Introduction

Despite the famous figure of speech, people do not always "reap what they sow". Humanity suffers from varieties of injustice such as world hunger and wars or everyday discrimination against women as well as racial, religious, and sexual minorities. Yet according to one of the most influential theories about human belief systems, people adopt just-world beliefs that serve to enable them to construe their social environment as if it were just, stable, and orderly (Lerner, 1980). Furthermore, these beliefs provide people with certain cognitive ways to justify the status quo, underestimate the prevalence of injustice, or even blame innocent victims. Given these functions, just-world beliefs have been found to be important factors in well-being, religion, and justice-related perceptions or behavior. Iran is a highly religious country, where the majority of people strongly believe that everything happens according to god’s will and must be accepted as it is. “God is just” or “Be good so the world is good to you” are commonly used phrases. Despite the prevalence and importance of just-world beliefs, rigorous research on the psychology of these beliefs is lacking in Iran. To help overcome this gap, we aimed to validate self-report measures of individual differences in just-world beliefs, test their correlates and predictive implications, and the distinction between personal and general dimensions of belief in a just world. As a common and understudied justice-related issue that many people, especially in Iran, are confronted with on a daily basis takes root in favoritism toward kinship, we focus on perceptions of kin favoritism as a part of investigating the validity and distinction of the two just-world belief scales. To test the distinction, we used a design similar to the work of Bai et al. (2014). Beyond testing our own hypotheses, we replicate their work and complement their arguments.

Belief in a just world

According to just-world theory, many people across the world believe in a just world where people get what they deserve and deserve what they get. The BJW is considered a personality trait with an importance in the interaction between personality and other areas of social science. There are some adaptive functions that BJW is thought to serve, such as the confidence that important decisions about people are made with justice and the perception of life as safe and structured. In line with this idea, BJW has been found to be associated with self-esteem (Donat et al., 2016; Tatsi & Panagiotopoulou, 2021), life satisfaction (Dalbert, 1999), and a decrease in the perception of discrimination and unfairness of others (Furnham, 2003). It is essential to consider that while an individual can be an observer who only judges and interprets the world, the individual can also act as a participant. Therefore, Lipkus et al. (1996) propose that a GBJW should be distinguished from a PBJW.

The GBJW and PBJW scales were developed to measure the explicit endorsement of just-world beliefs (Dalbert, 1999; Dalbert et al., 1987). GBJW refers to the confidence that people, in general, get what they deserve and deserve what they get, while PBJW refers to the confidence that a person is usually treated in a just manner and plays an important adaptive role for people.

GBJW and PBJW were found to be positively correlated, however, they are identified with different meanings and psychological functions (see Dalbert, 1999; Lipkus et al., 1996; Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2018; Sutton & Douglas, 2005). GBJW helps people maintain subjective well-being (Dalbert, 1999), minimize perceived injustices (Sutton & Douglas, 2005), and defend against threats to just-world beliefs by strategies such as victim blaming (Hafer & Gosse, 2011). PBJW positively relates to individuals’ forgiveness (Strelan & Sutton, 2011), trust in others (Cubela Adoric & Kvartuc, 2007; Yu et al., 2020), and different aspects of subjective well-being (Dalbert, 1999; Donat et al., 2016). In attempting to examine the distinction between GBJW and PBJW we will explore people’s trust in justice in two different situations (witness vs. victim of injustice).

BJW, trust in justice, and the differentiation of GBJW and PBJW

As noted earlier, a function of both GBJW and PBJW is that they could shape individuals’ trust in others or perceived risk of becoming a victim of unfair decisions (Bai et al., 2014; Cubela Adoric & Kvartuc, 2007; Hafer & Gosse, 2011). To the extent that individuals believe the world treats others fairly (GBJW), they are less likely to perceive injustice as prevalent and anticipate that others will not become a victim of injustice. In a similar fashion, individuals’ belief about how they themselves are treated by the world (PBJW) could be linked to a prediction of outcomes in situations where they are the potential victims of injustice. In this article, we leverage the above-mentioned arguments to validate BJW scales and also look at the differentiation between GBJW and PBJW regarding how they predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism.

