.6), showing an obvious difference among studies, which might contribute to higher heterogeneity. Effective values are also presented in Table 1, ranging from 0.20 to 70.10.

Table 1 Basic characteristics of included studies

Methods for assessing SCD (eg. “Do you feel like your memory is becoming worse?”) and criteria of diagnosing cognitive impairment or dementia, like NIA-AA, are presented in eTable 1 and eTable 2, respectively (Supplementary materials). Bias assessment based on the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale is provided in Supplementary eTable 3. All the included studies were of high quality, as they all got 7 or more than 7 stars (a maximum of 9 stars) (43).

Results of primary analyses

In the primary analyses, SCD showed an increased risk of developing subsequent cognitive impairment or dementia in Fig.2 (RR=2.12, 95%CI=1.75–2.58, 12=87%, P<0.01). Among the 22 cohorts included in our study, 11 cohorts with cognitive impairment as the outcome showed that 1,481 out of the total 8,346 individuals progressed into cognitive impairment at the last follow-up visit, demonstrating a significant association between SCD and cognitive impairment (RR=2.29, 95%CI=1.66–3.17, I2=83%, P<0.01) (Fig.3). And among the participants with SCD, the risk of developing dementia (RR=2.16, 95%CI=1.63–2.86, I2=81%, P<0.01) was similar to that of developing cognitive impairment (Fig.3). Individuals in four studies (24, 26, 29, 34) progressed to either cognitive impairment or dementia. Moreover, two (24, 34) of the four studies showed separate incidence rates of cognitive impairment and dementia. The other two studies showed incidence rate ratios of mixed cognitive disorders, which made it difficult for us to get the numbers of individuals who progressed to different types of cognitive disorders.

Figure 2
figure 2

SCD shows a significant association with the risk of developing objective cognitive disorders

RR: relative risk, CI: confidence interval, SCD: subjective cognitive decline.

Figure 3
figure 3

SCD shows significant associations with cognitive impairment and dementia

RR: relative risk, CI: confidence interval, SCD: subjective cognitive decline

Results of subgroup analyses

For further analysis, all included studies were stratified into subgroups based on their demographic characteristics, including age, female proportion, years of education, follow-up time and source of participants (Supplementary eTable 4). We observed that SCD conferred an excess risk of subsequent cognitive impairment in the individuals aged 65–75 years old (RR=2.29, 95%CI=1.83–2.88, I2=87%, P<0.01) (Supplementary eFig 1). SCD showed similar risks for cognitive disorders in the two subgroups stratified by female proportion (Female>50%: RR=2.18, 95%CI=1.26–3.75, 12=75%, P<0.01; Female≤50%: RR=2.11, 95%CI=1.69–2.64, I2=89%, P<0.01) (Supplementary eFig.2). There was a trend for well-educated individuals (>15 years) to be more strongly influenced by SCD (RR=3.71, 95%CI=2.10–6.56, I2=79%, P<0.01) (Supplementary eFig.3). In the subgroup with longer follow-up, individuals with SCD had a nearly doubled risk of progression to cognitive disorders (cognitive impairment and dementia) compared to those without (RR=1.98, 95%CI=1.61–2.44, I2=89%, P<0.01) (Supplementary eFig.4). In the subgroup of different settings, individuals with SCD showed approximately twice higher risks for cognitive disorders in community (RR=2.08, 95%CI=1.58–2.75, I2=88%, P<0.01) and population (mixed settings) groups (RR=1.93, 95%CI=1.37–2.72, I2=89%, P<0.01), as well as a four times higher risk for cognitive disorders in clinics (RR=4.25, 95%CI=1.08–16.77, I2=85%, P<0.01), compared with those without SCD (Supplementary eFig.5). The influence of SCD on the risks of cognitive disorders in various subgroups were summarized in eFig.6 (Supplementary materials).

When we further divided cognitive disorders into cognitive impairment and dementia, subgroup analyses were also conducted and the results were shown in eTable 5 and eTable 6 (Supplementary materials). In the subgroup analyses of the 11 cohorts focused on cognitive impairment, individuals with SCD had a higher risk of subsequent cognitive impairment in the subgroup of female proportion>50% (RR=2.64, 95%CI=1.61–4.33, I2=87%, P<0.01) (eFig.7) and in the subgroup of > 15 years of education (RR=2.64, 95%CI=1.61–4.33, I2=87%, P<0.01) (eFig.8). Additionally, the influence of SCD on cognitive impairment showed nearly no marked difference between individuals with and without the APOE ε4 allelic gene (APOE ε4+, RR=1.67, 95%CI=1.07-2.61, I2=58%, P=0.07; APOE ε4-, RR=1.89, 95%CI=1.17–3.03, I2=85%, P<0.01) (eFig.9). Individuals with SCD also showed higher risks of cognitive impairment in subgroup of 65–75 years old (RR=2.69, 95%CI=1.79–4.04, I2=83%, P<0.01), subgroup of shorter follow-up (RR=3.49, 95%CI=2.14–5.69, I2=0%, P<0.01) and subgroup of individuals from clinics (RR=8.06, 95%CI=1.68–38.67, I2=66%, P=0.09). Results on the influence of SCD on the progression into cognitive impairment were summarized in eFig.10 (Supplementary materials).

