Introduction

Behavioral economics, the field resulting from the combining of economics with important aspects of psychology, began to be applied to addiction in 1990 [1]. Approximately a decade later, neuroeconomics [2], which incorporated neuroscience, economics, and aspects of decision science, developed and was later applied to addiction [3]. The congress of behavioral economics and neuroeconomics has contributed substantially to the field of addiction. Among those contributions include a new conceptual understanding of addiction [4, 3, 5] identifying neural substrates contributing to addictive behaviors [6], clarifying the relationship between measures of impulsivity and executive function [7], evolving the measurement of those dual systems, providing novel methods for the analysis of abuse liability of substances [8, 9], and expanding the range of phenomena to which it has been applied [10, 9].

A more recent contribution of behavioral economic and neuroeconomic approaches is an important relatively new conceptual understanding of addiction referred to as reinforcer pathology [11] [12•]. Reinforcer pathology refers to the combined effects of (1) high valuation for immediate drug reinforcers and (2) an excessive preference for immediate acquisition of a reinforcer in spite of negative long-term consequences. The combination of these two factors may be important contributors to the addictive process. Thus, treatments and interventions that can reduce either of these effects may be important for diminishing addictive behaviors and may contribute to our understanding of therapeutic efficacy.

As the behavioral economics and neuroeconomics of addiction have matured, these fields have begun to focus their attention on developing interventions that can alter and change either of these two processes of reinforcer pathology or the addictive behavior itself. Moreover, the conceptual understanding of behavioral economics and neuroeconomics has led to reinterpreting interventions and treatments into theoretically coherent categories that contribute to a comprehensive approach to addiction. The goal of this paper is to elucidate the conceptual approach of the behavioral economics and neuroeconomics of addictive behaviors by identifying and clarifying how existing and novel interventions can be integrated into a conceptually coherent perspective.

The competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory of reinforcer pathology

The competing neurobehavioral decision systems (CNDS) approach is a theory developed within neuroeconomics that specifies neural underpinnings of reinforcer pathology, as well as unique insights into the etiology and treatment of addiction [3] [7] [13•] [14]. The CNDS theory is a dual system approach that assumes that behavior is the product of two competing neural systems: (1) the reward-driven impulsive system, which comprises limbic and paralimbic brain regions (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex and nucleus accumbens), and (2) the evolutionarily newer executive system, which governs self-regulatory processes (e.g., planning and behavioral inhibition) and comprises prefrontal and parietal brain regions (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; dlPFC). A variety of dual decision systems have been proposed to account for normative and non-normative decision-making (e.g., [15, 16]), although the CNDS theory is the only system focusing on addiction. In the CNDS theory, interactions between decision systems reflect a neuroeconomic understanding of the two components of reinforcer pathology, wherein addiction is the product of a hyperactive impulsive system and/or hypoactive executive system.

The interaction between impulsive and executive systems and their corresponding effects on behavioral control are represented in Fig. 1. The x- and y-axes depict the relative strength of each system, with interacting system strengths in this two-dimensional space ranging from dominant control by the impulsive system and high risk for reinforcer pathology (black) to dominant control by the executive system and low risk for reinforcer pathology (white). When systems are in balance (diagonal line), an individual may occasionally seek short-term, hedonic satisfaction associated with the impulsive system (e.g., overeating) but also engages in long-term, self-controlled pursuits (e.g., saving for retirement). In contrast, the behavior of individuals falling at the extreme ends of the color gradient is almost exclusively controlled by either short- or long-term sources of reinforcementFootnote 1 [17, 18].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Behavioral control in the competing neurobehavioral decision systems theory. Combined strengths of the impulsive and executive systems may fall at any coordinate in this two-dimensional space, with behavior controlled by relative contributions of each system (see color gradient). The diagonal line represents regulatory balance between impulsive and executive systems. In this illustration, interacting system strengths falling at coordinates a and b produce identical levels of impulsive behavior, with both producing more impulsive behavior than those at coordinate c.

One component of reinforcer pathology as it relates to addiction is excessive valuation of drug reinforcement [19], which may be measured directly through the assessment of demand elasticity for drugs of abuse (i.e., sensitivity of drug consumption to increases in price; see [20]; [21•]). In this view, estimates of demand elasticity provide straightforward measures of value because they reflect the extent to which an individual will defend consumption of a given drug across increasing price. In recent years, several studies have documented robust associations between drug demand and either addiction severity (e.g., [22], [23] [24] [25]) or drug treatment failure [26]. From the perspective of the CNDS theory, excessive valuation of drug reinforcement is a product of relatively greater control of the impulsive over the executive decision systems.

