Introduction

Reading comprehension is defined as a gained understanding of written text through the process of extracting and constructing meaning (Rimrodt et al. 2009), and is one of the most essential academic skills (Nation and Snowling 1997). In the United States alone, 10–15 % of school-age children exhibit poor comprehension skills despite normal levels of reading accuracy and fluency (Stothard and Hulme 1995; Yuill and Oakhill 1991). The Simple View theory of reading suggests that reading comprehension relies on both technical reading (decoding and word recognition) and language comprehension (Gough and Tunmer 1986). However, it is challenging to isolate the mechanisms underlying reading comprehension (Spencer et al. 2014). The current study used neuroimaging tools to identify associations between reading comprehension and more basic linguistic abilities.

Prior imaging studies of reading comprehension have largely focused on word- or sentence-level processing (Booth et al. 1999; Caplan et al. 2001; Cutting et al. 2006; Keller et al. 2003; Mason and Just 2007; Meyer et al. 2000; Ni et al. 2000; Rimrodt et al. 2009). In general, reading sentences activates the same regions as reading words with more widespread and bilateral activation, specifically in the middle and superior temporal gyri, bilateral temporal poles, and left frontal and parietal regions. A recent study using Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) data enabled the distinction between reading comprehension and technical/orthographic reading by demonstrating that these abilities are associated with two distinct white matter tracts (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014), which was done by correlating the white matter integrity (defined by Fractional Anisotropy; FA) with reading measures. The FA values in the left Arcuate Fasciculus [AF, a part of the Superior Longitudinal Fasciculus (SLF)] were correlated with greater reading scores (from the TOWRE-2 test; Torgesen et al. 2012), whereas the FA values in the right Inferior Longitudinal Fasciculus (ILF) were correlated with Passage Comprehension scores (from the Woodcock-Johnson III or WJ-III battery; see Woodcock et al. 2001). The results of that study also provided additional support for the role of the right hemisphere in reading/language comprehension. This view is supported by the different subpopulations of individuals with difficulties in reading comprehension: those who have impaired technical reading vs those who have intact technical reading, but other language problems (see Spencer et al. 2014). Studies in which participants were asked to comprehend auditory-presented narrative (listen to stories) reported larger regions of right temporal lobe activity than when participants listened to the same sentences presented in a conceptually unrelated manner (e.g., Plante et al. 2006; Robertson et al. 2000). These studies raise the question of precisely which language-comprehension skills the right hemisphere might support.

Evidence from patients with right hemisphere brain damage suggests that these patients demonstrate deficits comprehending nonliteral and indirect language including metaphors, humor, and sarcasm (e.g., see Johns et al. 2008 for a review). Despite evidence of understanding the main ideas contained in a narrative, these patients also were unable to use semantic context to interpret new information (Roman et al. 1987; Wapner et al. 1981). These findings raise the question as to the relationship between reading comprehension and neural circuits supporting language comprehension.

Semantics is one of the first stages in language comprehension (Friederici 2012; Muller and Hagoort 2006). Ventral and dorsal pathways have been shown to have a role in language comprehension (Friederici 2012). The ventral pathway supports semantic and syntactic processes and connects the temporal cortex with Broca’s area [Brodmann area (BA) 44] through two white matter tracts: the AF and the ILF. The dorsal pathway is involved in syntactic processes and connects the temporal cortex with premotor cortex through the parts of the SLF not including the AF (see Friederici 2012 for the complete model). Despite the knowledge of the involvement of the Superior Temporal Gyrus (STG; BA22), frontal operculum (BA 44), pars opercularis (BA 45) and the triangularis pars orbitalis (BA 47) in semantic processing, the exact neural circuits supporting semantic processing and how this ability corresponds with reading comprehension are still a matter of debate (Friederici et al. 2006; 2012).

The current gap in understanding of the Simple View theory is the lack of functional and anatomical evidence supporting the reliance of reading comprehension on neural circuits supporting language comprehension. Obtaining this information would provide biological support for the Simple View theory as well as for impairments, including for individuals who suffer from reading comprehension problems despite intact reading proficiency (for further description of this population, see Spencer et al. 2014). The purpose of the current study was to examine the structural connectivity in networks supporting reading comprehension using DTI data and its relationship to functional connectivity in a language comprehension fMRI task.