Injustice can take many forms, among which there is kin favoritism. Kin favoritism is a common type of injustice that arises from people’s preferences to favor members of their own extended family at the expense of injustice to outsiders. Such favoritism is especially prevalent in societies that have large close-knit family groups and strong family ties (Akbari et al., 2020). Iran is a country with strong family ties and is ranked 150th on the corruption perception index (Transparency International, 2021). Kin favoritism is the form of injustice that we plan to investigate because kin-based favoritism is probably the most common type of corruption in Iran. Previously, Bai et al. (2014) investigated the relationship between just-world beliefs and perceived intention of corruption using scenarios involving bribery and nepotism as forms of corruption. Their results suggested that BJW-others (i.e., GBJW), but not BJW-self (i.e., PBJW), can negatively predict perceived intention of corruptive behavior and that perceived likelihood of punishment acts as a mediator for this relationship. Later, Modesto et al. (2020) found that both GBJW and PBJW can predict perceived punishment and perceived corruption. In a design similar to their work, we will test our own hypotheses which include whether PBJW and GBJW can distinctly predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism in two different situations where the person is either a witness or a target of potential injustice. We hypothesize that GBJW, more likely than PBJW, can predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism when people imagine themselves as mere witnesses of a potentially unjust act of corruption. Bai et al. (2014, p.2) suggest that BJW-others, not BJW-self, is associated with corruption perceptions because “BJW-self is related to intra-individual outcomes, such as mental health and subjective well-being, whereas BJW-others is related to interpersonal level phenomena, such as higher levels of punitiveness toward offenders or harsh attitudes toward disadvantaged groups”. However, we argue that the nonsignificant relationship between BJW-self and corruption perception in their results is partly due to their participants being only a witness of potential corrupt behavior. As BJW-self or PBJW refers to the confidence that a person is usually treated in a just manner, it is most relevant when the person perceives themselves, rather than others, as a potential target of injustice. Thus, we expect that PBJW, more likely than GBJW, can predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism when participants imagine themselves as targets of a potentially unjust act of corruption. To better replicate the work of Bai et al. (2014) in Iran, we go beyond examining the mere association between BJW and perceived threats and take the further step of identifying the underlying processes. First, BJW has been linked with anticipating a high possibility of punishment (Bai et al., 2014, 2016). This link stems from a fundamental premise of BJW, which is that good behaviors are rewarded, and bad behaviors are punished. Second, perceived risk of punishment is a factor that people consider when they estimate the threat and prevalence of injustice. This relation occurs because, intuitively, people tend to engage in less illegal behaviors when the risk of punishment is high. Taken together, believing in a just world influences the extent to which individuals believe that people who do wrong will get punished, which in turn leads to the estimation of the likelihood that injustice takes place (Bai et al., 2014). While legal punishment is a central deterrent, the possibility of moral punishment and negative judgments of others can also relate to people’s perceived threat of injustice. Thus, the role of perceived risk of moral and legal punishment in the relationship between BJW and perceived likelihood of kin favoritism is worthy of investigation.

In summary, due to the distinction between GBJW and PBJW, we expect that GBJW, compared to PBJW, is more strongly associated with perceived likelihood of injustice (kin favoritism in this case) and punishment when the person is a mere witness of potential injustice. In contrast, when the person is the unfortunate target of unfair treatment, PBJW is expected to be more strongly associated with the anticipation of outcomes. Moreover, perceived likelihood of punishment is expected to explain the relationship between BJW dimensions and perceived likelihood of kin favoritism.

BJW and other correlates

In addition to trust in justice, previous research suggests several constructs to test the validity of GBJW and PBJW scales. Accordingly, we decided to inspect the correlations of GBJW and PBJW with well-being and personality dimensions. Moreover, we assume that religiosity is an important factor to consider in how beliefs in the just world are shaped in cultures that are highly influenced by religious beliefs. Thus, we will also investigate how religiosity is associated with GBJW and PBJW. We further discuss these associations and the relevant literature in the following sections.

BJW and well-being

The just-world hypothesis states that people who believe that the world treats them fairly may be confident in planning for long-term future goals, and expect their lives to be orderly, meaningful, and controllable. Directly or indirectly mediated by these implications, mental health is improved (Kiral Ucar et al., 2019). Indeed, previous research on BJW’s effects on well-being suggests that both GBJW and PBJW are associated with self-esteem (i.e., high self-esteem reflects a primary feeling of self-worth and self-admiration, while low self-esteem represents unhappiness with oneself, self-denial, and lack of self-respect), life satisfaction, and other components of well-being (Donat et al., 2016; Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2019; Tatsi & Panagiotopoulou, 2021). Also, the literature emphasizes that PBJW is a stronger predictor of these constructs than GBJW.

BJW and religiosity

Religious teachings promote the ideas that sufferings will be compensated and good deeds will ultimately be rewarded. Furthermore, several religious traditions and teachings across different religious affiliations, especially karmic religions, provide a supernatural framework in which actions are expected to lead to valence-congruent outcomes, meaning that mean people experience misfortune, and nice people experience success (Harvey & Callan, 2014; White et al., 2019). This idea reflects just-world believers’ conviction that – in a just world – good things happen to good people and bad things happen to bad people. The literature also generally supports a positive relationship between BJW and religiosity (Bègue, 2002; Harvey & Callan, 2014).

