Skip to main content

Intercorporeity: Enaction, Simulation, and the Science of Social Cognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Phenomenology and Science

Abstract

In this chapter, I want to address two issues. The first one is a local issue within current debates about social cognition pertaining to differences between simulation theory (ST) and interaction theory (IT) in the understanding of intercorporeity. I then want to use this issue to address a larger, less local one concerning science. More specifically, depending on what one concludes about the debate between ST and IT, the implication is that either one can continue to do science as we have been doing it, or one has to do it differently. This distinction between ways of doing science is not the same as the distinction between normal and revolutionary science described by Thomas Kuhn (1962). Something different is at stake. It’s not simply a paradigm shift that would change our conception of nature (or in this case, the nature of human behavior) in a way that would allow us to do science as usual, but rather a change in our conception of nature that would suggest a different way of doing science. This change, I’ll argue, is prefigured in the thinking of Merleau-Ponty (1967, 2012) concerning the notion of form or structure in his early works.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Bibliography

  • Anderson, M.L. 2010. Neural Reuse: A Fundamental Reorganizing Principle of the Brain. Behavioral and Brain Science 33: 245–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, K. 2008. It’s in Your Nature: A Pluralistic Folk Psychology. Synthese 165(1): 13–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, Ian A., and Stephen A. Butterfill. 2009. Do Humans Have Two Systems to Track Beliefs and Belief-like States? Psychological Review 116(4): 953.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., and S. Aglioti. 1997. The Body in the Brain: Neural Bases of Corporeal Awareness. Trends in Neuroscience 20: 560–564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlucchi, G., and S. Aglioti. 2010. The Body in the Brain Revisited. Experimental Brain Research 200: 25–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohl, V., and W. van den Bos. 2012. Toward an Integrative Account of Social Cognition: Marrying Theory of Mind and Interactionism to Study the Interplay of Type 1 and Type 2 Processes. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catmur, C., V. Walsh, and C. Heyes. 2007. Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System. Current Biology 17(17): 1527–1531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A.R. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., E. Di Paolo, and S. Gallagher. 2010. Does Social Interaction Constitute Social Cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(10): 441–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinstein, I., C. Thomas, M. Behrmann, and D.J. Heeger. 2008. A Mirror up to Nature. Current Biology 18(1): R13–R18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dominey, P.F., T. Prescott, J. Bohg, A.K. Engel, S. Gallagher, T. Heed, M. Hoffmann, G. Knoblich, W. Prinz, and A. Schwartz. Submitted. Implications of Action-Oriented Paradigm Shifts in Cognitive Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fadiga, L., L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti. 2000. Visuomotor Neurons: Ambiguity of the Discharge or ‘Motor’ Perception? International Journal of Psychophysiology 35(2): 165–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiebich, A., and M. Coltheart. 2015. Various Ways to Understand Other Minds. Towards a Pluralistic Approach to the Explanation of Social Understanding. Mind & Language 30(3): 235–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flanagan, O.J. 1992. Consciousness Reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, T., and H. De Jaegher. 2009. Enactive Intersubjectivity: Participatory Sense-Making and Mutual Incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8(4): 465–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2006. The Intrinsic Spatial Frame of Reference. In The Blackwell Companion to Phenomenology and Existentialism, ed. H. Dreyfus and M. Wrathall, 346–355. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2007. Simulation Trouble. Social Neuroscience 2(3-4): 353–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2012. The Body in Social Context: Some Qualifications on the ‘Warmth and Intimacy’ of Bodily Self-Consciousness. Grazer Philosophische Studien 84: 91–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2015. Reuse and Body-Formatted Representations in Simulation Theory. Cognitive Systems Research 34: 35–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and D. Hutto. 2008. Understanding Others Through Primary Interaction and Narrative Practice. In The Shared Mind: Perspectives on Intersubjectivity, ed. J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, and E. Itkonen, 17–38. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and S. Varga. 2014. Social Constraints on the Direct Perception of Emotions and Intentions. Topoi 33(1): 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and D. Zahavi. 2012. The Phenomenological Mind, 2nd ed. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., L. Reinerman, B. Janz, P. Bockelman, and J. Trempler. 2015. A Neurophenomenology of Awe and Wonder: Towards a Non-reductionist Cognitive Science. London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2001. The ‘Shared Manifold’ Hypothesis: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 33–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2009. The Two Sides of Mimesis: Girard’s Mimetic Theory, Embodied Simulation and Social Identification. Journal of Consciousness Studies 16(4): 21–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2010. 5.1 Embodied Simulation and Its Role in Intersubjectivity. In The Embodied Self. Dimensions, Coherence and Disorders, eds. T. Fuchs, H.C. Sattel, and P. Henningsen. Stuttgart: Schattauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2011. Neuroscience and Phenomenology. Phenomenology and Mind 1: 34–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. 2014. Bodily selves in relation: Embodied simulation as secondperson perspective on intersubjectivity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369, 20130177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., and A. Goldman. 