In the subgroup analysis of the cohorts which progressed into dementia, SCD individuals in the subgroup of short follow-up time showed a higher incidence rate of dementia (RR=3.40, 95%CI=1.46–7.89, I2=34%, P=0.22) (eFig.11). Moreover, when we classified dementia into AD and Non-AD groups, SCD showed a significant association with AD (RR=2.39, 95%CI=1.00–5.74, I2=76%, P<0.01), while it had a non-significant association with non-AD dementia (RR=1.37, 95%CI=0.93–2.03, I2=0%, P=0.73) (eFig.12). Results in subgroups of 75–85 years old (RR=1.75, 95%CI=1.43–2.14, I2=0%, P=0.93), female proportion more than 50% (RR=2.10, 95%CI=1.44–3.05, I2=85%, P<0.01) and individuals from clinics (RR=1.77, 95%CI=1.41–2.22, I2=0%, P=0.62) might need further investigation, since they were limited by the numbers of included studies in the subgroups. Results on the influence of SCD on the progression into dementia were summarized in Supplementary eFig.13.

Meta-Regression Analysis, Sensitivity Analysis and Publication Bias

Based on the results of meta-regression analysis (Supplementary eTable 7), the influence of the covariates on heterogeneity, such as participant’s mean age (p=0.163; 95%CI, −0.100238 to 0.0181451; τ2=0.1979), female proportion (p=0.271; 95%CI, −3.221842 to 0.9562178; τ2=0.2041), years of education (p=0.329; 95%CI, −0.068722 to 0.1892374; τ2=0.3314) and length of follow-up (p=0.231; 95%CI, −0.0968754 to 0.0248457; τ2=0.1917) were not statistically significant, as the two-tailed P values were all greater than 0.05, ranging from 0.072 to 0.928.

The sensitivity analysis showed two studies (36, 38) significantly influenced the heterogeneity (Supplementary eFig.14). When each of the studies was excluded separately, the heterogeneities still remained at 84%. The funnel plot showed relatively bilateral symmetry and the p value was 0.4557 (Supplementary eFig.15), indicating no publication bias.

Discussion

SCD was associated with a higher risk of subsequent cognitive disorders, which increased that SCD would possibly elevate the risk. Individuals with SCD showed higher risks of subsequent cognitive impairment and dementia both of which were more than two-fold compared with those without. When data were stratified by their basic characteristics, participants with SCD in subgroup of 65–75 years old, subgroup of female proportion more than 50%, long education subgroup, short follow-up subgroup and subgroup of individuals from clinics had higher risks of objective cognitive disorders. SCD participants showed significant higher risk of developing cognitive impairment compared to non-SCD participants, but there was nearly no marked difference in the rate of progression to cognitive impairment between individuals with/without the APOE ε4 allelic gene in SCD participants. Moreover, SCD participants also showed a significantly higher risk of developing AD dementia rather than non-AD dementia compared with those without SCD.

Biological alternations induced by SCD could occur before objective cognitive decline, such as gray matter volume reduction (44). Individuals with SCD have also been reported to have larger white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volumes, hippocampal atrophy (45) and increased β-amyloid (Aβ) deposition (46, 47), which are typical characteristics of AD. Furthermore, some studies illustrated that SCD was a subjective symptom reflecting anxiety or depression about senility and health rather than neurodegenerative causes (48, 49) and was just a risk factor rather than a mechanism underlying preclinical AD or other neurodegenerative dementias (6), as many participants with SCD might not develop subsequent cognitive impairment or even dementia (50). Hence, SCD was more likely to be a risk factor for cognitive disorders. Previous studies also suggested that cognitively unimpaired individuals with SCD were at a significantly increased risk of future objective cognitive disorders and clinical progression to symptomatic disease stages (12, 36, 51) which was in accordance with our results that SCD conferred excess risks of subsequent cognitive impairment and dementia. Furthermore, individuals with SCD were considered as high-risk individuals and they need necessary interventions during stages at which objective cognitive impairment remains clinically unapparent.(52)

