Introduction

Sleep plays an important role in brain development and contributes to children’s health and well-being. In particular, children’s sleep duration is of great concern to parents. For example, some of parents’ most frequently asked questions to health professionals include whether their child is getting sufficient sleep, and how many hours of sleep are minimally required to achieve good health and development. However, according to a recent review on children’s sleep needs, there is no “magic number” or a specific hour-based sleep recommendation predicted upon the best available evidence [1].

The aim of this study was to investigate the possible existence of a bifurcation point in sleep duration, which could differentiate children with behavioral problems from those without. Chronotype, which indicates individual differences in diurnal preference, may affect the ability to determine sleep duration and sleep need [2]. Therefore, we included chronotype as a consideration, when examining the association between children’s sleep duration and their behavioral problems.

Materials and methods

Subjects and data collection

A total of 383 children aged 4–6 years were studied (193 boys, 190 girls; average age 4.7 years; 223 and 160 children attended kindergarten and childcare centers, respectively). We used parent-reported anonymous questionnaires on children’s sleep and health. The questionnaires were provided to the parents by teachers at kindergartens and childcare centers, located in Wako city in the metropolitan area of Tokyo, between June and July 2012. Details of the study protocol and procedures have been described previously [3]. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Institute of Public Health, Japan.

Measures

Sleep durations and chronotype

Sleep durations were measured with the sleep–wake parameters in the Children’s ChronoType Questionnaire (CCTQ) [4], for weekdays and weekends. Sleep durations were defined as follows: the difference between sleep onset time and wake-up time for sleep period, the difference between bedtime and get-up time for time in bed (TIB), and the sum of TIB and naptime for 24 h TIB. The sleep durations on weekdays were used for the analyses in this study.

Based on our previous study [3], sleep–wake timings and sleep durations were not normally distributed; therefore, the medians [interquartile ranges (IQRs)] were presented. Sleep onset time and wake-up time (h:min) were 21:20 (1:04) and 7:00 (0:45), and bedtime and get-up time were 21:00 (1:00) and 7:00 (0:45), respectively [3]. Sleep period, TIB, and 24 h TIB were 9.58 (1.17), 10.00 (1.08), and 10.50 (1.00) h, respectively [3].

Chronotype was measured with a 10-item morningness (M)/eveningness (E) scale in the CCTQ (CCTQ-M/E). The assignment of scores for items 1, 2, 8, and 9 in the CCTQ-M/E is reversed. The total score of the CCTQ-M/E, ranging from 10 to 48, was divided into three different measures of chronotypes: ≤23 points for morning-type (M-type), 24–32 points for neither-type (N-type), and ≥33 points for evening-type (E-type). Based on the previous study [3], sleep onset times were 20:50 (1:01), 21:30 (0:53), and 22:00 (0:56), and wake-up times were 6:30 (0:40), 7:00 (0:40), and 7:00 (0:40) for M-, N-, and E-types, respectively. Bedtimes were 20:30 (1:00), 21:00 (0:45), and 21:30 (1:00), and get-up times were 6:35 (0:40), 7:01 (0:34), and 7:05 (0:40) for M-, N-, and E-types, respectively [3]. Sleep periods were 9.83 (1.09), 9.50 (1.17), and 9.00 (1.11) h, TIBs were 10.17 (1.08), 10.00 (1.25), and 9.75 (1.17) h, and 24 h TIBs were 10.50 (0.83), 10.50 (1.00), and 11.00 (1.21) h for M-, N-, and E-types, respectively [3].

The CCTQ and its Japanese version have been previously reported to be psychometrically reliable and valid [46].

Behavioral problems

Behavioral problems were measured with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [7]. The SDQ comprises five subscales with five items each: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer problems, and prosocial behavior. In the subscales for emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems, in which 5 positively worded items are reversed, each subscale score ranges from 0 to 10. In addition, the scores of the subscales, except for prosocial behaviors, can be summed to generate a total difficulties score for total behavioral problems, which ranges from 0 to 40. Therefore, higher scores for the subscales, except for the subscale of prosocial behavior, represent more behavioral problems. The cut-off values for borderline and abnormal behaviors were determined using the reference values derived from a representative sample of Japanese children [8]. The SDQ and its Japanese version have been shown to have good psychometric properties [810].

Statistical analyses

Sleep durations were not normally distributed; therefore, they were presented as half-hour intervals ranging from 7 to 14 h for weekdays and weekends (Fig. 1). The medians (IQRs) of sleep durations were calculated. A series of binomial logistic regression analyses were performed to examine whether sleep duration was associated with behavioral problems. The dependent variable was the dichotomized behavior of total behavioral problems as measured with the SDQ; the cut-off value was ≥13 points. The independent variables were the sleep period, TIB, and 24 h TIB on weekdays. The bifurcation point of sleep duration was assumed to be the median value; therefore, the corresponding short and long sleep durations were set at <9.5 and ≥9.5 h for the sleep period, <10.0 and ≥10.0 h for the TIB, and <10.5 and ≥10.5 h for the 24 h TIB, respectively [3]. Binomial multiple logistic regression analyses were performed separately for each sleep duration, the sleep period, the TIB, and the 24 h TIB, which were controlled for chronotype (M-type vs. N-, and E-types), sex (girls vs. boys), age (5–6-year-olds vs. 4-year-olds), and childcare programs (kindergarten vs. childcare centers); those corresponded to Models 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Both unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios (ORs) with 95 % confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated. Computations were based on the pairwise deletion of missing data. The statistical significance level was set at p < 0.05 (two-tailed). All analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 20.0.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Distributions of sleep duration [sleep period (a), time in bed (TIB) (b), and TIB and naptime (24 h TIB) (c)], using the Children’s ChronoType Questionnaire (CCTQ), in preschool children aged 4–6 years attending kindergarten or childcare centers (N = 383). Black and white bars represent the number of children grouped by the categories of sleep duration at half-hour intervals from 7.0 to 14.0 h on weekdays and weekends, respectively

