Introduction

The process of sexual identity development in sexual minority people (e.g., gay, lesbian, bisexual, pansexual) is complex and entails the experience of cognitive incongruence between expected heterosexuality and the experience of same-sex sexual and romantic feelings (Cass, 1979). This process is often associated with poorer psychological and physical outcomes, such that people who struggle with their sexual identity are at higher risk of developing depressive symptoms later on (Everett, 2015). When people are able to resolve such incongruity, they are more likely to accept their sexual identity and—to some extent—disclose it to others (D’Augelli, 1994; Manning, 2015; Rosario et al., 2001).

The coming out process can have several psychological benefits, including increased self-esteem and sense of belonging, decreased minority stress and psychological distress, and better overall mental health (e.g., Bybee et al., 2009; Meyer, 2013; Morris, 2001; Rosario et al., 2001). However, it can also be associated with negative psychological and physical outcomes, especially when people experience negative reactions or lack of support from their close social network (e.g., Goldbach et al., 2014; Ryan et al., 2015). For example, Legate et al. (2011) showed that sexual minority people were less open about their sexual identity and experienced more anger and less self-esteem in contexts with low autonomy support (e.g., religious community), when compared to contexts with high autonomy support (e.g., friends). There is also evidence that sexual minority people who go through the coming out process in heteronormative socio-cultural contexts experience greater isolation, stigmatization, and violence (Martin & Hetrick, 1988). For example, Wagner et al. (2013) found that sexual minority men who experienced social hostility (e.g., verbal harassment) were less comfortable with their identity, and more likely to develop coping mechanisms (e.g., social avoidance) to limit their exposure to stigma. Arguably, sexual minority people in heteronormative societies integrate negative societal views about sexuality as part of their sexual identity (Berg et al., 2016) and experience greater internalized homonegativity (Herek, 2004). In turn, internalized homonegativity emphasizes negative evaluations and stereotypes about gender. For example, López-Sáez et al. (2020) found that gay men who expressed greater discomfort with their sexuality also reported greater ambivalent sexism. Equally important, internalized homonegativity has been construed as a proximal stressor associated with poorer psychological health and well-being (e.g., Meyer, 2013). Hence, in this study, we examined if heteronormative beliefs—resulting from socially imposed heteronormativity—were associated with sexual identity stigma in sexual minority men and if this association was explained by their internalized homonegativity. We also explored the extent to which these variables were associated with the disclosure of sexual identity to friends and family. To have a more comprehensive understanding of these phenomena, we compared two socio-cultural contexts—Portugal and Turkey—that have distinct social and legal perspectives on same-sex relationships (e.g., Carrol & Mendos, 2017; Costa, 2021), different levels of internalized homophobia (Berg et al., 2013) and different attitudes toward homosexuality and same-sex relationships (Costa et al., 2018; O’Neil & Çarkoğlu, 2020; Pew Research Center, 2020).

Heteronormativity and the Coming Out Process

Heteronormativity refers to a political, social, philosophical, and economic regime that imposes specific gender roles, sexual behaviors, and sexual orientation (e.g., through laws, religion, education, family) to conform with heterosexuality, and with how men and women should behave (Habarth, 2015; Kitzinger, 2005; Nielsen et al., 2000). This norm conveys the notion that heterosexuality is superior to other sexual identities (Eguchi, 2009; Elia, 2003; Habarth, 2015), rendering sexual minority people at risk for stigmatization and violence. Indeed, these people report being victims of physical and verbal harassment, property crime, and employment or housing discrimination (e.g., Drydakis, 2015; Herek, 2009; Swank et al., 2013). For example, heterosexual people are less likely to recommend gay-sounding men for typically masculine job positions (e.g., leadership; Fasoli et al., 2017). These people are also stigmatized by political structures. For example, Arreola et al. (2015) found higher levels of sexual identity stigma in countries that criminalize same-sex behaviors. In this sense, heteronormativity influences and justifies the assumptions of homonegativity.