The second component of reinforcer pathology is persistent drug use despite negative, long-term consequences [19]. Such disregard for the delayed outcomes associated with drug use suggests a rapid devaluation of future events. Thus, one measure of the relative activation of the impulsive and executive systems is delay discounting or the devaluation of reinforcement with increasing delay. Accumulating evidence demonstrates that high discounting rates are cross-sectionally and longitudinally associated with addiction (e.g., [27]; [28]; for review and meta-analysis, see [29, 30]). Such findings are consistent with the view that delay discounting plays an etiological role in addiction, as rapid devaluation of the delayed reinforcement associated with drug abstinence (e.g., long-term psychological and physical health) likely increases the reinforcing efficacy of immediate drug reinforcement. Consistent with the CNDS theory, high rates of delay discounting may be the product of relatively greater control of the impulsive over the executive decision system (e.g., see points A and B in Fig. 1). This possibility is further supported by reports in which rates of delay discounting are correlated with differential age- or species-dependent development of the impulsive and executive systems (e.g., [13•]; [31] [32]; [33]; [34]).

In the following sections, we provide overviews of interventions emerging from behavioral economic and neuroeconomic research on drug demand and delay discounting.

Drug demand

Reinforcer pathology is characterized, in part, by excessive valuation of select commodities. Excessive valuation of drugs is often measured by behavioral economic analyses of demand consisting of two interrelated concepts: (1) elasticity of demand, which is the sensitivity of consumption to price increases, and (2) intensity of demand, which is the level of consumption at near zero cost. Therapeutic opportunities targeting drug demand can shift consumption along an individual’s curve (i.e., increasing the price of the drug), shift the entire demand curve to a lower level (i.e., reducing the valuation of the drug across all prices), and/or shift the elasticity of the demand curve (i.e., increasing sensitivity to price) [35] [36].

Single commodity demand

When considering elasticity, the primary predictor of consumption is unit price or the cost-benefit ratio of the commodity cost to the magnitude of benefit of the reward. Unit price stipulates that the same consumption should be achieved at the same unit price regardless of the price’s constituents; that is, consumption is the same whether the unit price of 10 is achieved by 10 responses for 1 unit of drug or 100 responses for 10 units of drug [1, 37]. Thus, consumption of a substance can be decreased by increasing the unit price to obtain it (e.g., [38, 39, 39]).

Contingency management

Contingency management increases the price of substance use by adding an immediate incentive for abstinence that will be forfeited by drug consumption. This treatment delivers incentives such as money, vouchers, or prizes contingent on drug abstinence and is effective in stimulant, opioid, marijuana, and tobacco use (see [40] for meta-analysis). Contingency management has been used to encourage other healthy behaviors such as weight loss and medication adherence. This treatment uses a specialized form of price referred to as opportunity cost [41]. For example, if an individual has the choice between using an illicit drug now or receiving $3 at their next appointment and chooses to use the illicit drug, the opportunity cost of using the drug is $3. These incentives are then offered over repeated appointments and can encourage a reduction in consumption that allows for contact with alternative prosocial, therapeutic reinforcers that may later decrease overall drug valuation. In this way, contingency management decreases consumption by increasing the unit price of drug use, decreasing the relative control of the impulsive decision system.Footnote 2

Cost-focused motivational interventions

Another way to increase the opportunity cost of substance consumption is cost-focused motivational intervention. These interventions combine behavioral economic enhancements and traditional treatments to alter drug consumption by increasing awareness and salience of delayed consequences of substance use [42]. These cost-focused motivational interventions emphasize the opportunity costs associated with substance use, such as lost productivity and health (e.g., [43]). However, unlike contingency management, these interventions do not directly increase cost; rather, they increase salience of natural consequences. These treatments have been tested in problematic alcohol and marijuana use in early adulthood and have been shown to reduce drug use, though not uniformly, while increasing the relative value of prosocial reinforcers [44, 45]. As such, cost-focused motivational interventions shift the relative control of the competing decision systems in favor of the executive.