1998. Mirror Neurons and the Simulation Theory of Mind-Reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., and V. Cuccio. 2015. The Paradigmatic Body – Embodied Simulation, Intersubjectivity, the Bodily Self, and Language. In Open MIND, ed. T. Metzinger and J.M. Windt, 14(T). Frankfurt am Main: MIND Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I. 2002. Simulation Theory and Mental Concepts. In Simulation and Knowledge of Action, ed. J. Dokic and J. Proust, 1–19. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. 2006. Simulating Minds: The Philosophy, Psychology and Neuroscience of Mindreading. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. I. 2012. A moderate approach to embodied cognitive science. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 3(1), 71–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A. I., & Vignemont, F. 2010. Is social cognition embodied? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(4), 154–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman, A.I., and C.S. Sripada. 2005. Simulationist Models of Face-Based Emotion Recognition. Cognition 94: 193–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. 1989. Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy: Second Book, Studies in the Phenomenology of Constitution. Trans. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. 1997. Thing and Space: Lectures of 1907. Trans. R. Rojcewicz. New York: Springer Science & Business Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Issartel, J., L. Marin, and M. Cadopi. 2007. Unintended Interpersonal Coordination: ‘Can We March to the Beat of Our Own Drum’? Neuroscience Letters 411: 174–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, P. 2008. What Do Mirror Neurons Contribute to Human Social Cognition? Mind & Language 23(2): 190–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, R.T. 2009. Motor Intentionality and the Case of Schneider. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 8(3): 371–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. 1992. Concerning the Ultimate Ground of the Differentiation of Directions in Space. In The Cambridge Edition of the Works of Immanuel Kant: Theoretical Philosophy, 1755–1770, ed. D. Walford and R. Meerbote, 365–372. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. 1990. Conducting Interaction: Patterns of Behavior in Focused Encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T.S. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindblom, J. 2015. Embodied Social Cognition. Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. 2013. How Things Shape the Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., and A. Iriki. 2004. Tools for the Body (Schema). Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(2): 79–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maravita, A., C. Spence, S. Kennett, and J. Driver. 2002. Tool-Use Changes Multimodal Spatial Interactions Between Vision and Touch in Normal Humans. Cognition 83(2): B25–B34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcel, A. 2003. The Sense of Agency: Awareness and Ownership of Action. In Agency and Self-Awareness, ed. J. Roessler and N. Eilan. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGivern, P. 2008. Reductive Levels and Multi-Scale Structure. Synthese 165(1): 53–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1964. Sense and Non-Sense. Trans. H. Dreyfus and P.A. Dreyfus. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1967. The Structure of Behavior. Trans. A. Fischer. Boston: Beacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 1968. The Visible and the Invisible: Followed by Working Notes. Trans. A. Lingis. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. 2012. Phenomenology of Perception. Trans. D.A. Landes. New York: Routldge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J., Christensen, W., & Overgaard, S. (2014). Mindreading as social expertise. Synthese, 191(5), 817-840.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, S.D. 2002. Integrative Pluralism. Biology and Philosophy 17(1): 55–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, D. 2010. The Enigma of Reversibility and the Genesis of Sense in Merleau-Ponty. Continental Philosophy Review 43(2): 141–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese. 2001. Neurophysiological Mechanisms Underlying the Understanding and Imitation of Action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2(9): 661–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, R.R., S. Whitfield-Gabrieli, J. Scholz, and K.A. Pelphrey. 2009. Brain Regions for Perceiving and Reasoning About Other People in School-Aged Children. Child Development 80(4): 1197–1209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schilbach, L., B. Timmermans, V. Reddy, A. Costall, G. Bente, T. Schlicht, and K. Vogeley. 2013. Toward a Second-Person Neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4): 393–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soliman, T., and A.M. Glenberg. 2014. The Embodiment of Culture. In The Routledge Handbook of Embodied Cognition, ed. L. Shapiro, 207–219. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. 1979. Communication and Cooperation in Early Infancy: A Description of Primary Intersubjectivity. In Before Speech: The Beginning of Interpersonal Communication, ed. M. Bullowa, 321–347. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, A.D., and S. Golonka. 2013. Embodied Cognition is Not What You Think It is. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltshire, T.J., E.J.C. Lobato, D.S. McConnell, and S.M. Fiore. 2015. Prospects for Direct Social Perception: A Multi-theoretical Integration to Further the Science of Social Cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 1007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodward, J. 2007. Interventionist Theories of Causation in Psychological Perspective. Causal Learning: Psychology, Philosophy, and Computation Oxford University Press 19--36 (2007).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2016 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gallagher, S. (2016). Intercorporeity: Enaction, Simulation, and the Science of Social Cognition. In: Reynolds, J., Sebold, R. (eds) Phenomenology and Science. Palgrave Macmillan, New York. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-51605-3_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics