Introduction

The past decade has seen a surge in research on the topic of “food addiction” [1]. Although the rewarding nature of food has been recognized for some time [2], it is only recently that an empirical measure of food addiction was developed [3]. Sophisticated neuroimaging techniques and behavioral paradigms have revealed overlap in responses to food and addictive drugs, and studies in both humans and animals have identified neural and behavioral parallels between compulsive eating and substance use disorders. Despite these advances, however, the idea that people can be addicted to food items in the same way as they can to drugs of abuse remains controversial (e.g., [4]). The broad scope of the current paper is to summarize the state of this research field, discussing what is currently known about food addiction. The first section of our review defines food addiction, describing its classification, prevalence, and commonly associated characteristics. We then discuss research findings that support the notion of food addiction, describing first the similarities between food and drugs of abuse, and then parallels between overeating and substance use disorders. In the third section, we discuss studies on the neurobiology of food addiction. Finally, we discuss the validity and usefulness of the food addiction model of overeating.

What Is Food Addiction?

Classification

The idea that food has addictive properties is not new [2]. Indeed, the first use of the word “addiction” by one of the earliest addiction journals (the Journal of Inebriety) in 1890 was in reference to chocolate [5]. The year 1960 saw the introduction of “Overeaters Anonymous”—a self-help group based on an addiction model of overeating, followed two decades later by the “Foodaholics Group Treatment Program” [6]. Although the addictive potential of food was being discussed in the late nineteenth century [2], it was only in 2009 that a formal measure of food addiction was introduced [3]. The Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS; [3]) is a self-report measure based on the Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders’ (DSM) criteria for diagnosing substance-related and addictive disorders [7, 8]. The original version of the YFAS was based on symptoms of substance use disorders listed in the DSM-IV, and assessed seven symptoms: (1) food taken in a larger amount or for a longer period than intended; (2) persistent desire or repeated unsuccessful attempts to reduce or stop intake of certain foods; (3) Much time/activity to obtain, use, recover from eating certain foods; (4) important social, occupational, or recreational activities given up or reduced as a result of symptoms; (5) continued intake of the food(s) despite knowledge of adverse consequences; (6) tolerance (requiring increasingly more of a food to obtain the desired effects, or a markedly reduced effect of eating the same amount); and (7) withdrawal (physical or psychological symptoms when the individual reduces or stops intake of the food). In line with the DSM’s criteria for diagnosing substance use disorders, food addiction is considered present if three or more symptoms are endorsed and the respondent reports distress or impairment associated with their symptoms (assessed using additional items on the YFAS). Responses on the YFAS are also used to calculate a continuous symptom count (0–11), or to classify the level of food addiction as “mild,” “moderate,” or “severe” based on the number of symptoms endorsed [9]. As discussed later in this review, symptom count is sometimes used to define “low” and “high” food addiction in research participants who do not necessarily meet the criteria for food addiction.

To reflect updated criteria for substance use disorders in the DSM-5 [8], the YFAS 2.0 was introduced in 2016 [9]. The main change in the YFAS 2.0 was the addition of four additional symptoms: (8) continued use despite social or interpersonal problems; (9) failure to fulfill major role obligations; (10) intake of certain foods in physically hazardous situations; and (11) craving or strong desire/urge to eat certain foods (see Table 1 for a comparison of different versions of the YFAS). The YFAS has been translated into several different languages (e.g., [10,11,12]), and there are modified (shortened) versions of both the original [13] and version 2.0 of the YFAS [14]. In addition, Gearhardt and colleagues [15] adapted the content and reading level of the original YFAS items to create the YFAS for children (YFAS-C). A side-by-side comparison of the items on each version of the YFAS can be seen in Table 1.

Table 1 Table compares the items on four versions of the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS): the original YFAS (labeled here as YFAS 1.0), the YFAS 2.0 (updated to reflect changes in the fifth edition of the DSM), the modified YFAS (mYFAS) 2.0, and the YFAS for children (YFAS-C). Note that the YFAS-C was adapted from the original YFAS

Prevalence

The prevalence of food addiction is estimated to be around 10% in individuals who fall into the “normal” body mass index (BMI) category, and 25% in individuals classified as overweight or obese [16]. Studies of bariatric surgery candidates, who typically have a BMI in the obese range, have found the prevalence of food addiction to be around 15% [17,18,19,20], with the exception of one study that found a prevalence rate of 25% [21]. Food addiction appears to be significantly more prevalent in females than males; however, this could reflect an over-representation of females in studies using the YFAS [16]. Interestingly, studies so far suggest that food addiction is more common in individuals aged over 35 years compared to younger individuals [16], although studies should be conducted to examine age differences systematically.