BJW and personality

BJW and the Big Five personality factors represent divergent constructs conceptualized as assumptions about justice in the world and self-describing attributes, respectively. Accordingly, Big Five personality factors can be used for testing the divergent validity of BJW scales. Although the two constructs are broadly considered as differentiable constructs and the Big Five items do not include the words “fair” or “just”, researchers found inconsistent results about the associations of GBJW and PBJW with the Big Five factors (Bollmann et al., 2015; Nudelman, 2013). Previous studies investigating these associations did not report any notable difference in the strength of the correlations with regard to GBJW and PBJW. Consistent with the literature (Bollmann et al., 2015; Nudelman, 2013), we expect that both BJW dimensions will have weak positive associations with extraversion and emotional stability, and nonsignificant associations with agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience.

Dark Triad traits (i.e. narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism) are characterized by exploiting others, dishonesty, aggressiveness, duplicity, and emotional coldness (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). We argue that antisocial people do not need to form a specific assumption about justice in the world as they may not care for justice and fairness at all. However, one might argue that BJW has been negatively correlated with rule-breaking behavior and antisocial tendencies (e.g., Donat et al., 2012). Thus, we argue that dark triad traits could be used for divergent validity as we expect that there will be weak negative relationships between BJW dimensions and the dark triad traits, if any. Consistent with this assumption, previous research found no significant associations between BJW and narcissism (Ames et al., 2006) as well as the honesty-humility personality factor (Bollmann et al., 2015).

The present studies

The aim of the present set of studies is two-fold. In study 1, a principal component analysis (PCA) and a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) were done on the Persian version of the GBJW and PBJW scales in order to investigate whether the two-factor structure, as reported in other cultures, can be supported in the Iranian context. Furthermore, the convergent and divergent validity of the Persian BJW scales were examined. For convergent validity, we tested the correlations of BJW scales with self-esteem, life satisfaction, and religiosity. For divergent validity, we examined the correlations of BJW scales with Big Five personality factors (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness) as well as Dark Triad traits (Narcissism, Psychopathy, and Machiavellianism). More specifically, in study 1, we hypothesize that both GBJW and PBJW will be positively associated with life satisfaction, self-esteem, and religiosity, but negatively associated with dark triad traits. Moreover, Big Five factors will be unrelated or weakly associated with GBJW and PBJW.

In study 2 and study 3, we aim to further test the validity of the scales by assessing the distinction between GBJW and PBJW in predicting perceived threat of injustice and punishment directed at others (study 2) and themselves (study 3). More specifically, in study 2, we hypothesize that GBJW, compared to PBJW, is more strongly associated with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism when the person is a mere witness, and perceived likelihood of punishment is expected to mediate the relationship. However, in study 3, we hypothesize that PBJW, compared to GBJW, is more strongly associated with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism when individuals imagine themselves as potential victims, and perceived likelihood of punishment is expected to mediate this relationship.

Study 1

In the first study, our aim was to test the psychometric properties of the Persian version of the GBJW and PBJW scales by using component factor analysis and confirmatory factor analysis and examine the convergent and divergent validity of these scales.

Method

Participants

A total of 454 Iranian participants, aged between 17 and 65 years (M = 32.09, SD = 10.63), were recruited through online social media platforms such as Instagram, Twitter, etc. The sample consisted of 68.5% female and 31.5% male participants, and the majority (59.5%) were not married. Among the participants, 35 held a doctorate degree, 191 held a master’s degree, 138 held a bachelor’s degree, and 91 had a diploma or less. The recommended minimums for sample size in studies conducting factor analysis typically range between 5 and 20 per variable (Henson & Roberts, 2006). Thus, the sample size was adequate as we sought to surpass even the conservative suggestions for choosing sample sizes.

Procedure and Measures

After a brief introduction, the consenting participants completed an online questionnaire consisting of the GBJW and PBJW scales, as well as Rosenberg Self-esteem scale, Satisfaction with Life scale, Religiosity scale, Big Five scale, and the Dirty Dozen Dark Triad scale. Except for items in the religiosity scale, the extent to which the participants agreed with each statement was measured through a seven-point Likert scale that ranged from 1 (totally disagree) to 7 (totally agree).

GBJW and PBJW

The GBJW and PBJW scales, the psychometric properties of which were identified by Dalbert (1999) and Dalbert et al. (1987), were translated into Persian through the method of translation-back translation. Participants are asked to indicate the degree to which they agree with the statements about GBJW (6 items) and PBJW (7 items).

Self-Esteem

We measured self-esteem with the Persian version of the 10-item Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg, 1965). The RSES is commonly used to measure a person’s overall self-esteem (e.g., "On the whole, I am satisfied with myself."). The Persian translation of this scale has shown adequate validity and reliability in previous work (Shapurian et al., 1987). The alpha coefficient of the RSES for this sample was 0.85.

Life Satisfaction

The 5-item Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS; Diener et al., 1985) was used to assess participants’ global life satisfaction (e.g., “In most ways my life is close to my ideal.”). The Persian version of the scale has shown adequate psychometric properties in previous research (Joshanloo & Afshari, 2011). In our sample, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86.