Heteronormativity shapes not only the identity and behaviors of heterosexual people but also how sexual minority people construe their sexual identity and coming out. Indeed, the coming out process can be influenced by a myriad of values, including societal norms and public policies, and more particularly individual values and religious beliefs (Mohr & Fassinger, 2003; Shapiro et al., 2010). For example, Gough (2007) showed that gay male athletes were more likely to embrace typically masculine behaviors to avoid disclosing their sexual identity because they feared the negative consequences of coming out in a typically hypermasculine context. Similarly, Gottschalk and Newton (2009) found that sexual minority people living in conservative rural contexts were less likely to disclose their sexual orientation, to avoid stigmatization and prejudice. In contrast, Toomey et al. (2012) found that sexual minority students perceived more safety when their schools engaged in policies to reduce heteronormativity. Similarly, Bauermeister et al. (2010) found that sexual minority people were more comfortable with exploring their sexual identity in contexts that supported non-conforming sexual identities.

Theoretical models focusing on the coming out process define self-acceptance as the process whereby people embrace their non-heterosexual identity as part of the self (e.g., Bilodeau & Renn, 2005; Fassinger & Miller, 1997), and disclosure as the process whereby people openly share their sexual identity with others (e.g., Collins & Miller, 1994; Mohr & Fassinger, 2003). Both processes are interrelated, albeit not simultaneous, and can mutually influence each other (McCarn & Fassinger, 1996). Studies typically examine if and why heteronormativity acts as an external force (e.g., heteronormative contexts) in the coming out process. We focused instead on the psychological internalization of social heteronormativity (Crocker et al., 1998).

Heteronormativity Beliefs and Internalized Homonegativity

People are continuously exposed through institutions (e.g., school, family, religion) to heterosexist beliefs (e.g., Jackson, 2006; Nielsen et al., 2000; Seidman, 2009). This likely results in internalized homonegativity, that is, negative attitudes and stigmatization of others solely based on their sexual identity (Habarth, 2015; Herek, 2000; T. G. Morrison et al., 1999). To sexual minority people, internalized homonegativity conflicts with the self-acceptance and disclosure of their sexual identity. According to the minority stress model (Meyer, 1995, 2013), internalized homonegativity can be associated with poorer mental outcomes, such as negative feelings toward one’s sexual identity and discrimination of non-heterosexual people (Herek, 2004), anxiety and depression (Lorenzi et al., 2015), and social anxiety (Lingiardi et al., 2012). These correlates were found to be cross-culturally robust (Sattler & Lemke, 2019). For example, Feinstein et al. (2012) found that sexual minority people who were stigmatized by others and their society reported more social anxiety and depressive symptoms. This occurred, at least in part, because sexual minority people had more internalized homonegativity, and therefore felt worse about themselves and their sexual identity. Internalized homonegativity was also one of the reasons why sexual identity discrimination is related to less subjective well-being (Conlin et al., 2019; Walch et al., 2016). This can have implications for physical health, such that people with more internalized homonegativity were more likely to have been diagnosed with sexually transmitted infections and to report substance abuse (Andrinopoulos et al., 2015; Berg et al., 2015), and are likely to perceive more stress one year later (Tatum & Ross, 2020). Internalized homonegativity can also shape interpersonal experiences. For example, people with more internalized homonegativity have exaggerated gender role performances (Eguchi, 2009) and engage in typically masculine behaviors (Herek et al., 2007), arguably to avoid being discriminated against by others. As such, sexual minority men with more heteronormative beliefs should have more internalized homonegativity, and in turn, report more sexual identity stigma. More broadly, these variables should also be associated with the likelihood of having disclosed one’s sexual identity to friends and family (e.g., Camacho et al., 2020).