Since sentence processing is considered a language comprehension task that encompasses semantic and syntactic components as in reading comprehension, we examined the shared neural circuits between reading and language comprehension as measured by the percentile for the Passage Comprehension test (from WJ-III; Woodcock et al. 2001) and a Sentence Picture Matching functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) task in 7–9 year old children using DTI. This age range was selected because children were part of a larger longitudinal study that enabled us to collect the WJ-III Passage Comprehension percentiles from them in the final year of scanning and testing. This is an interesting age range because during this developmental period, children are transitioning from the stage of “learning to read” to “reading to learn” (Chall 1983). Namely, they master technical reading and start developing reading comprehension skills (Chall 1983). So we expect to see the emergence of right hemisphere comprehension abilities, activity, and connectivity. We hypothesized that Regions of Interest (ROIs) resulting from the voxel-wise correlation of DTI white matter FA (focusing on the dorsal pathway, SLF and the ventral pathway, ILF) with reading comprehension measures (i.e., percentiles from the Passage Comprehension task) also would be part of the activation observed in response to the fMRI Sentence Picture Matching task. We also hypothesized that in addition to the involvement of the right hemisphere in the Sentence Picture Matching task, ROIs related to reading comprehension would show greater functional connectivity in the right hemisphere as compared to the homologous ROIs in the left hemisphere.

Methods

Participants

Twenty three healthy children (8.5 ± 0.81 years old, 8 males) participated in the current study. All children were right-handed with an average to above average IQ (107.81 ± 13.84) as measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV) test (Wechsler 2003).

Behavioral measures

Reading comprehension was measured using the Passage Comprehension subtest from the WJ-III (Woodcock et al. 2001). In this test, participants are required to read a sentence or paragraph and supply a missing word that is implied by the context of the text. Within the range of 7–9 years of age, typical readers may demonstrate different levels of reading comprehension skills (see Chall’s model for reading acquisition; Chall 1983). To compare the level of age-appropriate reading comprehension skills across all participants, we calculated the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest based on the age norms from the WJ-III battery. An independent t-test analysis revealed no significant difference between boys and girls in Passage Comprehension percentiles (t(21) =−0.034, P > 0.05).

Imaging data - diffusion tensor imaging data acquisition and processing

DTI data were acquired using single-shot spin-echo, echo planar imaging with TR/TE = 8800/88 ms, 61 gradient directions, and b-value of 1000 s/mm2. Images were pre-processed in FSL (FMRIB, Oxford, UK) including correction for eddy currents and head motion. Tensor decomposition was done using FDT (FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox) (Behrens et al. 2003) for generating FA indices. Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS) (Smith et al. 2006) was then used to prepare the individual diffusion maps for voxel-based group analysis by performing the following steps: all FA images were nonlinearly registered to a template of averaged FA images (FMRIB-58) in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space with 1 mm resolution using FNIRT (FMRIB, Oxford, UK). It has been shown previously that the MNI adult framework is adequate for co-registration of DTI data from adolescent and child brains (Wilke et al. 2003; Wang et al. 2012).

After transformation into MNI space, a cohort mean-FA image was created and thinned to generate a cohort mean-FA skeleton of the white matter tracts. Each subject’s aligned FA image was then projected onto the cohort mean-FA skeleton by filling that with FA values from the nearest relevant tract center, which was achieved by searching perpendicular to the local skeleton structure for maximum value. A second localized co-registration step helped alleviate alignment problems (see Fig. 1 for this procedure). Then, two white matter tracts, ILF and SLF, were defined as ROIs to further constrain the voxel-wise TBSS analysis in these two tracts in each hemisphere (Yeatman et al. 2012) (see Fig. 2 for the ILF and SLF masks).

Fig. 1
figure 1

The TBSS pipeline

Fig. 2
figure 2

Masks for the right and left ILF (red) and SLF (blue). Masks for the right and left ILF (red) and SLF (blue). The masks were created based on the JHU white matter tractography atlas. Masks presented in radiological orientation (L = R/R = L)

Statistical analysis for the DTI correlation with the percentiles for the passage comprehension subtest

After the projection of each subject’s FA skeleton to the group mean-FA skeleton and following the second co-registration, we correlated the mean FA measures from these ROIs from each individual with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest from WJ-III. On a voxel-wise basis within each ROI, correlation of FA with percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest was measured and tested for statistical significance by a non-parametric permutation-based method provided by the RANDOMIZE algorithm (Nichols and Holmes 2002) using 5000 permutations with Threshold-Free Cluster Enhancement (TFCE) (Smith et al. 2007). Using the Harvard-Oxford atlas implemented in FSL, we extracted the nearest grey matter point (and the corresponding BA) for each cluster found in the ILF and SLF ROI that reached a significance level of P < 0.005, uncorrected for multiple comparisons among voxels. These Brodmann areas were then used as ROIs in functional connectivity analysis of fMRI data acquired during a Sentence Picture Matching task. It is important to note that each BA ROI in the functional connectivity analysis may correspond to multiple proximal DTI sub-clusters.