Religiosity

Finally, participants responded to the 5-item Duke University Religion Index (DUREL; Koenig et al., 1997). The first two items measuring religious activities (e.g., "How often do you attend mosque or other religious meetings?") are rated along a 6-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (more than once a week/day). The last three items measuring intrinsic religiosity (e.g., "In my life, I experience the presence of God.") are rated along a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (definitely not true) to 5 (definitely true of me). The scores were transformed to z-scores since they have different response scales. The Persian version of this scale has shown very good psychometric characteristics (Hafizi et al., 2013). In the present study, the alpha coefficient of the scale was 0.89.

Big Five Personality

We measured the Big Five personality factors with the Ten-Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling et al., 2003) which is a very short measure of the Big Five. The TIPI consists of five subscales: Extraversion (e.g., "I see myself as: extraverted, enthusiastic."), Agreeableness (e.g., "I see myself as: sympathetic, warm."), Conscientiousness (e.g., "I see myself as: dependable, self-disciplined."), Emotional Stability (e.g., "I see myself as: calm, emotionally stable."), and Openness to Experience (e.g., "I see myself as: open to new experiences, complex."); each of them measured with two items. In the present study, internal consistency coefficients for extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and openness to experience were 0.77, 0.17, 0.53, 0.52, and 0.32, respectively. Despite being used extensively, estimates of the internal consistency of the TIPI are low in different contexts (Ehrhart et al., 2009; Gosling et al., 2003; Romero et al., 2012). The Persian translation of this scale has been used in previous research in Iran (Atari & Yaghoubirad, 2016).

Dark Triad Traits

The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen Scale (Jonason & Webster, 2010) was used to measure narcissism (4 items; e.g., “I tend to seek prestige or status”), psychopathy (4 items; e.g., “I tend to lack remorse”), and Machiavellianism (4 items; e.g., “I tend to manipulate others to get my way”). The Persian translation of the 12-item questionnaire has shown satisfactory psychometric characteristics in previous research (Aghababaei et al., 2014). In the present study, internal consistency coefficients for narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism were 0.81, 0.23, and 0.73, respectively.

Results

In order to investigate the dimensional structure of the Persian BJW items, Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) were employed. First, the data sample was randomly split into two equal subsamples. The first subsample was used for PCA (n = 227) and the second subsample was used for CFA (n = 227).

Principal component analysis

We evaluated the structure of the 13 items using a PCA with direct oblimin rotation. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO = 0.902) and Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (< 0.001) indicated that our sample is adequate and suitable for PCA. The first principal component analysis showed two factors with eigenvalues greater than 1 (6.10 and 1.71), which was also confirmed by Parallel and Scree Analyses as well. Consequently, a second PCA with direct oblimin rotation was performed with a fixed number of two factors. As can be seen in Table 1, all items of the PBJW formed the first factor, and all items of the GBJW formed the second factor. This two-factor solution accounted for 60.15% of the total variance.

Table 1 Principal component analysis results and descriptive statistics of GBJW and PBJW

Consistent with previous studies (Dalbert, 1999), participants endorsed the PBJW more strongly than the GBJW, M = 4.38 (SD = 1.32) versus M = 3.50 (SD = 1.44), t(1, 45) = -14.58, p < 0.001. Also, age was positively correlated with GBJW (r = 0.20, p < 0.001), but not with PBJW (r = 0.02, p = 0.532). However, there were no gender differences in GBJW (Welch t statistic = 0.62, p = 0.430) and PBJW (Welch t statistic = 0.31, p = 0.575).

Confirmatory factor analysis

We used CFA to examine the previously identified factor structure of the scale. Due to positive correlations between GBJW and PBJW, the two factors were permitted to co-vary. We assessed model fit using the Chi-square over degree-of-freedom (χ2 /df), Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and the Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR). Maximum likelihood was used as the estimation method. The analysis was performed using the Lavaan statistical package in the R software environment (Rosseel, 2012).

In the first model, we assumed that the two-factor with a covariation between latent factors would account for the variance of 13 items. Similar to the Russian adaptation of just-world scales (Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2018), our first model was not fit (χ2 (64) = 217.504, χ2/df = 3.398; GFI = 0.872, CFI = 0.890; TLI = 0.866; RMSEA = 0.103; SRMR = 0.069). Following Nartova-Bochaver et al. (2018)’s solution, we allowed the items with wording similarities that might share common variance to correlate (PBJW1 & PBJW3, PBJW3 & GBJW2, PBJW5 & GBJW5, and GBJW3 & GBJW5). The second model fit parameters reached values indicating good fit of the model (χ2 (64) = 136.168, χ2/df = 2.127; GFI = 0.921, CFI = 0.945; TLI = 0.929; RMSEA = 0.075; SRMR = 0.055). Internal consistency scores of GBJW (α = 0.85) and PBJW (α = 0.87) were high. To sum up, the two-dimensional structure of the GBJW and PBJW scales in Iran and the distinction between GBJW and PBJW has been confirmed.