The prevalence of food addiction is increased in individuals who meet diagnostic criteria for bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder (BED; [16]). BED is characterized by recurrent binge eating episodes, during which the individual eats more than would be eaten by most people over the same time period, while experiencing a sense of loss of control. Bulimia nervosa is also characterized by binge eating episodes, but individuals also engage in compensatory behaviors such as purging or excessive exercise [8]. According to the current literature, the prevalence of food addiction is around 40–60% in individuals with BED [22,23,24,25], and 84–100% in those with bulimia nervosa [24, 26,27,28].

Associated Clinical and Behavioral Characteristics

The overlap and high comorbidity of food addiction and BED has led some to query whether food addiction represents a subtype of BED, rather than being a distinct disorder [1]. Indeed, the concepts of food addiction, BED, and obesity have been conflated by researchers in the past [1, 29]. Despite their similarities, however, several studies have shown that food addiction and BED do not always co-occur [22, 30, 31]; for example, Davis et al. [30] found that around half of those with BED do not meet the criteria for food addiction, and around 30% of those with food addiction do not meet criteria for BED. Likewise, not all individuals who meet criteria for food addiction are overweight or obese [16, 32].

It is important from a treatment standpoint to distinguish food addiction from BED and obesity, because food addiction is associated with heightened psychopathology and poorer treatment outcomes compared to BED and/or obesity alone. Individuals with both BED and food addiction score higher on measures of depression and emotion dysregulation, and are lower in self-esteem, than those with BED alone [23]. Similar to substance use disorders, food addiction is associated with heightened impulsivity (e.g., [30]), and is significantly comorbid with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; [30, 33]). Food addiction is associated with greater eating psychopathology, such as emotional and external eating, binge-eating, and nighttime eating syndrome [2, 3, 19, 23, 24, 30, 34]. Food cravings are consistently higher in individuals who meet the criteria for food addiction [30, 35, 36]. Finally, food addiction is associated with higher BMI, higher cholesterol, a history of smoking, and lower physical activity [13]. More generally, food addiction in treatment-seeking individuals is associated with a significantly lower health-related and psychosocial quality of life [17, 37].

Some studies have found food addiction in treatment-seeking individuals to be associated with less weight loss following treatment [38,39,40], although Koball and colleagues [19] found that food addiction was not predictive of weight loss, rehospitalization, or follow-up attendance in a sample of patients who underwent bariatric surgery. Together, these behavioral and clinical outcome characteristics suggest that food addiction is an important factor for consideration in treatment-seeking populations, but further research is needed to examine other factors that could explain the discrepancies between studies.

Support for the Food Addiction Model

Food as an Addictive Substance

Foods that are high in sugar or fat tend to be more palatable than others [41], leading to these foods being commonly overconsumed in both healthy individuals [42] and those with food addiction [21]. The observation that an animal will expend effort, and even endure pain [43, 44], to gain access to a palatable food demonstrates the inherent reward value of food [45]. Indeed, the most commonly craved food in humans is chocolate (e.g., [46, 47])—a food high in both fat and sugar [48]. Animals exposed to a high-sugar diet increase their intake of these foods after only a short period of exposure [49]. In addition, chocolate itself induces a conditioned and persistent preference for the location where the food was delivered, in a similar way to drugs of abuse (e.g., [50]). In some cases, motivation for food surpasses motivation for other substances commonly thought of as “addictive;” Lenoir and colleagues [51] found that rats preferred heavily saccharin-sweetened water over cocaine, regardless of cocaine dose, and even in animals that had been previously conditioned to prefer cocaine over the sweetened water. These results suggest that intense sweetness, or supranormal stimulation of sweet receptors, can be more rewarding than drugs of abuse. These findings also have implications for the highly sugar-enriched food environment in modern society.