Functional MRI sentence picture matching task

Five cycles of alternating blocks of sentences and words (64 s per block) were presented for a total scan time of 10 min 40 s; 10 blocks overall. The “stimulated” condition of the sentences involved auditory presentation of a simple active or passive sentence simultaneously with visual presentation of a picture of two objects either acting on each other (e.g., “the dog is licking the cat”) or in a spatial relationship (e.g., “the book is on the table”). During the control condition, single nouns were presented auditorily and repeated four times so that noun stimuli were similar in duration to sentence stimuli, and a picture of a single object was presented (designed to occupy roughly the same degree of the visual field as the two-object pictures). Participants were asked, for both conditions, to make a button-press response indicating if the auditory stimulus accurately described the picture. Each block included 15 trials (4250 ms each), and these blocks were counterbalanced for yes/no responses and sentence type. See Fig. 3 for task details. The participants’ behavioral responses were recorded, and performance scores (i.e., the percentage of correct responses) were calculated in order to ensure that the task was understood.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The functional MRI Semantic task

Functional MRI data acquisition and processing

Data for the Sentence Picture Matching task was acquired using a combined Whole-Brain Functional ASL/BOLD Gradient-Echo EPI Acquisition. MRI data was obtained using a Philips 3 T Achieva system and a 32-channel head coil. For the ASL/BOLD acquisitions, a double-excitation approach was used, which has been determined both theoretically and empirically to be more optimal as compared to double-echo acquisitions or other methods (Schmithorst et al. 2014). However, only standard BOLD acquisitions were included in the present analysis. Acquisition parameters were: TR = 4000 ms, TE1 = 11 ms, TE2 = 35 ms, matrix = 64 × 64, FOV = 25.6 × 25.6 cm, slice thickness = 5 mm, 25 slices acquired covering the whole brain.

Participants were acclimated and desensitized to the scanner (see Byars et al. 2002 and Vannest et al. in press). Prior to study visits and in order to prepare the child for the study visit, the parents and child watched a video describing the MRI as an adventure in space. This video is available on our project website https://research.cchmc.org/c-mind/visitors/preparing. Specific procedures were used at each visit to acclimate the child to the scanner environment. Before the scanning session, children practiced the tasks in a mock scanner that included scanner sounds. Children also were offered to move the actual scanner bed up and down and to sit on the bed. Tasks were practiced both on a computer outside of the scanner as well as inside the scanner before image acquisition. Children were introduced with a framed picture of a scanned brain during motion as compared to a framed picture of a scanned brain where the participant was lying still. They also practiced a position of “lying still as a statue” within the scanner (meaning, lying still without moving their head, legs, and arms). Head motion was further minimized using elastic straps that were attached to either side of the head-coil apparatus.

Functional MRI data analysis

First-level fMRI data processing was carried out using FEAT (FMRI Expert Analysis Tool) Version 6.0, which is a part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library, www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl) involving motion correction, spatial smoothing, and coregistration to an age-appropriate pediatric template (template for 4–18 year olds, for details see: https://research.cchmc.org/c-mind-db/py-doc/html/pipelines_struct.html#registration-to-the-study-template) that was then normalized to 2 mm MNI standard space using symmetric diffeomorphic image normalization (SyN) (Avants et al. 2008) as implemented in ANTs (Advanced Normalization Tools, stnava.github.io/ANTs/). Independent motion correction of the ASL and BOLD time series (to a common reference frame) was carried out using MCFLIRT (Jenkinson et al. 2002). In an initial run, the central time point was used as the reference frame. Based on the derived motion parameters, the time point with the minimal displacement from the average position of the brain over the time series was determined. Motion correction was then restarted, using this minimal displacement time point as the reference frame. Following motion correction, spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of FWHM 8 mm and high-pass temporal filtering (cutoff of 104 s) were applied. Grand-mean intensity normalization by a single multiplicative factor was applied to each 4D dataset.

To verify involvement of the right hemisphere in the Sentence Picture Matching task even in the absence of a priori selection of ROIs (i.e., for a whole-brain analysis), a general linear model analysis was performed. A general linear model with autocorrelation correction was used to compute BOLD and ASL activation at the single-subject level. For the present analysis, only BOLD data was considered (comparison with ASL is beyond the scope of this paper – See Schmithorst et al. 2014). Sentences and words conditions were then modeled and parameter maps were generated for the contrast between sentences and words (sentences > words).