Convergent validity

Self-esteem, life satisfaction, and religiosity were used to establish the convergent validity of the PBJW and GBJW scales. As presented in Table 2, self-esteem was positively associated with PBJW and GBJW. Also, self-esteem was more strongly associated with PBJW than GBJW (Fisher’s ζ = 4.46, p < 0.001). Results yielded significant associations between life satisfaction and both PBJW and GBJW. Similar to self-esteem, life satisfaction was more strongly associated with PBJW than GBJW (Fisher’s ζ = 4.17, p < 0.001). Moreover, there was a positive correlation between religiosity and both dimensions of BJW. However, the association between religiosity and GBJW was stronger (Fisher’s ζ = 6.56, p < 0.001).

Table 2 Correlations among Study 1 variables

Divergent validity

To inspect the divergent validity of the BJW scales, we tested the correlations between PBJW and GBJW with the Big Five personality factors (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability, and Openness) and dark triad traits. The results indicated that GBJW was positively associated with extraversion, conscientiousness, and emotional stability. In a similar pattern, PBJW was positively associated with extraversion, conscientiousness, and emotional stability. However, the associations of the BJW dimensions with agreeableness and openness were not significant. Concerning the relations between dark triad traits and BJW dimensions, while Machiavellianism and narcissism were not significantly associated with BJW dimensions, there was a weak positive association of both GBJW and PBJW with psychopathy.

Discussion

The findings of this study confirm the two-dimensional structure of the GBJW and PBJW scales in Iran. In general, GBJW scores were higher among Iranians than PBJW scores. Age was found to be positively correlated with GBJW. However, there was no significant relationship between PBJW and age. Gender was not significantly related to either GBJW or PBJW.

Convergent validity of GBJW and PBJW was confirmed by their positive correlation with self-esteem, life satisfaction, and religiosity. Religiosity had a stronger correlation with GBJW compared to PBJW. On the contrary, life satisfaction and self-esteem were more strongly correlated with PBJW than GBJW. Divergent validity of the BJW scales was partially confirmed by their small effect sizes and non-significant correlations with the Big Five personality factors and the Dark Triad traits. GBJW and PBJW were both positively correlated with the extraversion, conscientiousness, and emotional stability factors. There was no significant correlation between the BJW scales and the agreeableness and openness factors of the Big Five personality factors and Machiavellianism and Narcissism traits. There was a weak correlation between GBJW and PBJW with psychopathy. However, due to the low internal consistency of the scales in the Big Five personality factors and the Dark Triad Traits, these interpretations should be considered with great caution.

Study 2

In this study, we aim to test the relations of GBJW and PBJW with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism which participants witnessed as a potential injustice. We hypothesize that as the participants take on the role of “witnesses to a potential unjust situation”, GBJW, more likely than PBJW, will predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism. The relation is expected to be mediated by perceived likelihood of punishment.

Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited from social media platforms (e.g., Twitter, Telegram). We recruited 279 Iranian participants, 103 males and 176 females who were between 18 and 58 years old (M = 31.14; SD = 7.16). A sensitivity power analysis revealed that with a sample of N = 279 and a standard statistical power (1 – b = 0.8), we can reliably detect an effect size of r = 0.16.

Procedure and measures

After reading the informed consent, participants first completed the GBJW and PBJW scales. Then, they read the designed scenarios and responded to measures of perceived likelihood of injustice and perceived likelihood of punishment. After completing the questionnaire, participants were thanked and fully debriefed. The contact information of the main researcher was provided in case participants had further questions. Full ethical review and approval were not required for this study in accordance with the national and institutional guidelines.

GBJW and PBJW

GBJW and PBJW was assessed with the Persian translation of the 6-item GBJW Scale and 7-item PBJW Scale that were validated in Study 1. All responses were made on 7-point scales. The scores from the GBJW and PBJW items were averaged showing good internal consistency (α = 0.84 and α = 0.89, respectively). Higher scores indicated stronger endorsement of GBJW and PBJW.

The kin favoritism scenarios

We designed five scenarios in a panel discussion among the authors and a social activist in justice-related issues. In these scenarios, three individuals were portrayed; a target of injustice, a favor receiver, and a favor provider. The favor provider could unfairly treat their kin favorably in each scenario which were in different contexts, including hiring for a government job and applying for an educational position. The full scenarios are presented in Appendix 1. A sample scenario about hiring for a government job reads:

Two individuals have been chosen as the final candidates for a government job position. Considering the resumes, one of them is clearly better qualified for the job. A recruiter who works under the supervision of higher ranks has to do an interview with these candidates to decide who gets the job. Coincidentally, the recruiter turned out to be a relative of the less qualified candidate. This recruiter knows that discriminating between candidates and not choosing the most qualified person is against the rules.