Transcranial magnetic stimulation

A neuroeconomic intervention may effectively reduce the overall demand curve by reducing the reward valuation across prices. For example, decreased cigarette consumption [46] [47] and cocaine use [48] following repeated administration of a non-invasive neurotherapeutic brain stimulation technique, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), suggests decreased reward valuation. This decrease in consumption following administration of repetitive TMS (rTMS) to the dlPFC is consistent with increasing the relative control of the executive decision system of the CNDS. What remains to be determined is whether this intervention functions as a shift in the demand curve or, rather, increases unit price indirectly (see below for a discussion of the effects of TMS on delay discounting, the second reinforcer pathology subtype).

Cross-price drug demand

Real-life choices often require us to choose between more than one commodity at the same time. The economic relationship between multiple commodities at varying prices is called cross-price demand elasticity. If the price of one commodity is increased and consumption of a second increases, the second commodity is a substitute; if consumption of the second decreases, it is a complement; and if it stays the same, it is an independent. Importantly, substitutes, complements, and independents exist on a continuum where substitutes and complements make up the two extremes and independents fall in the middle [49]. For example, both denicotinized cigarettes and gum function as substitutes as the price of cigarettes increases [50]; however, money functions as an independent [51]. Experimental studies of human behavior demonstrate how consumption of different commodities, such as addictive substances, is altered when price changes. Thus, cross-price demand interactions inform how purchase patterns will change in response to price manipulations.

Community reinforcement approach

Experimental evaluation of cross-price demand has set the stage for therapeutic interventions that are based on a tradeoff between substance use and alternative forms of reinforcement. The community reinforcement approach, a treatment where prosocial activities are encouraged, while substance use is discouraged, leverages cross-commodity substitution to combat substance use disorders. Given that excessive valuation of substances result, in part, from the paucity of alternatively available reinforcers by facilitating employment, community, recreation, and family opportunities, new behaviors can compete with substance use as viable reinforcers. The treatment forces a tradeoff between interdependent choices of substance use and prosocial reinforcers. For example, a patient can choose between incompatible behaviors such as drinking at the bar or spending time playing in a community sports league. In this way, the community reinforcement approach decreases relative impulsive decision system control by discouraging drug use while simultaneously increasing relative executive decision system control by nurturing prosocial behaviors. The community reinforcement approach, alone and in conjunction with contingency management, outperforms comparator treatments including 12-step and standard outpatient treatment [52] [53] [54] [55].

Drug agonist therapies

In addition to behavioral interventions, pharmacological interventions seek to provide a substitute for drugs of abuse. Though no FDA-approved medications for the treatment of stimulant dependence exist [56], approved medications for the treatment of opioid, nicotine, and alcohol dependence are available and function as substitutes by decreasing drug demand through both positive and negative reinforcement [12•]. For example, opioid replacement therapies, including methadone [57] and buprenorphine [58], provide alternative, possibly less addictive, and better - controlled alternatives to treat dependence. Though no current research directly investigates the effects of pharmacotherapy on reinforcer demand, the mechanism by which agonist replacement therapies function suggests that demand may be decreased [12•]. That is, these medications may function mechanistically by decreasing relative control of the impulsive decision system to decrease drug demand. When used in conjunction with behavioral interventions, such as contingency management or the community reinforcement approach, drug agonist therapies may be highly efficacious and offer the benefit of providing alternative reinforcers for both the external and internal reinforcing properties of substances [59].

Drug demand summary

In summary, behavioral economic laboratory studies have elucidated the relationship between addictive substances and the excessive demand subtype of reinforcer pathology. Experimental research on demand processes supports interventions including contingency management, cost-focused motivational interventions, TMS, the community reinforcement approach, and agonist replacement therapies. The reinforcer pathology perspective has contributed to the development of some of the therapies reviewed above and in other cases provides a theoretical understanding in order to develop new and employ existing treatments.

Delay discounting

Delay discounting can be used to measure the extent to which an individual has an excessive preference for immediate acquisition of a reinforcer. High rates of discounting measured by choice of smaller, immediate rewards over larger, delayed rewards is associated with substance use, overeating, and excessive gambling (see [19] for review). Excessive preference for immediate rewards may be described as a trans-disease process [60•], wherein hyperactivation of the impulsive system and/or hypoactivation of the executive system act in concert to produce these maladaptive behaviors [3]. While some interventions seek to decrease the relative control of the impulsive decision system, we hypothesize that amplification of the executive decision system will enhance self-control in order to decrease substance use. Here, we suggest potential behavioral therapeutic and neurotherapeutic interventions for increasing relative executive control.