For the second-level analysis, group-level analyses were carried out using a random-effects ordinary least-squares model. To control for the percentage of false-positive active voxels within each activated cluster, we employed a False Discovery Rate (FDR) correction with a threshold of P < 0.05 (also see Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). Since the current study focuses on comprehension beyond the single-word level, our analysis was focused only on the sentences > words contrast.

Functional connectivity analysis

Our regions of interest for functional connectivity analysis were selected by first locating the nearest grey matter points to the white matter regions (in the right and left ILF and SLF) where FA was significantly correlated with percentile scores for the Passage Comprehension subtest. Then, we chose the Brodmann Areas where these grey matter points were located as ROIs, for functional connectivity analysis (see also Statistical analysis for the DTI correlation with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest). To represent the linear temporal association between the ROIs, bivariate regression coefficient values were estimated using the CONN fMRI connectivity toolbox in SPM8 (v14, http://www.nitrc.org/projects/conn; Whitfield-Gabrieli and Nieto-Castanon 2012). This toolbox implements a component-based noise correction method (CompCor) for reducing physiological and other nuisance effects (i.e., white matter and CSF effects, sentence and word tasks’ effects and their first temporal derivatives, and motion effects), allowing group inferences to be made using random effects in a second-level analysis. Bivariate correlation (i.e., connectivity) from the sentences condition for each participant was correlated with the participant’s performance score during the task (i.e., the overall performance percentage that reflects whether the participant provided a correct or erroneous response during the task), such that the correlation coefficients (β) represent a performance weighted within-group measure of functional connectivity between ROIs. Only connections that survived the significance threshold for functional connectivity and behavioral measures were included in the results (P < 0.0005 FDR-corrected, two-sided; see Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). An intensity value for each ROI was defined as the number of significant connections made to all other ROIs (representing the degree of connectivity).

Results

Behavioral measures

Reading comprehension scores (from the Passage Comprehension subtest)

Average percentile for the Passage Comprehension WJ-III: 50.87 % ± 22.36 %.

Behavioral scores for the Sentence Picture Matching task (performance scores)

Results from the average correct responses for the sentences condition showed 86.0 % ± 10.7 % accuracy.

Imaging measures

Diffusion tensor imaging measures

Clusters of white matter voxels in the right and left ILF and SLF with moderate positive correlation between the FA values and the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest were found (P < 0.005, uncorrected). Percentiles for Passage Comprehension were positively correlated with FA values in both the right and left ILF and SLF. See Table 1 and Fig. 4 for more information.

Table 1 Regions within the ILF and SLF corresponding with the correlation between FA values and Passage Comprehension scores (P < 0.005, uncorrected). Regions were defined using the Harvard-Oxford atlas in FSL. Coordinates are reported in MNI space
Fig. 4
figure 4

Illustration of the major correlation between the FA values right and left in SLF and ILF with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension task. Right and left ILF (red) and SLF (blue). Tracts were defined based on the JHU white matter atlas. Data is presented in Radiological orientation (L = R/R = L). a Three dimensional tractography results demonstrating right lateralization of correlation of FA values with percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest (P < .005, uncorrected). Axial, coronal, and sagittal views are shown at MNI coordinates x = 12, y = -28, z = 0. b Three dimensional tractography results demonstrating right lateralization of correlation of FA values with percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest (P < .005, uncorrected). Axial, coronal, and sagittal views are shown at MNI coordinates x = 47, y =−74, z = 44

General linear model for the sentence picture matching task

The composite map for the fMRI task reveals activation clusters within key language regions as listed in Table 1 (left and right frontal poles, inferior frontal gyri, middle temporal gyri, angular gyri, and fusiform gyri; P < 0.05, FDR-corrected). See Fig. 5 for the activation of the sentences > words contrast and Table 2 for these regions.