Following each scenario, perceived likelihood of kin favoritism was assessed by “please estimate the likelihood that the favor provider will favor/choose his/her kin” on a 9-point scale ranging from 1 (definitely will not) to 9 (definitely will). For perceived likelihood of punishment, participants answered two questions: the likelihood of legal punishment and the likelihood of negative moral judgment by others. A sample item was “please estimate the likelihood of legal punishment from the favor provider’s authorities if he/she favors his/her kin” on 9-point scales ranging from 1 (definitely will not be) to 9 (definitely will be). We averaged the scores of the five scenarios for perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (5 items, α = 0.71) and perceived likelihood of punishment (10 items, α = 0.84).

Results

Table 3 shows the descriptive statistics and correlations of the variables in study 2. Correlations between GBJW, PBJW, perceived likelihood of punishment, and perceived likelihood of kin favoritism were significant in the expected directions.

Table 3 Correlations among Study 2 variables

Using the Lavaan statistical package (Rosseel, 2012) in the R software environment, a series of hierarchical regression analyses were performed to examine the effects of GBJW, PBJW, perceived punishment, and demographics on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism. As shown in Table 4, we first entered age and gender as controlled variables followed by entering GBJW and PBJW in the second step. As expected, GBJW predicted perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (β = -0.26, t = -3.83, p < 0.001). PBJW also predicted perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (β = -0.14, t = -2.09, p = 0.037), but less strongly. In the third step, perceived likelihood of punishment was included as a variable in the regression analysis and emerged as the strongest predictor of perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (β = -0.31, t = -5.50, p < 0.001). The effect of GBJW, but not PBJW, remained significant after including perceived likelihood of punishment (β = -0.18, t = -2.79, p = 0.006).

Table 4 The results of hierarchical regression for predicting the likelihood of kin favoritism when individuals are observers of a potential injustice

We further investigated the mediating effect of perceived likelihood of punishment in the relationship between GBJW and perceived likelihood of kin favoritism by the bootstrapping method (5000 bootstrap samples). The results showed that the indirect effect of GBJW (β = -0.09, p = 0.001) and PBJW (β = -0.07, p = 0.001) on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism were significant. Direct effects of GBJW (β = -0.25 p < 0.001) and PBJW (β = -0.20 p < 0.001) on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism were also significant.

We note that there are variables in the proposed mediation framework that were measured in a temporal order different from that tested in the mediation analysis. However, all variables were measured within minutes of each other, in a single experimental session. For data to be consistent with mediation, temporal order is both unnecessary and insufficient; as with any correlational analysis, alternative causal structures can only be ruled out by experimentation which is able to establish causation (Gelfand et al., 2009; Fielder et al., 2011; Shrout & Bolger, 2002).

Discussion

In this study, the findings point out the distinction between GBJW and PBJW. When people imagine themselves as mere observers of a situation where an unjust behavior might occur, they utilize GBJW more than PBJW in their perception of the risk of injustice. Furthermore, the results indicate that the relationships of GBJW and PBJW with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism are partially mediated by perceived likelihood of punishment.

Study 3

In the third study, we aim to test the relations of GBJW and PBJW with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism which participants imagined as a potential injustice directed at themselves. We hypothesize that as the participants take on the role of “victim of a potential unjust situation”, PBJW, more likely than GBJW, will predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism. The relation is expected to be mediated by perceived likelihood of punishment.

In addition, we plan to assess whether perceived risk of becoming a victim of injustice is higher when the threat of unjust behavior is directed to self rather than others.

Methods

Participants

We recruited 292 Iranians from social media platforms (e.g., Twitter, Telegram). There were 61 males and 231 females between 18 and 60 years old (M = 32.65; SD = 7.25). A sensitivity power analysis revealed that with a sample of N = 292 and a standard statistical power (1 – b = 0.8), we can reliably detect an effect size of r = 0.16.

Procedure and measures

The study was presented to participants as in Study 1. Participants were asked to read the informed consent before reading the scenarios and answering the questions.

GBJW and PBJW

GBJW and PBJW were assessed using previous studies’ measures. The internal consistency for GBJW and PBJW were 0.88 and 0.91, respectively.

The kin favoritism scenarios

The same five scenarios as in Study 2 were used with only a difference in the subject of injustice; participants imagined themselves as someone who is the unfortunate target of potential injustice. A sample scenario that participants read was:

“You have been chosen as one of the two final candidates for a government job position. Considering the resumes, you are clearly better qualified for the job than the other candidate. A recruiter who works under the supervision of higher ranks has to do an interview with you and the other candidate to decide who gets the job. Coincidentally, the recruiter turned out to be a relative of the other candidate. This recruiter knows that discriminating between candidates and not choosing the most qualified person is against the rules.”