Socio-cultural Differences in the Experiences of Sexual Minority Men

Research has shown higher levels of internalized homonegativity in more conservative socio-cultural contexts and lower levels of sexual identity stigma in more inclusive socio-cultural contexts (e.g., Brown et al., 2016; Herek et al., 2015; Riggle et al., 2017; Tran et al., 2018). Hence, a more comprehensive understating of sexual identity stigma and coming out of sexual minority men should examine socio-cultural differences, particularly in contexts typically underrepresented in the literature. Our research was focused on two countries—Portugal and Turkey—that have different approaches to the sexual minority community. Portugal has inclusive social policies and laws for sexual minority people, such that the Portuguese legal framework has laws against discrimination based on sexual identity since 2005, recognized same-sex unions in 2001, legalized same-sex marriage in 2010, and allows same-sex couples to adopt since 2016 (Carrol & Mendos, 2017; Costa, 2021). Despite not criminalizing same-sex behaviors, Turkey does not have socially inclusive policies or protection laws against sexual identity discrimination and does not recognize same-sex marriage (Carrol & Mendos, 2017; Engin, 2015). The way with which heteronormative beliefs are conveyed in both countries is also different. In Portugal, the disapproval of current inclusive policies mainly comes from opposing far-rights political parties and members of the Catholic Church (Brandão & Machado, 2012; Oliveira et al., 2013), although some degree of sexual discrimination is still observed in society. For example, results from the Eurobarometer on Discrimination (European Commission, 2019) showed that 71% of Portuguese people consider sexual identity discrimination to be a common practice in their country, but 78% would be comfortable with having a sexual minority person as a coworker and 50% would be comfortable with having their children be in a same-sex romantic relationship. In another study, Costa et al. (2018) found that most Portuguese people were supportive or extremely supportive of same-sex marriage (78%) and parenting (68%). Contrasting with these findings, results from the Public Perceptions of Gender Roles Survey in Turkey (O’Neil & Çarkoğlu, 2020) showed that 77% of Turkish people considered same-sex relationships to be against the social norm and 55% considered that sexual minority people should not have access to equal rights. In another study, results showed that only 25% of Turkish people believe that homosexuality should be accepted by society, contrasting with an average of 87% reported by some Western European counties (Pew Research Center, 2020). Unsurprisingly, then, research has shown that sexual minority Turkish people tend to engage in different identity concealment strategies to avoid being discriminated against (Bakacak & Ōktem, 2014; Ikizer et al., 2018).

Overview and Hypotheses

Most studies focus on the consequences of socially imposed heteronormativity on the mental health of sexual minority people (Herek et al., 2007; Meyer, 1995, 2013). Yet, research examining the role of the heteronormative beliefs—internalized through socialization—on the coming out process of sexual minority men is still scarce. We focused on this sample because previous research suggested that men tend to be more affected by heteronormative beliefs than women. For example, Eguchi (2009) showed that gay men tend to be more pressured to conform to masculine heteronormative roles, which arguably explains why they tend to engage in typically masculine behaviors in more conservative contexts. Moreover, sexual minority men also tend to report high levels of sexual identity stigma (Lingiardi et al., 2012). We also extended the literature by comparing two distinct and underrepresented socio-cultural contexts—Portugal and Turkey—that differ in heteronormative beliefs and social policies toward sexual minorities (Carrol & Mendos, 2017).

The coming out process results from a balance between heteronormative beliefs and sexual identity (Fassinger & Miller, 1997; Habarth, 2015). Hence, sexual minority men with more heteronormative beliefs should report more sexual identity stigma—more identity stigma and more social discomfort with their sexual identity (Hypothesis 1). The constant exposure to heteronormative values conveyed by society (Nielsen et al., 2000; Seidman, 2009) results in negative views and prejudice toward sexual minority people (Habarth, 2015; Herek, 2000; T. G. Morrison et al., 1999), which conflicts with a sexual minority identity and likely results in stigmatization. Based on this reasoning, we expected heteronormative beliefs to be positively associated with internalized homonegativity (Hypothesis 2). However, heteronormative beliefs and internalized homonegativity differ between socio-cultural contexts (Brown et al., 2016; Riggle et al., 2017; Tran et al., 2018), such that Portugal has more inclusive social policies and laws than Turkey (e.g., Costa, 2021; European Commission, 2019; O’Neil & Çarkoğlu, 2020). As such, we expected heteronormative beliefs to have stronger positive associations with identity stigma (Hypothesis 3) and internalized homonegativity (Hypothesis 4) among Turkish (vs. Portuguese) sexual minority men. Lastly, we expected internalized homonegativity to explain the association between heteronormative beliefs and sexual identity stigma, particularly among Turkish (vs. Portuguese) sexual minority men (Hypothesis 5). The hypothesized moderated mediation model is depicted in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Hypothesized moderated mediation model

Lastly, there is a dearth of research examining the correlates of coming out among Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men. As such, we explored the extent to which heteronormative beliefs, internalized homonegativity, and sexual identity stigma were associated with the likelihood of having disclosed one’s sexual orientation to friends and family. Given their role in the coming out process, we controlled for age, sexual orientation, relationship status, political orientation, and religiosity in all analyses (e.g., Barnes & Meyer, 2012; Crawford & Pilanski, 2014; Dyar et al., 2019; Hooghe et al., 2010; M. A. Morrison & Morrison, 2003; Pacilli et al., 2011; Pistella et al., 2016; Rosario et al., 2001; Ross et al., 2018; Taggart et al., 2019).

Method

Participants

A total of 952 people assessed the survey and 337 abandoned before survey completion. From the eligible sample, we removed participants who identified themselves as women or transgender women (n = 22), and those with missing values in all of our main measures (n = 31). The final sample included 562 sexual minority men (93.4% cisgender) with ages ranging from 18 to 67 (M = 26.69, SD = 9.59). All participants indicated to be sexually and/or romantically attracted to other men, most identified themselves as gay (76.2%), were single (61.9%), had center-left or left political orientations (68.5%), and never attended religious services (50.3%). Country comparisons further showed that Portuguese sexual minority men were more likely to be older, to identify themselves as gay, to be dating or to have a domestic partnership, to have center-right or center-left political orientations, and to attend religious services on special occasions, whereas Turkish minority men were more likely to be younger, to identify with other sexual orientations (e.g., pansexual, asexual, queer), to be single, to have a left political orientation, and to never attend religious services, all ps < .050 (see Table 1 for details).

Measures

Heteronormative Beliefs

We used the Normative Behavior subscale of the Heteronormative Attitudes and Beliefs Scale (Habarth, 2015). Participants were asked to indicate the extent with which they agree or disagree (1 = Strongly disagree to 7 = Strongly agree) with eight items, assessing typical gender role expectations in relationships (e.g., “Things go better in intimate relationships if people act according to what is traditionally expected of their gender”). We computed a single index by averaging responses across all items (Portugal: α = .62, Turkey: α = .72), with higher scores indicating more heteronormative beliefs.

Internalized Homonegativity

We used the Modern Homonegativity Scale (Morrison & Morrison, 2003) to assess modern prejudice toward sexual minority people. Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they agree or disagree (1 = Strongly disagree to 7 = Strongly agree) with 12 items (e.g., “Many gay men use their sexual orientation so that they can obtain special privileges”). One item was modified to fit other socio-cultural contexts (i.e., we rephrased “Canadians’ tax dollars…” to “Taxes…”). We computed a single index by averaging responses across all items (Portugal: α = .89, Turkey: α = .80), with higher scores indicating more internalized homonegativity.

Sexual Identity Stigma

We used two subscales of the Measure of Internalized Sexual Stigma for Gay Men (Lingiardi et al., 2012) to assess negative attitudes sexual minority men hold about their sexual orientation. The Identity stigma subscale (five items; Portugal: α = .74, Turkey: α = .79) assesses negative self-attitudes sexual minority men have about their sexuality and the internalization of sexual stigma as part of the identity (e.g., “Sometimes I think that if I were heterosexual, I could be happier”). The Social discomfort subscale (seven items; Portugal: α = .86, Turkey: α = .84) assesses the fear of public identification as a sexual minority man in social contexts and the fear of disclosure in social spheres (e.g., “I am careful of what I wear and what I say to avoid showing my homosexuality”). Participants were asked to what extent they agree or disagree with each item on a 7-point scale (1 = Strongly disagree to 7 = Strongly agree). We computed indexes for each subscale by averaging responses, with higher scores indicating more identity stigma and more social discomfort.

Political Orientation and Religiosity

We used two single-item measures to assess political orientation and religiosity (Morrison & Morrison, 2003). Participants were asked to indicate their political orientation (e.g., 1 = Right, 2 = Center-right, 3 = Center, 4 = Center-left, 5 = Left) and the frequency with which they attend to religious services (1 = Never, 2 = On special occasions, 3 = Now and then, 4 = Usually).

Procedure

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee at Iscte-Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ref.: 11/2019) before data collection. An online survey hosted on Qualtrics was available in Portuguese, Turkish, and English, between March and July 2019. The study protocol was the same in both countries and participants could select the language best suited to them. To recruit participants, we advertised the study in LGBTI + oriented Facebook groups and in gay dating apps (e.g., Grindr), used Instagram ads, and asked LGBTI + NGOs from both countries to share the survey in their social media and among their members.

Before starting, people were told that they would be taking part in a study examining their ideas on sexual orientation, gender roles, and relational behaviors. To participate, people had to be Portuguese or Turkish (or at least live in those countries), to have 18 years or older, to identify themselves as men, and to be sexually/romantically attracted to other men. Lastly, they were informed that participation was anonymous and voluntary, that participants had the right to skip or omit any questions or abandon the survey at any time. After providing written informed consent (clicking in the yes option), participants were asked to provide demographic information (e.g., nationality, country of residence, age, sex assigned at birth, gender identity, sexual orientation, relationship status) and to indicate if they came out to friends (No/Yes) and family members (No/Yes). This was followed by the main measures. Throughout the survey, participants were free to skip or omit their answers, i.e., they were reminded of any missed questions in the survey but allowed to continue. Upon completion, participants were informed that the study aimed to understand the role of heteronormative beliefs on self-accepting and disclosing one’s sexual orientation, and provided contact information of the main researcher. The mean completion time for the survey was 17 min.

Data Analysis

We first present overall descriptive statistics in our main measures, compared both countries using t tests, and overall correlations between the measures. We then test our theoretical model. We expected heteronormative beliefs to be positively associated with sexual identity stigma (Hypothesis 1) and with internalized homonegativity (Hypothesis 2). We also expected both associations to be moderated by country, such that heteronormative beliefs should have stronger associations with sexual identity stigma (Hypothesis 3) and internalized homonegativity (Hypothesis 4) for Turkish (vs. Portuguese) sexual minority men. Lastly, we expected internalized homonegativity to mediate the association between heteronormative beliefs and identity stigma, especially among Turkish (vs. Portuguese) sexual minority men (Hypothesis 5). We computed two 10,000 bootstrapped moderated mediation models using PROCESS 3.4 for SPSS (Model 8; Hayes, 2017). In both models, heteronormative beliefs were the predictor (X), internalized homonegativity was the mediator (M), and country (− 1 = Portugal; + 1 = Turkey) was the moderator (W). Variables that defined products were mean-centered before the analyses. Identity stigma (Y1) and social discomfort (Y2) were the different outcomes. In both analyses, age, sexual orientation, relationship status, political orientation, and religiosity were entered as covariates (see also Table 1). Lastly, we explored the correlates of coming out to friends and family (− 1 = No; + 1 = Yes) by computing two binary logistic regressions. Country, age, sexual orientation, relationship status, political orientation, and religiosity were entered in Step 1, and heteronormative beliefs, internalized homonegativity, identity stigma, and social discomfort were entered in Step 2.

Table 1 Demographic information

Results

Preliminary Analysis

As shown in Table 2, Turkish (vs. Portuguese) sexual minority men reported more heteronormative beliefs, p < .001, more homonegativity, p < .001, more identity stigma, p < .001, and more social discomfort, p < .001. We also found significant positive correlations between all measures, all ps < .001. For example, sexual minority men with more heteronormative beliefs also reported more internalized homonegativity, more identity stigma, and more social discomfort.

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and overall correlations

Main Analysis

Results of the moderated mediation models are summarized in Table 3. As expected, heteronormative beliefs were positively associated with identity stigma, p < .001, and social discomfort, p < .001 (Hypothesis 1). However, the expected interaction with country was only observed in Model B, p = .034 (Hypothesis 3), such that the positive association between heteronormative beliefs and social discomfort was significant for Turkish sexual minority men, p < .001, but not for Portuguese sexual minority men, p = .074 (Fig. 2).

Table 3 Results of the moderated mediation model
Fig. 2
figure 2

Heteronormative beliefs and social discomfort for Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men

Results also showed the expected positive association between heteronormative beliefs and internalized homonegativity, p < .001 (Hypothesis 2). Although this interaction was moderated by country, p = .002 (Hypothesis 4), we found significant associations for Turkish, p < .001, and Portuguese men, p < .001 (Fig. 3). On a closer inspection of the associations in each country, contrast analyses showed that Portuguese sexual minority men with less heteronormative beliefs (− 1 SD) reported less internalized homonegativity than Turkish sexual minority men, t(463) = 4.56, p < .001, whereas no differences emerged among Portuguese and Turkish men with more heteronormative beliefs (+ 1 SD), t < 1.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Heteronormative beliefs and internalized homonegativity for Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men

Lastly, results showed that internalized homonegativity was positively associated with identity stigma, p = .004, and social discomfort, p < .001. As expected (Hypothesis 5), conditional direct effects were stronger for Turkish sexual minority men. However, indexes of moderated mediation showed stronger indirect effects for Portuguese sexual minority men (see Table 4).

Table 4 Conditional direct effects, indirect effects, and index of moderated mediation

Coming Out to Friends and Family

Considering the overall sample, results showed that most participants made their coming out to friends (87.3%), and this was particularly evident among Portuguese (91.3% vs. Turkish: 81.0%) sexual minority men, χ2 (1) = 11.62, p = .001, V = .15. In contrast, only about half of the overall sample disclosed their sexual orientation to family members (51.7%). Again, such disclosure was more likely among Portuguese (65.1% vs. Turkish: 30.7%) sexual minority men, χ2 (1) = 61.54, p < 0.001, V = .34.

Results of the logistic regressions are summarized in Table 5. Disclosing one’s sexual orientation to friends was more likely among younger sexual minority men, p = .001, those identifying as gay, p < .001, those with less internalized homonegativity, p = .026, and those with less social discomfort, p < .001. Disclosing one’s sexual orientation to family members, on the other hand, was more likely among Portuguese men, p < .001, those identifying as gay, p = .032, those in a relationship, p = 0.010, and those with less social discomfort, p < .001.

Table 5 Logistic regression on the likelihood of having disclosed one’s sexual orientation to friends and family members

Discussion

Building upon the Stress Minority Model (Meyer, 2013), we examined if socially imposed heteronormative beliefs were associated with minority identity (i.e., sexual identity stigma), because sexual minority men experience more proximal minority stress processes (i.e., internalized homonegativity). The study also accounted for socio-cultural contextual differences, by examining if this process differed between Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men. Lastly, the study examined the correlates of having disclosed one’s sexual orientation to friends and family. Results generally supported our hypotheses, such that heteronormative beliefs were associated with more identity stigma and more social discomfort, and internalized homonegativity explained these associations. As heteronormative values regulate gender roles (Habarth, 2015; Jackson, 2006; Kitzinger, 2005; López-Sáez et al., 2020; Nielsen et al., 2000; Seidman, 2009), the internalization of such norms implies the stigmatization of non-conforming sexual identities and the development of negative attitudes toward people from sexual minorities (Herek, 2004). Faced with internalized homonegativity, sexual minority men have more difficulties in sexual identity development and experience more identity stigma and social discomfort.

These experiences should be particularly evident in socio-cultural contexts in which heteronormativity is endorsed and, consequently, internalized homonegativity is reinforced (for a discussion, see Herek et al., 2015). And yet, our results showed that Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men have different coming out experiences. Portuguese (vs. Turkish) men reported less heteronormative beliefs, internalized homonegativity, identity stigma, and social discomfort. This is aligned with past evidence showing that contexts with more supportive and inclusive policies—like the Portuguese context—can help sexual minority people develop their sexual identity (e.g., Bauermeister et al., 2010; Toomey et al., 2012), which can benefit the coming out process. We also found that heteronormative beliefs were associated with more identity stigma in both countries—albeit stronger among Turkish sexual minority men—and only with more social discomfort among Turkish sexual minority men. This resulted in stronger direct effects in this sample. These findings may be explained by the fact that identity stigma is a predisposition to consider sexual identity stigma as part of society's values (Lingiardi et al., 2012). To the extent that Portuguese and Turkish minority men acknowledge the existence of sexual identity stigma in their socio-cultural contexts (European Commission, 2019; O’Neil & Çarkoğlu, 2020), it is not surprising that socially imposed heteronormative beliefs shape the stigma sexual minority men experience with their own sexual identity. However, because Portuguese sexual minority men also acknowledge they would accept having a sexual minority person in their immediate social network (European Commission, 2019) and are more accepting of same-sex marriage and parenting (Costa et al., 2018), sexual minority men in this socio-cultural context may be more comfortable with expressing their sexual identity in social contexts, and less restrained by heteronormative social impositions. The opposing reasoning would be applied to the Turkish context (e.g., O’Neil & Çarkoğlu, 2020; Pew Research Center, 2020), which could explain why Turkish sexual minority men tend to conceal their sexual identity (Bakacak & Ōktem, 2014; Ikizer et al., 2018). Our results also showed country differences in the association between heteronormative beliefs and internalized homonegativity. Against our expectations, we found a stronger (rather than weaker) association in the Portuguese sample. A closer inspection of the slopes, however, showed that Portuguese sexual minority men who endorsed less heteronormative beliefs also had significantly less internalized homonegativity, whereas that was not the case with Turkish sexual minority men. In contrast, sexual minority men who endorsed more heteronormative beliefs also had significantly more internalized homonegativity, regardless of their socio-cultural context. As a consequence of these findings, indirect effects were stronger among Portuguese sexual minority men. A possible explanation for the steeper slope in the Portuguese sample could be taken from Oliveira et al. (2013). The authors suggested that Portuguese political strategies for sexual minority inclusion continue to be influenced by historical events and discourses that sustain strong heteronormative values (e.g., the persistence of Catholic morality, the Estado Novo regime). The authors also argued that heteronormativity values are still embedded in inclusive policies, because sexual minority inclusion is still somewhat dependent on how heteronormatively sexual minority people behave. For example, heteronormativity values regulate gender roles and sexual behavior by imposing the idea that sexual minority people are accepted insofar they comply with family-oriented values (Seidman, 2005). Aligned with this argument, Rodrigues et al. (2018) found no evidence of stigmatization when different-sex and same-sex romantic partners were in a committed monogamous relationship. In their analysis of the Portuguese context, Oliveira et al. (2013) further argued that some sexual minority groups and sexual minority people embraced this compliance idea to increase visibility, ensure safety, decrease discrimination and promote inclusive policies. The internalization of this dichotomy as part of the heteronormative beliefs system may explain who sexual minority people have internalized homonegativity toward others who do not comply with societal values, fit in society, or need to constantly draw attention to their sexual identity (Morrison & Morrison, 2003).

Examining the correlates of the coming out process, our results also showed clear country differences. We found that Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men were likely to have disclosed their sexual orientation to friends—but even more so in the Portuguese sample. However, Portuguese sexual minority men were more likely to have disclosed their sexual orientation to family members, whereas only one third of the Turkish sexual minority men were likely to have done so. Again, these results reflect the socio-cultural differences between Portugal and Turkey, and how these differences shape the likelihood of coming out to close others. Our exploratory results further showed that identifying as a sexual minority and being more socially comfortable with one’s sexual identity increased the likelihood of making one’s coming out to friends and family (see also Rosario et al., 2006). There were also specific correlates of coming out, such that being younger and having less internalized homonegativity were associated with coming out to friends, whereas having a romantic relationship was associated with coming out to family members. These findings suggest that coming out is experienced differently depending on the source of social support. To the extent that friends are often a source of emotional support and identification during identity development (e.g., Barry et al., 2016; Helsen et al., 2000; Rodrigues et al., 2017), a similar process might also occur in the development of sexual identity (e.g., Elizur & Ziv, 2001; Maguen et al., 2002). In this sense, younger sexual minority men might be more comfortable with disclosing their sexual identity to friends with whom they share a sense of belonging. In contrast, sexual minority men may be more comfortable disclosing their sexual identity to family members when they have emotional support from an intimate romantic partner (e.g., Martos et al., 2015; Pistella et al., 2016; Rodrigues, Huic, et al., 2019; Rodrigues, Lopes, et al., 2019; Rodrigues, Lopes, et al., 2019).

Limitations and Future Research

We must acknowledge some limitations to our study. First, our study does not allow us to establish causality between our variables. Nonetheless, it seems more likely that heteronormative beliefs become internalized through socialization and causes sexual minority people to develop homonegativity, rather than the other way around. Still, future research should seek to develop cross-cultural longitudinal studies to understand if heteronormative beliefs indeed predict the coming out process in different socio-cultural contexts and whether these temporal effects are explained by the levels of internalized homonegativity. Second, we did not measure relevant variables that could further moderate our findings or have implications for interpersonal processes. For example, internalized homonegativity has been associated with identity concealment (Gonçalves et al., 2020), dissatisfaction with sex life (Berg et al., 2015), less quality in romantic relationships (Doyle & Molix, 2015; Rostosky & Riggle, 2017), and less emotional intimacy with the romantic partner (Šević et al., 2016; Thies et al., 2016). To the extent that romantic partners, friends, and parents are important sources of emotional support to sexual minority people (e.g., Bauermeister et al., 2010; Doty et al., 2010; Friedman & Morgan, 2009; Rodrigues, Huic, et al., 2019; Rodrigues, Lopes, et al., 2019; Rothman et al., 2012; Whitton et al., 2018), future research should examine if such sources of support moderate some of our findings and their implications for interpersonal relationships and individual well-being. Future research should also seek to further understand the coming out process by using more detailed measures of coming out (e.g., breadth and depth of disclose in the social network) and the engagement in strategies to conceal one’s sexual identity from others, and include other relevant measures to assess prejudice and discrimination (e.g., polymorphous prejudice; Lopes et al., 2017).