Behavioral treatments

Working memory training

Significant functional overlap occurs in the left dlPFC, a brain region associated with executive function, during both delay discounting and working memory tasks [61] supporting the notion that executive control and self-regulation interact [7]. For example, increases in working memory load significantly increase discounting [62]. Importantly, working memory impairments are present in pathologies, such as substance use [63] [64] [65] [66] [67] [68] [69]. Moreover, individuals who use substances perform worse than matched controls on both gambling and working memory tasks [70]. From the CNDS perspective, improvement in working memory will strengthen executive function and relative control [3]; [65].

Indeed, working memory training has been demonstrated to successfully improve self-regulation in some cases. For example, working memory training reduced delay discounting in stimulant-dependent participants [71] and decreased alcohol intake in alcohol-dependent individuals [72]. While these results support the use of working memory training as an effective adjunct therapy for addiction [73], some evidence suggests that working memory training was ineffective in improving self-regulation among heroin-dependent individuals (see [74]) Thus, further investigation into the mechanism of the therapeutic efficacy of working memory training in addiction is required.

The CNDS theory proposes a mechanism that involves increasing the relative control of the executive decision system to buffer hyperactive impulsive decision-making. Functional activation has been demonstrated consistently with the CNDS theory following working memory training. For example, the left dlPFC activates proportionally with the difficulty of a working memory task in cigarette smokers. Importantly, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have demonstrated increases in prefrontal and parietal region activation [75] and plasticity in frontal cortex [76] in individuals following working memory training. In sum, several studies suggest working memory activates regions associated with the executive decision system and working memory training increases its relative control.

Episodic future thinking

Episodic future thinking is the second behavioral intervention that may improve valuation of temporally distant reinforcers. By vividly describing and imagining specific, plausible, and positive future events for specific time points, individuals can significantly widen their temporal window and enhance the salience of delayed rewards. Episodic future thinking reduced delay discounting and ad libitum food intake in healthy participants [77, 78, 79] and in obese adults and adolescents [80, [10, 81, 82]. Importantly, episodic future thinking also decreases delay discounting in alcohol-dependent participants [83] which suggests that preexperiencing a positive future event can help bring the future closer in time for drug-dependent individuals, thus supporting a potential therapeutic intervention for improving the relative control of the executive decision system.

Paramount for improved relative executive control following episodic future thinking is increased activation of the prefrontal brain regions. During the generation of future events and during a delay discounting task, episodic future thought activates frontal poles (i.e., executive decision system areas; [84] [85] and episodic future thought reduces delay discounting via activation of the medial rostral prefrontal cortex (mrPFC), another prefrontal region within the executive decision system [86]. Thus, increased relative control in the executive decision system can be manipulated by episodic future thinking while also decreasing delay discounting (i.e., excessive immediate preference).

Neurotherapeutic treatments

Transcranial magnetic stimulation

Change in delay discounting has also been investigated with non-behavioral interventions, including TMS. Activating the executive decision system through high-frequency rTMS to the medial prefrontal cortex [87] and a specific type of rTMS, continuous theta burst stimulation, targeting the dlPFC [88], both decreased delay discounting in healthy participants. High frequency stimulation to the left dlPFC also decreased rate of delay discounting for monetary gains in both smokers and non-smokers [89]. Conversely, low-frequency rTMS to the left lateral prefrontal cortex, which inhibits the executive system, increased delay discounting rate [90]. Together, these results suggest that targeting the dlPFC directly manipulates relative control of the executive decision system. Moreover, TMS administration may be the most focused intervention to change the relative control between the executive and impulsive decision systems [91] given the ability to directly manipulate the neurocorrelates of at least one decision system of the CNDS (see above for a discussion of the effects of TMS on drug demand, the first reinforcer pathology subtype).

Nootropic therapies

Excessive preference for the immediate choice can also be altered by pharmacotherapies, most often investigated with stimulant medications, in both preclinical and clinical populations [91] [92] [93]. In clinical populations, modafinil administration decreased delay discounting in alcohol-dependent individuals, while preference did not change in healthy participants [94]. Decreased activation in the vmPFC and simultaneous increased activation in the frontal and parietal cortices following stimulant administration suggests that regulatory balance of the CNDS accompanies decreases in delay discounting [94]. The effects of medications on delay discounting may be a result of increased executive control; however, further neuroimaging research is required to determine the functional neurocorrelates of nootropics as CNDS-targeted interventions.