Results

No significant association was found between total behavioral problems and sleep period, TIB or 24 h TIB. However, total behavioral problems were significantly associated with chronotype; using M-type as a reference, adjusted ORs for N-type and E-type were 2.06 and 3.72 in Model 1, 2.09 and 3.89 in Model 2, and 1.89 and 3.61 in Model 3 (Table 1).

Table 1 Associations between sleep durations and total behavioral problems, adjusted for chronotype and other background factors (Models 1, 2, and 3), in Japanese preschool children aged 4–6 years attending kindergarten or childcare centers (N = 383)

Discussion

This study showed that whilst sleep period, TIB, and 24 h TIB are not associated with total behavioral problems in children aged 4–6 years, chronotype does have an impact on children’s behavior. That is, a specific cut-off value of sleep duration that could differentiate children with behavioral problems from those without was not identified. N- and E-types were significantly associated with total behavioral problems.

These results are in contrast with those reported in previous studies conducted on children under 6 years of age, which suggested an association between short sleep duration and hyperactivity/impulsivity, attention problems, internalizing behavioral problems, and total psychiatric symptoms [11, 12]. However, none of these studies took into account chronotype or circadian rhythmicity in examining the aforementioned associations.

The findings obtained in our study are, however, in line with those of other studies; Komada et al. [13] reported that attention and aggressive behavioral problems were not associated with sleep duration, but rather with bedtime irregularity in 4–5-year-old Japanese nursery school children. In Komada’s study, parent-reported sleep duration was divided into short and long sleep groups by the median value (9.3 h), and behavioral problems were assessed using the Children’s Behavioral Check List (CBCL). van der Heijden et al. [14] studied school-aged children and reported that eveningness was related to the increase in total behavioral problems and attention problems; however, sleep duration hardly affected the behavioral problems. In their study, sleep duration was measured using a sleep diary. Chronotype and behavioral problems were assessed using the CCTQ and the CBCL, respectively.

According to Bax, it is rare for a child under 5 years of age, unless he or she has been forcibly kept up and kept awake, to suffer from a shortage of sleep [15]. In the present study, the median (IQR) of 24 h TIB was 10.50 (1.00) h both for weekdays and weekends, although nocturnal sleep durations were shorter on weekdays than weekends [3]. In fact, nocturnal sleep durations lengthened from weekdays to weekends without change in sleep latency [10.2 (10.2)] min: from 9.58 (1.17) to 9.83 (1.17) h for sleep period, and from 10.00 (1.08) to 10.25 (1.17) h for TIB [3]. Taking these findings together, it suggests that daytime napping compensates for an accumulated weekday sleep shortage in preschool children aged 4–6 years. As such, daytime napping as compensation for lesser total sleep duration may provide an explanation for the absence of a significant association between sleep duration and behavioral problems among these preschool children. However, the benefits and adverse effects of a daytime nap are controversial. For example, Fukuda and Asaoka [16] suggest that the delayed bedtime of nursery school children caused by a daytime nap may give rise to such problems as unwillingness to go to school and poor morning moods during their elementary school days.

Matsuoka et al. conducted a school-based study to compare sleep/wake patterns and sleep problems between children aged 6–12 years with and without developmental disorders. The former were more likely than the latter to have sleep problems including parasomnias and disordered breathing; however, there was no significant difference in sleep start time, sleep end time, and sleep duration between the two groups [17]. These findings suggest that children with developmental disorders have delayed development of sleep architecture, or a combination of abnormal neuromuscular control and anatomical upper airway narrowing, whereas they show no difference in the regulation of sleep–wake timing and sleep duration compared to children without developmental disorders. Further research on younger children to examine whether a significant difference in sleep/wake patterns exists between developmentally delayed and developmentally normal children will provide us with insight into developmental sleep–wake control mechanisms.

This study had a few limitations. First, the participants, who all lived in a certain area, were not necessarily representative of preschool children attending kindergarten or childcare centers across the whole of Japan. Second, the sample size was small. Therefore, the present study did not analyze a relationship between sleep duration and each subscale of behavioral problems—emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer problems, and prosocial behavioral problems—because the reliable value of OR with a 95 % CI for each behavioral problem was not obtained. Therefore, the study did not consider more than two categories of sleep durations. Third, the cross-sectional design used in this study could not establish a cause-effect relationship.

In conclusion, the present study reveals that there is substantial individual variation in the amount of sleep among preschool children aged 4–6 years attending kindergarten or childcare centers. It suggests that their behavioral problems are not significantly associated with sleep duration, but rather associated with their chronotype, if the child’s actual sleep duration meets his or her biological sleep need. Further research is warranted to explore optimal sleep, in terms of its amount, timing, regularity, and circadian preference, for improving developmental behavioral function in young children.