Following each scenario, the same items as in Study 2 for measuring perceived likelihood of kin favoritism and perceived likelihood of punishment were used. The scores of the five scenarios were averaged for perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (5 items, α = 0.80) and perceived likelihood of punishment (10 items, α = 0.87).

Results

Table 5 shows the descriptive statistics and correlations of the variables in Study 3. There were significant correlations between GBJW, PBJW, perceived likelihood of punishment, and perceived likelihood of kin favoritism in the expected directions.

Table 5 Correlations among Study 3 variables

We used the Lavaan statistical package (Rosseel, 2012) in the R software environment for regression and mediation analyses. Initially, a series of hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to investigate the effects of GBJW, PBJW, perceived likelihood punishment, and demographics on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism. As can be seen in Table 6, we entered age and gender as controll variables in the first step before entering GBJW and PBJW in the second step. As expected, PBJW predicted perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (β = -0.24, t = -3.10, p < 0.001). GBJW (β = -0.21, t = -3.10, p = 0.002) also predicted perceived likelihood of kin favoritism, but less strongly. In the third step, perceived likelihood of punishment was included as a variable in the regression analysis and emerged as the strongest variable to negatively predict perceived likelihood of kin favoritism (β = -0.30, t = -5.52, p < 0.001). The effect of PBJW (β = -0.18, t = -2.84, p = 0.005) and GBJW (β = -0.15, t = -2.26, p = 0.024) remained significant after including perceived likelihood punishment.

Table 6 The results of hierarchical regression for predicting the likelihood of kin favoritism when individuals are potential victims of injustice

Then, we further investigated the mediating effect of perceived likelihood of punishment in the relationship of GBJW and PBJW with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism by the bootstrapping method (5000 bootstrap samples). The results showed that the indirect effect of GBJW (β = -0.12, p < 0.001) and PBJW (β = -0.12, p = 0.001) on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism were significant. The direct effects of GBJW (β = -0.32, p < 0.001) and PBJW (β = -0.31, p < 0.001) on perceived likelihood of kin favoritism were also significant.

Discussion

In study 3, the distinction between GBJW and PBJW was further confirmed. When individuals imagine themselves as targets of potentially unjust behavior, they utilize both GBJW and PBJW in their perception of the risk of injustice but the effect of PBJW is slightly stronger. Again, as anticipated, perceived likelihood of punishment partially mediated the relationship of GBJW and PBJW with perceived likelihood of kin favoritism.

General discussion

This set of studies provided support for the use of BJW scales in a predominantly Muslim country. As in the case of the original versions of the scales, the two-factor model that includes GBJW and PBJW was proved to fit well. Study 1 findings confirm the two-dimensional structure of the GBJW and PBJW scales in Iran, thus replicating the previous findings (Dalbert, 1999; Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2018). Consistent with the literature (Dalbert, 1999; Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2018; Sutton & Douglas, 2005), GBJW among Iranians was higher than PBJW. Regarding the relations between age and BJW dimensions, we found that older individuals believed more strongly in GBJW than younger people. However, there was no significant relation between PBJW and age. Furthermore, in line with prior research (Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2018; Sutton & Douglas, 2005), GBJW and PBJW scores were not different with respect to gender.

The validity of GBJW and PBJW scales was further investigated by examining the associations between these dimensions and self-esteem, life satisfaction, religiosity, big five personality factors, and dark triad traits. Consistent with the literature, GBJW and PBJW were correlated with these constructs with different strengths. The significant positive relations between BJW dimensions and self-esteem as well as life satisfaction and religiosity demonstrate the convergent validity of GBJW and PBJW scales. Moreover, the stronger correlations of PBJW, compared to GBJW, with self-esteem and life satisfaction indicate that well-being may be more dependent on individuals’ belief in being treated fairly by others rather than their belief in the existence of justice for people in general. Conversely, the stronger correlation of GBJW, compared to PBJW, with religiosity implies that religion may be more involved with the aspects of justice in society and for people in general. In addition to replicating previous research on BJW correlates (Bègue, 2002; Donat et al., 2016; Nartova-Bochaver et al., 2019), these findings support the different psychological functioning of GBJW and PBJW.

Our findings showed weak effect sizes or non-significant correlations of BJW dimensions with big five personality factors which partially confirm the divergent validity of BJW scales. However, as we will discuss in the limitations of this study, these findings should be interpreted cautiously due to low internal consistency of the scales. In line with previous studies (Nudelman, 2013), there were positive correlations of GBJW and PBJW with extraversion suggesting that believing in a just world may contribute to sociability and energetic behavior by reducing feelings of vulnerability. Furthermore, our findings on the relations between BJW dimensions and conscientiousness were consistent with the meta-analysis of Nudelman and Otto (2021). We found positive correlations of both GBJW and PBJW with conscientiousness which demonstrates that just-world beliefs may be positively related to diligence, carefulness, and deliberation because believers in a just world are convinced that being conscientious will lead to deserved outcomes. Also, the positive associations between BJW dimensions and emotional stability were consistent with previous research (Nudemlan, 2013). This finding supports the notion that GBJW and PBJW are buffers against unfortunate incidents in people’s life that decrease anxiety and depression (Furnham, 2003; Lipkus et al., 1996). Finally, agreeableness and openness did not have a significant relationship with BJW dimensions which supports findings of previous research (Bollmann et al., 2015; Nudelman, 2013).