Rate dependence and the CNDS

Discrepant results between studies investigating the same intervention are often attributed to variance intrinsic to the individual when, in fact, an orderly, lawful relationship with baseline rate of behavior may exist [95] [96]. This relationship is referred to as rate dependence and is a well-known behavioral phenomenon, often demonstrated as an inverse association when responding at low rate increases and responding at high rate decreases after drug administration [97]. Recent investigations have found that rate dependence is a robust and overlooked phenomenon that occurs in approximately 50 % of studies, including those with non-pharmacological interventions [60•, 92].

The first explicit description of a rate-dependent brain activation effect using fMRI used rTMS as an intervention technique. In alcohol-dependent treatment-seeking individuals, 15 sessions of rTMS over a 4-week program changed anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) activation dependent upon baseline ACC activation [98]. This baseline activation level was also predictive of relapse such that individuals with high baseline ACC activation were less likely to relapse than those with low baseline ACC activation. This evidence suggests that identifying individuals that may benefit the most from a particular treatment is dependent on their individual baseline level of activation or excitability. Thus, rate dependence may be an observation of approximating regulatory balance between the two decision systems of the CNDS, wherein increases in low baseline levels of activation of the executive decision system may occur following some intervention producing greater regulatory balance. Conversely, individuals with high baseline levels of executive decision system activation may not change following the same intervention.

Delay discounting summary

In sum, behavioral and neurotherapeutic interventions reduce excessive preference for the immediate choice and demonstrate brain activation consistent with the CNDS theory, in a variety of populations. That is, implementation of these interventions activates and enhances function in brain regions associated with greater relative control of the executive decision system. Future exploration of the association between the treatment efficacy of interventions that change delay discounting and clinical outcomes (e.g., decreased drug use) are necessary to apply these treatments more widely in the clinic. Moreover, rate dependence analyses can identify individuals who may most benefit from particular treatments based on their baseline level of CNDS regulation.

Conclusion

We are currently at the forefront of a new conceptual approach to the science of addiction and its treatment. The application and utility of behavioral economic and neuroeconomics to the problems of drug dependence have become increasingly evolved and the scope of this work is broadening. One important arbiter of the success of this approach will be the extent to which it is able to alter drug use and the concomitant behaviors associated with addiction. In this chapter, we have clarified and outlined the important conceptual components of this new system. That is, we have elucidated the CNDS and reinforcer pathology conceptions and how they are related to each other. More importantly, they provide a theoretical basis for treatment development and a conceptual frame by which to evaluate existing interventions.

Just as some of these behavioral economic processes may be operative in multiple disorders beyond addiction, it may be that some of these treatments discussed above, although diverse in procedure and detail, may also have what we would call trans-disease therapeutic properties. Exploring the treatments that we reviewed here in the context of the CNDS and reinforcer pathology conceptions provides a way to examine the extant knowledge and to highlight future areas of potential investigation. Table 1 depicts the types of interventions discussed here and how they may target components of the CNDS, separated by reinforcer pathology subtype. Consistent with the long-term view of addiction as being primarily an issue of the impulsive decision system, we see that the majority of established treatments fall into the high valuation/impulsive decision system quadrant of Table 1. While, the novel and less well established treatments appear in the excessive immediate preference/executive decision system quadrant. Surprisingly, there are no therapies, as we have categorized them, in the excessive immediate preference/impulsive system quadrant. This absence may suggest an important avenue for future therapeutic development. Conversely, the absence of any intervention in this quadrant may reflect the paucity of opportunity there.

Table 1 Conceptual framework by which therapeutic interventions can be categorized based on reinforcer pathology subtype and the targeted decision system

The treatment of addiction has improved over time, but arguably, we have reached some of the limits of our existing conceptions and treatment approaches. At such moments, it is useful to consider whether new conceptual approaches permit us to view our subject matter in a new light. Whether this new conception will adequately provide new therapeutic approaches that will significantly improve our ability to treat addiction remains to be seen. However, given the strength of the basic laboratory findings, the important translational advancements, and the new treatment options that are being developed, we are optimistic that this approach may hold substantial benefit for the treatment of those afflicted with the challenges of addiction.