Neurophysiological and neuroimaging studies demonstrate that palatable food intake can elicit a neural response similar to that associated with addictive drugs such as cocaine. Specifically, food and drug rewards are associated with striatal dopamine release in both humans [52] and animals (e.g., [53]). In humans, this response is positively associated with the subjective experience of hunger and food enjoyment [52, 54]. Similarly, frontostriatal dopamine release relates to craving in cocaine use disorder [55]. A growing body of neuroimaging work now demonstrates substantial overlap between palatable foods and addictive substances, with common patterns of brain activation associated with exposure to food and drug cues [56, 57]. This overlap could reflect a shared mechanism by which individuals develop dependence on palatable food or drugs.

Prolonged exposure to high-fat or high-sugar foods is also associated with neural changes similar to those associated with chronic substance use. In rats, the expression of striatal dopamine receptors is affected by long-term intake of a high-fat [58], high-sugar [49], or “cafeteria” style [43] diet. Similar alterations in dopamine receptor availability have been observed in substance use disorders [59]. Although the direct relationship between dietary intake and long-term alterations in dopamine has not been examined in humans, alterations in dopamine receptor availability have been found in individuals who are obese or overweight [60, 61] and those who engage in binge eating [62]. Preclinical studies are beginning to provide mechanistic insights: in rats, exposure to high-fat foods combined with food deprivation increases the expression of fumarate hydratase, ATP synthase subunit alpha (ATP5a1), and transketolase [63]—proteins that have been identified as possible biomarkers of vulnerability to cocaine addiction [64]. Thus, both the acute and long-term effects of exposure to certain food types bear resemblance to those associated with drug use, supporting the idea that food could act as an addictive substance.

Addictive Behavior in Animals Exposed to High-Fat and High-Sugar Foods

In further support of the food addiction model, preclinical studies show that animals exposed to high-sugar, high-fat, and other highly palatable, high-energy diets display some of the behavioral characteristics associated with substance use disorders (for a review, see [65]). These behaviors map onto many of the clinical criteria for substance use disorder. Rats given restricted access to a sweet or high-fat food persist in pressing a lever associated with reward for a longer period (when the reward was no longer being delivered) than animals given no or unrestricted access to the food [66, 67], exhibiting attenuated extinction learning. The extended and perseverative responding to obtain the rewarding food in these animals could be analogous to “a great deal of time spent obtaining, using, or recovering from substance use” in humans—one of the core DSM-5 criteria for substance use disorders [8, 65]. Demonstrating continued use despite negative consequences (as well as use in a physically hazardous environment), Johnson and Kenny [43] found that rats exposed to the cafeteria diet—a varied diet comprising high-fat and high-sugar foods—continued to access the food when doing so was associated with a conditioned foot shock (see also [44]). In contrast, animals with no or restricted access to this diet reduced their food intake when the foot shock was introduced [43]. In another study, animals continued to seek the food when it had been altered to taste unpleasant [68].

Signs of withdrawal and tolerance have also been observed in animals exposed to high-fat or -sugar diets. Animals given intermittent access to sugar and chow exhibit withdrawal symptoms, such as anxiety, aggression, and teeth-chattering, when access to sugar is subsequently removed (for a review, see [1]). Suggestive of tolerance, animals given either restricted or unrestricted access to high-sugar foods progressively increase their intake after only a short period of exposure [49]. A possible physiological marker of tolerance was reported by Johnson and Kenny [43], who found that animals given extended access to the cafeteria diet exhibit decreased sensitivity to reward. Reward sensitivity was assessed using the brain stimulation reward paradigm, in which reward threshold is reflected in the intensity of electrical stimulation required to elicit self-stimulation in an animal. Extended access (18–23 h daily for 40 days) to the cafeteria diet was associated with weight gain and a progressive increase in reward threshold compared to animals given restricted (1 h per day) or no access to the cafeteria diet [43].