What was more, Wang et.al found that age modified the association between SCD and future cognitive disorders, with HR decreasing from 6.0 at age 70 to 1.6 at age 80 (42). Though previous studies have proven that older elderly are more likely to develop cognitive disorders than younger elderly (12, 35, 53, 54), older elderly may have a casual attitude towards their cognitive conditions. Older elderly are less likely to worry about themselves, so subjective complaints from younger elderly are likely to be more predictive than those from older elderly. Therefore, this might explain our result that the influence of SCD was more obvious in the subgroup of older elderly. In the subgroup analysis by female proportion, individuals with SCD showed a nearly 2.5 times risk of developing cognitive impairment than those without in the subgroup of female proportion more than 50%, which was consistent with the previous conclusion that women were prone to cognitive impairment (27). A previous study reported that education affected the process of memory decline (55). Well-educated people usually seem knowledgeable, and more concerned about their health, suggesting their self-reported of SCD is more accurate. For this possible reason, longer education may contribute to an increased risk of progression from SCD to cognitive disorders, which was in accordance with the results of our subgroup analysis stratified by education including the one of all 22 studies with the outcome of cognitive disorders and the one of the 11 studies with the outcome of cognitive impairment.

Individuals with SCD in the subgroup of follow up>3years showed lower risks of developing cognitive disorders, especially dementia, compared with the subgroup of not more than 3 years, which might be explained by the increased drop-out rate or increased mortality of participants during longer follow-up. Several studies (5, 36) clearly showed that settings might affect the influence of SCD. In our study, SCD showed the strongest association with cognitive disorders in individuals chosen from clinics, as people might be classified explicitly and diagnosed in clinical settings, using available and easily measurable criteria and standard definitions of cognitive impairment or/and dementia. Moreover, previous studies also illustrated that patients in clinics were more likely to experience the first sign or the preclinical stage of a neurodegenerative disease (47, 56). A study found a significant effect of APOEε4 on memory (57). And our result suggested that SCD was a risk factor for cognitive impairment independent of the APOEε4 gene, which was likely to be limited by insufficient samples. Additionally, some individuals with SCD showed gray matter volume reduction (44) and greater similarity to an AD gray matter pattern (58) compared with subjects without SCD, which was consistent with our subgroup analyses.

There was considerable heterogeneity, which might be due to the different characteristics of individuals. Therefore, we conducted specific analyses, such as subgroup analyses based on different characteristics of studies and sensitivity analysis to find out cohorts which were significantly different from others. Apart from basic characteristics, measurements of SCD have also been reported to influence the risk of developing cognitive impairment (51). Cohorts included in our study used different assessments of SCD, which might be one of the factors leading to a bit higher heterogeneity. Recruiting larger samples, comparing important characteristics of participants, unifying the assessment of SCD and searching for methods to lower drop-out rates are necessary in future well-designed longitudinal studies.

The primary strength of our meta-analysis lies in the unity in design of studies (prospective longitudinal studies). The prospective longitudinal study minimized the potential influence of recall and selection bias, which might be inevitable in retrospective design. Besides, our retrieval was comprehensive, since we screened the three databases involving almost all available assays. Also, our search term contained, as more as possible, expressions of the same meaning we focus on (including SCD and dementia), and used “OR” as conjunctions for expressions of the same meanings, which could expand our retrieval range. Furthermore, our studies had independent blind assessments or reliable diagnostic criteria of outcomes (cognitive impairment and dementia), which were reflected in the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale questionnaire (Supplementary eTable 3). Studies included are all of high quality (Newcastle-Ottawa Scale ≥ 6 stars) (43), without having publication bias. Overall, the result that SCD increases the risk of subsequent cognitive impairment and dementia is reliable.

Limitations

There are some limitations in our meta-analysis. First, various questionnaires had different criteria for identifying SCD, which might contribute to a lack of uniformity in diagnosis of SCD. In addition, due to the association of patients and informants, the accuracy of SCD detection could be easily influenced by informants’ expectations of being normal. Second, during the follow-up, as time went on, more and more participants dropped out. Those who are lost to follow-up usually tend to be older, sicker, and have lower socioeconomic status, which might lead to attrition bias. Finally, the reliability of our subgroup analyses might be oppugned owing to our insufficient studies in certain subgroups and the possibility of type I error. Future studies are required to reduce these limitations and make more reliable inferences.

Conclusions

In conclusion, SCD is associated with an increased risk of objective cognitive disorders, including cognitive impairment and incident dementia. Individuals with SCD in subgroup of 65–75 years old, subgroup of female proportion more than 50%, longer education subgroup and subgroup of individuals from clinics showed higher risks of cognitive disorders. SCD deserve more attention, as it could serve as a potential target for early intervention trials in cognitive disorders.