Lastly, our sample was recruited from Portuguese and Turkish LGBTI + organizations, internet ads in social media, groups on the internet and dating apps targeting gay men, and flyers in gay venues. This particular strategy of recruitment has some limitations in itself, and may limit the generalizability of our findings to sexual minority men that are less involved with the LGBTI + community, or less knowledgeable about LGBTI + culture. Although we acknowledge the difficulties to collect data with reserved (e.g., closeted) sexual minority men, researchers should seek to collect data from these men to have a broader understanding of how internalized socio-cultural values shape the socialization, identity development, and coming out (or lack thereof) of these men.

Social and Policy Implications

Our study contributes to the existing knowledge about sexual identity. Some models of sexual identity development (e.g., Cass, 1979; Fassinger & Miller, 1997) suggest that sexual minority people experience a phase of identity confusion, defined by the feelings of being different, sexual identity confusion, anxiety, and fear. Our findings suggest that this phase could be fostered by the contradiction between one’s feelings and socially imposed heteronormativity. As identity confusion is the result of sexual minority people feeling they are different because they do not share a heterosexual identity, having non-heterosexuality as a socially valid identity would likely reduce the emotional turmoil of accepting one’s sexual identity. Therefore, to decrease or end the struggles of sexual minority people during their coming out process, heteronormative beliefs imposed by socio-cultural traditions and social institutions (e.g., family, education, neighborhood, religion) should be openly discussed and revised by educators, support providers, opinion leaders, and policymakers to promote sexual diversity in society and more inclusive policies against discrimination, and consequently promote psychological health and well-being (see also Pachankis et al., 2015, 2020; Pachankis & Bränström, 2019).

Our findings can also be relevant to gay affirmative therapy and LGBTI + activism, by offering a new view on how to understand the difficulties sexual minority men experience with self-hate, internalized homonegativity, and lack of connectedness with the LGBTI + community. According to Meyer (1995, 2013), sexual minority people can experience decreased levels of minority stress when they get actively involved with LGBTI + communities because these people stop evaluating themselves in comparison to members of the dominant heteronormative culture. Nonetheless, even after sexual minority people assumed their sexual identity, many of them still struggle with internalized homonegativity. Therefore, and based on our findings, to successfully tackle the issues that compromise the development of sexual identity and the coming out of men, it is crucial to deconstruct the heteronormative beliefs they have internalized. This is consistent with the reasoning of D’Augelli (1994), suggesting that sexual minority people must become aware and demystify internalized stereotypical preconceptions about non-heterosexuals in the development of their sexual identity. This is crucial to reduce the importance of heteronormative beliefs to the internalization of homonegativity and sexual identity stigma and to destigmatize their feelings, experiences, and behaviors. For example, different support groups (e.g., ONGs, associations, online forums) could strive to develop evidence-based awareness campaigns and interventions targeted not only at LGBTI + people of different ages but also at members of close networks (e.g., friends, family) outside the LGBTI + community. Indeed, perceived social support from close networks and identification with the LGBTI + community are positively associated with sexual identity acceptance and psychological well-being (e.g., Brandon-Friedman & Kim, 2016; Snapp et al., 2015). These efforts would help people challenge their heteronormative beliefs, and question their views about gender identity, sexual behavior, sexual orientation, and consequently promote inclusion and acceptance. For sexual minority people, it would also help them accept and be comfortable with their sexual identity, therefore aiding in the coming out process and promote well-being. Lastly, the particular ways heteronormative beliefs are conveyed in different socio-cultural contexts must also be considered. Based on our findings, campaigns and interventions would likely be more beneficial in hostile environments (e.g., Turkey; Engin, 2015), even though specific needs must also be considered even in more inclusive environments (e.g., older gay men in Portugal; Ribeiro-Gonçalves et al., 2021).