Addictive Behavior in Humans Who Exhibit Compulsive-Eating Tendencies

In humans, behaviors resembling characteristics of substance use and addictive disorders are noted in some individuals who are overweight and those who report compulsive eating. People often eat to excess despite the known negative consequences (e.g., weight gain, metabolic syndrome), and cravings are reported by both healthy individuals (e.g., [69]) and those with eating pathologies ([35, 70]. Eating can also preclude social, occupational, and/or recreational activities: according to a study by Lent and Swencionis [71], 60% of bariatric patients report that they sometimes forego other activities in favor of eating. The same study found that a significant amount of variance in social isolation was accounted for by scores on a putative measure of “addictive personality” (comprising items from the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire) that were previously found to discriminate individuals with a substance use disorder from those without [72], possibly suggesting that social activities were given up. The progression of symptoms in bulimia nervosa could also point to aspects of tolerance in humans; tentative evidence suggests that longer duration of the illness is associated with increased frequency, duration, and severity of binge eating episodes (data cited in [3]). Finally, the presence of self-help groups akin to Alcoholics Anonymous to help people moderate their eating [73] demonstrates that many people eat larger amounts or for longer periods than intended, and engage in (often unsuccessful) attempts to control their food intake. Indeed, the presence of these grass-roots mutual support organizations that adapt addiction-related perspectives reveals the extent to which individuals in the community resonate with the parallels between food addiction and drug addiction.

Another line of evidence in support of the food addiction model is that individuals who engage in pathological overeating display some of the same characteristics as individuals with substance use disorders. Specifically, individuals with BED show higher levels of impulsivity, anxiety, and depression compared to individuals without eating or addictive disorders (for a review, see [74]). Importantly, impulsivity is increasingly understood as a multidimensional construct (e.g., [75]) and food addiction has exhibited particularly notable associations with two forms of impulsivity: negative urgency (e.g., [76, 77]) and delay discounting [78]. In the first case, negative urgency refers to a tendency to act out in response to negative affect and it is an impulsive personality trait that has been robustly associated with substance use disorders in numerous previous studies (for meta-analyses, see [79,80,81]). In the second case, delay discounting is a behavioral economic index of impulsivity that reflects how much a person values smaller immediate rewards relative to larger delayed rewards. Steep discounting of future rewards has been associated with substance use disorder [82, 83], ADHD [84], and obesity [85] in recent meta-analyses, and, interestingly, food addiction symptom score mediated the link between delay discounting and obesity in a recent study [78].

Finally, environmental antecedents of consumption appear to be the same for food addiction and drug addiction. In the same way that individuals with substance use disorders display attentional biases towards drug-relevant stimuli [86], individuals with BED show an attentional bias towards food (e.g., [87]). In addition, high-palatability food cues elicit craving and other appetitive responses (e.g., [88]), akin to drug cues [89]. Substance use disorders and BED also show similar epidemiological features: chronic or early life stress are strongly associated with the development of both BED [90] and substance use disorders [91], and acute stress is a common antecedent of binge eating episodes and substance use [92, 93]. This shared risk factor could suggest a common mechanism contributing to the development of BED and substance use disorders whereby the experience of early/chronic stress or trauma can alter subsequent reward responses in these individuals.

Neurobiology of Food Addiction in Humans

To date, few studies have examined the relationship between neural activity and food addiction symptoms as measured by the YFAS; however, those that have support the notion of distinct characteristics associated with food addiction. Gearhardt and colleagues [94] found that when anticipating receiving a palatable milkshake, food addiction severity was positively related with left hemisphere activity in the medial orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and amygdala. Individuals with high YFAS scores (three or more symptoms) also showed greater milkshake anticipatory activity in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)—a region implicated in inhibitory control and reward expectancy [95]. Heightened amygdala activity during food cue exposure is associated with higher self-rated hunger and desire to eat [96], while medial OFC and ACC activity is associated with higher trait reward sensitivity [97]. These findings point to the particular importance of anticipatory processing and food reward expectations in food addiction. Heightened responsivity of these regions during anticipation of palatable food intake could thus contribute to the difficulty in controlling food intake that is characteristic of food addiction. Neural responses during actual receipt of the milkshake was not associated with YFAS scores, although individuals who endorsed three or more food addiction symptoms showed lower activity in the left lateral OFC compared to individuals who endorsed one or no symptoms [94]. Interestingly, increased activity in the lateral OFC was associated with increasing satiety in a study of chocolate consumption in healthy participants [98], so it is possible that the relatively low lateral OFC activity during milkshake receipt in individuals with food addiction reflects a weaker satiety response.