Regarding the relations with dark triad traits, there was no relationship between BJW dimensions and Machiavellianism as well as narcissism which further confirms the divergent validity of BJW scales. As people high in Machiavellianism and narcissism may not simply care about justice and fairness, they may not feel the need to form a particular view about the existence of justice in the world. Although psychopathy had weak positive correlations with GBJW and PBJW, this finding is unreliable because the internal consistency coefficient of psychopathy scale in this study was very low.

Study 2 and study 3 provided additional support for the distinction between GBJW and PBJW by exploring perceptions of injustice in two different conditions. Both GBJW and PBJW predicted perceived likelihood of kin favoritism when individuals imagined themselves as either observers or potential victims of unjust behavior, and perceived likelihood of punishment partially mediated the associations. Importantly, when individuals imagined themselves as mere observers of injustice (study 2), GBJW was a stronger predictor. On the other hand, when they imagined themselves as potential victims, PBJW was slightly more important. While our findings are consistent with previous research (Bai et al., 2014; Modesto et al., 2020), they extend the literature by stressing the important role of people’s perspectives when imagining unjust behavior in their perception of outcomes and how it can be predicted by GBJW and PBJW. Overall, our findings provide empirical support for the distinction between GBJW and PBJW and underscore the importance of considering this distinction in research on the functions of just world beliefs in risk perception and trust in justice (Cubela Adoric & Kvartuc, 2007; Hafer & Gosse, 2011). While replicating previous research on the role of BJW in risk perception and trust in others, we examined GBJW and PBJW scales’ roles in perceived threat of becoming a victim of injustice in the context of kin favoritism which is an overlooked and pervasive form of injustice.

Practical and theoretical implications

The present study validated a much-needed scale of GBJW and PBJW in the Iranian context. Iranian psychologists can now use these scales to further study different aspects of BJW and develop interventions. For instance, consistent with previous research, BJW and perceived punishment can also influence people’s intention for corrupt behavior (Bai et al., 2016). Thus, emphasizing the importance of justice and highlighting punishment can be potential interventions for corruption and rule-breaking behavior. Overall, with the prevalence of kin favoritism and other forms of corruption in Iran, researchers can leverage how BJW and other relevant factors interact to develop measures for preventing corruption.

The current study contributes to understanding individual differences in the prediction of injustice and corrupt behavior. As suggested in our results, both GBJW and PBJW may have a role in perceived corruption. However, their role may be different when they perceive themselves or other people as victims of corruption. We also replicated previous findings regarding the important role of perceived punishment in explaining the link between BJW and corruption perception.

Limitations

There were some limitations in the present studies. First, our samples mainly consisted of educated people living in Tehran which is the capital of Iran with a population of above-average education, economic status, and liberal political orientation. Thus, the results of our studies can be generalized to only some degree.

A major limitation concerns our measures of the big five personality factors and dark triad traits. Big five personality dimensions were assessed using a short measure with two items for each factor. Given the brevity of the TIPI scales, it is not surprising that they usually have low alpha coefficients (Atari & Yaghoubirad, 2016; Ehrhart et al., 2009; Romero et al., 2012). The original creators of TIPI have noted that the scale was designed to maximize the content validity and breadth of coverage in a few adjectives which would cost them the standards of internal consistency. Nevertheless, future studies could use longer versions of the big five personality factors scales to rule out measurement errors of short scales. Moreover, the internal consistency of the psychopathy subscale in the dark triad traits scale was very low. Therefore, we encourage future researchers to consider these issues in their work.

Furthermore, there was a relatively high correlation between GBJW and PBJW throughout the three studies which is consistent to the literature. While multicollinearity was checked and was not found to be problem, readers should be cautious in interpreting the effect sizes of GBJW and PBJW in our regression analyses.

Finally, a methodological limitation of the current studies is their correlational nature which implies that one should be cautious in ascribing causality to our findings. Future studies should use experimental designs for investigating BJW’s role in well-being, religiosity, and perceived threat of injustice. Although GBJW and PBJW are usually measured as traits, it is also possible to manipulate them (see Kogut, 2011).

Concluding remarks

Here we applied the just-world theory to the Iranian context and showed how it is relevant to Iranians’ perception of kin favoritism as a common corrupt behavior in Iran and probably similar societies with strong family structures. After validating the Persian version of GBJW and PBJW scales, we demonstrated the importance of people’s perspectives as victims or observers of potential injustice in how GBJW and PBJW can predict perceived likelihood of punishment and kin favoritism.