There is also emerging evidence for altered neural activity in food addiction, independent of cue reactivity. Similar to Gearhardt and colleagues [94], de Ridder and colleagues [99] reported a positive correlation between YFAS scores and activity in the ACC, but this time in a study of resting-state brain activity. In their study, rostral ACC gamma band activity was positively associated with self-rated hunger in individuals with high YFAS scores (6.8 symptoms on average). This is also one of the first studies to include a control comparison group with substance use disorder and found that self-rated craving also related to rostral ACC gamma-band activity in individuals with alcohol use disorder. By contrast, individuals with obesity who had low YFAS scores (3.5 symptoms on average) showed no correlation between hunger ratings and gamma-band activity in the rACC, but a negative correlation between hunger and beta-band activity in the same area. Conjunction analyses showed common areas of activity for individuals with high YFAS scores and those with alcohol use disorder, but no significant overlap between the low YFAS and alcohol use disorder group. These findings suggest common resting-state activity in individuals with higher food addiction symptoms and those with alcohol dependence with similar relationships with hunger and drug craving [99].

Food addiction is also associated with greater functional connectivity within frontoparietal brain regions [100], which is similar to what has been observed in individuals with substance use disorders (e.g., [101, 102]). Finally, a neuroimaging study of adolescents with BMIs in the overweight or obese range found no association between neural activation during gustatory cue exposure and YFAS scores [103]. Although the lack of association reported in this study is at odds with the results of the other studies [94, 99, 100], it is important to note that the average YFAS score was lower in Feldstein Ewing and colleagues’ participants compared to previous samples. It is possible that the range of food addiction severity was not large enough to detect neural differences in this sample. It is also possible that neurodevelopmental factors affected the relationship between food addiction and brain activity in this sample of adolescents.

Differences in the dopaminergic system have also been linked to food addiction [35, 36]. Food addiction was positively associated with a genetic marker of heightened dopamine signaling, which also correlated with binge eating, food cravings, and emotional eating [35]. Alterations in dopamine transmission in the reward system have been observed in individuals with BED [62] and those with substance use disorders [59]; these findings support the notion of common mechanisms underlying compulsive behavior in these conditions. Along the same lines, Davis et al. [36] found that individuals who met the criteria for food addiction showed no change in appetite when they received the dopamine agonist methylphenidate, which is typically associated with appetite suppression (e.g., [104, 105]). These findings suggest unique food intake patterns following a pharmacologic challenge as well as alterations in dopamine signaling strength in food addiction—the latter being similarly characteristic in substance use disorders.

Validity and Usefulness of the Food Addiction Model

Although there is clear overlap between neural activity associated with anticipation/intake of addictive substance and palatable food, there are also clear distinctions between food and drugs which bring into question the validity of the food addiction model (e.g., [4]). A complication for the food addiction model, which does not typically apply for drug addiction, is that food is essential for life. It is unclear where the line between use and misuse of certain foods lies, making it difficult to advocate for any treatment involving abstinence or reduction of intake. In defense of the food addiction model, the YFAS asks respondents about their eating behavior with regard to “certain foods,” rather than food in general [3]. Therefore, the fact that food (unlike addictive drugs) is consumed ubiquitously is in some ways irrelevant to the validity of the food addiction model. Indeed, a food addiction model does not propose that compulsive behavior pertains to all foods, but rather those high in fat, sugar, or salt, which pharmacodynamically act like addictive drugs in the brain (i.e., provoking acute increases in dopaminergic and opioidergic release, and other changes in neural activity). If food addiction is considered in terms of dependence on specific food types—for example, chocolate, sweets, fast-food—then distinguishing “use” from “misuse” is more feasible and useful.

With a strong focus on the overlap between neural processing of addictive drugs and palatable food, the relative magnitude of these responses is often overlooked; addictive drugs are typically more potent in evoking reward-related activity compared to food rewards [106, 107]. This does not preclude the possibility that individuals can become addicted to food in a similar way to drugs of abuse, but could lead to a different presentation of symptoms in each. For example, it is possible that symptoms such as withdrawal and tolerance are present but much less discernible in food addiction compared to drug addiction, simply because the neural responses to food are smaller than those to drugs. If this is the case, we might expect higher reward sensitivity to be associated with stronger signs of withdrawal and tolerance in an individual with food addiction.

Another point of discussion is whether it is meaningful to calculate a continuous symptom score in individuals who do not meet “diagnostic” criteria for food addiction [4]. In some cases, participants have been classified as being “high” or “low” in food addiction symptomology (e.g., [94, 99]), and behavioral or neural variables compared between the two groups. If we assume that food addiction and its classification using the DSM’s criteria for substance use disorders is valid, then it could be argued that the only meaningful comparison is between individuals who meet criteria for food addiction and those who do not. That is, there is no theoretical basis on which to assume fundamental differences between individuals with different sub-clinical levels of food addiction. Furthermore, the number of symptoms present can vary considerably across samples, meaning that high and low food addiction can look very different in different studies. For example, the average symptom count of the “high food addiction” group in one neuroimaging study [94] was closer to the symptom count of the “low food addiction” than the high food addiction group in another study [99]. It is also not always clear how many of the individuals endorse significant impairment or distress associated with their symptoms; since this symptom is necessary to meet criteria for food addiction, it should be at least reported in any study using the term “addiction.” To facilitate comparisons across studies, it would be useful to standardize the definitions of high and low food addiction, or to compare only those who endorse clinically significant symptoms. Given the varying symptoms of food addiction assessed by the YFAS, examining neural or behavioral correlates of specific symptoms might be most valuable in cases where not all symptoms are endorsed.

Behavioral or Chemical Addiction?

Although certain combinations of food nutrients and sensory properties are associated with reward-related neural activity, there has been debate over whether food addiction would be better conceptualized as “eating behaviour,” putting it more akin to behavioral addictions such as gambling disorder [108, 109]. In favor of the “eating addiction” model, Hebebrand and colleagues state that there is little evidence of any one food or food component being addictive, and that individuals who overeat typically eat a wide range of different foods and have diverse food preferences [108]. However, Schulte and colleagues [109] argue that in order to be considered a behavioral addiction, the type of food eaten should have no effect on whether or not addictive-like eating develops—a feature that is refuted by findings that foods high in fat and sugar are most closely associated with addictive-like eating [109]. According to Schulte and colleagues, gambling disorder is characterized by compulsive engagement in a behavior (gambling) that in itself is rewarding. Although money (typically the object of gambling) is a rewarding stimulus, it is the behavior of gambling—with its intermittent rewards and inherent risk of losses—that becomes reinforcing [109].

It is possible that gambling increases the reward value of money [109]; in the same way, food might become more rewarding with certain patterns of intake (e.g., binge eating, stress-induced eating). However, an important difference between food and money is that food is considered a primary reinforcer (i.e., inherently rewarding), whereas money is a secondary reinforcer since it is conditioned to be rewarding (at least in many societies). Compulsive sexual behaviors might be more akin to addictive-like eating, since sex—like food—is considered a primary reinforcer. Indeed, compulsive sexual behavior (also referred to as “hypersexuality,” “sex addiction,” and “excessive sexual drive”) has been compared to substance use and addictive disorders [110]. While more systematic studies are necessary, early findings demonstrate neurobiological similarities across addictions (both substance-based and non-substance-based), hypersexuality, and food addiction (Table 2).

Table 2 Key criteria across substance use disorders, gambling disorder, hypersexuality, and food addiction and neurobiological similarities and differences across the four conditions in cue-reactivity studies, effect of opioid antagonists, and striatal activation and dopaminergic systems

Summary and Future Directions

At this point, the evidence for addictive properties of certain foods and overlap between compulsive eating behavior and substance use disorders is compelling. Preclinical and human studies alike demonstrate that foods high in fat and sugar elicit neural responses that are qualitatively similar to those associated with addictive drugs, suggesting that exposure to these foods could produce a cluster of symptoms similar to those seen in substance use disorders. Neuroimaging and pharmacological studies show that individuals who meet the YFAS criteria for food addiction show similar patterns of resting and task-related brain activity and similar alterations in dopamine transmission as are seen in substance use disorders. Studies of clinical populations suggest that food addiction has detrimental effects on several aspects of physical and mental health, although causal relationships have not yet been established. To better understand the relationship between the core symptoms of food addiction and other health outcomes, longitudinal studies should be conducted to examine the development and course of food addiction in clinical and non-clinical populations. Given discrepant findings on the clinical implications of food addiction, another priority for this research area is to more closely examine the influence of specific symptoms of food addiction, and their interaction with other behavioral and physiological variables, on treatment outcomes.