Fig. 5
figure 5

GLM analysis for the Sentence Picture Matching task (sentences > words). GLM second-level analysis (sentences > words), P < 0.05, FDR-corrected (Z = 2.3–6.2), Data presented in radiological orientation (R = L, L = R). The red color represents the data for sentences > words contrast. Slices shown are Z = 27–71

Table 2 Coordinates, Brodmann areas and cluster sizes for the second-level analysis for the Sentence Picture Matching task (sentences > words), P < 0.05-FDR corrected. x, y, and z coordinates correspond to the center of each cluster. Regions were defined using the Harvard-Oxford atlas in FSL

Functional connectivity for the sentence picture matching task

Based on the clusters found in the correlation of the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest with the FA values in the selected white matter tract ROIs, 19 ROIs were included in the functional connectivity analysis (11 ROIs in the right hemisphere and 8 ROIs in the left hemisphere). Participants showed significant positive performance-related functional connectivity between the chosen ROIs, at P < 0.0005, FDR corrected. A trend of greater intensity values for right lateralized ROIs compared to the intensity of their left homologues was observed. Despite this trend, a paired t-test analysis comparing the intensity of ROIs that had homologous ROIs (i.e., BAs 10, 19, 22, 41) did not reveal a statistically significant difference (P > .05). See Fig. 6 and Table 3 for details.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Functional connectivity between the selected ROIs for the sentences condition. Highest functional connectivity (strongest connections >10) is colored in red, mild in orange, lowest in yellow (<5 connections), P < 0.0005, FDR-corrected. Data presented in neurological orientation (R = R, L = L). ROIs were defined based on the Talairach-Daemon atlas implemented in the CONN toolbox

Table 3 Weight of each network for the selected RO (seeds), for the functional connectivity analysis (sentence condition only), P < 0.0005, FDR-corrected. ROIs were defined using the Talairach-Daemon atlas in SPM 8

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to provide neurobiological support for the utilization of the right hemisphere in reading comprehension through the use of structural and functional connectivity. Our results indicate a significant correlation of FA values in SLF and ILF with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest from WJ-III, and with a greater distribution in the right hemisphere, which adds an interesting laterality aspect to the Freiderici model (2012). While the GLM maps for the Sentence Picture Matching task fMRI task showed a bilateral activation in language-related regions during the sentence processing vs words contrast, the functional connectivity results showed a trend of greater intensity for right-lateralized ROIs as compared to the left homologues. This is a novel and important finding guided by the underlying white matter structure defined by the DTI results correlating the FA values from the chosen white matter tracts (right and left ILF and SLF) with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest from the WJ-III.

The results of our study confirm the hypothesis that reading comprehension relies mainly on the right hemisphere, even in children as young as 7–9 years of age. This is reflected by the significant correlation we determined between FA values in SLF and ILF and the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest, specifically in the right hemisphere. These results are in line with our previous analysis in adolescents ages 15 to 19 (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014). Using TBSS voxel-wise analysis, we demonstrated positive correlations between regional FA values in the right and left Arcuate Fasciculus (AF) and the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest, with a robust correlation in the right hemisphere. A left-lateralized correlation between the ILF and a word recognition measure [the Test of Word Recognition (i.e., TOWRE-2; Torgesen et al. 2012)] also was previously found (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014). There, we suggested that the AF supports reading comprehension due to its wide distribution among reading-related regions (see Yeatman et al. 2012). Regions of the right frontal lobe connected to posterior temporal parietal language areas are an inherent part of an effective reading comprehension circuit in typical reading, and are connected through the AF (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014). In the current study, we demonstrated a significant correlation of percentile score for the Passage Comprehension test with FA values in specific other parts of the right SLF. Previous studies have shown that the AF is the last white matter tract supporting language to myelinate (Su et al. 2008), which may explain the absence of a significant correlation in the current study since it included relatively young children (7–9 years in the current analysis vs 15–19 years in Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014). However, our results do confirm that the role of the right hemisphere in reading comprehension begins as early in life as at the age of 7. Interestingly, it seems that the utilization of the right hemisphere for reading comprehension continues into late adolescence, while engaging different brain regions as well as extending the connections between reading regions in the right hemisphere also occur in 15–19 year-old children (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014). A future longitudinal study comparing the correlation between FA values and percentile scores for the Passage Comprehension test across development should verify this point.

Interestingly, in contrast to the trend of greater functional connectivity values in the majority of the right ROIs vs the left ROIs in the functional connectivity analysis, BA 19 showed a trend of greater intensity in the left hemisphere rather than in the right (118.64 vs 109.57, respectively). Two other closely related regions showed significant functional connectivity only in the left hemisphere: left BAs 18 and 37 as well as left BA 7. Interestingly, these regions in the left hemisphere are key elements of the ventral occipito-temporal stream, also known as the Visual Word-Form Area (VWFA; see Turkeltaub et al. 2003). A future study should verify if the role of the left BAs 18, 19 and 37 in reading comprehension may be due to the technical reading component in reading comprehension. Moreover, the role of left BA 7 was thought to be related to visual attention, which is a crucial component in technical reading (Vogel et al. 2014). A future study should specifically examine the functional connectivity of the left BAs 18, 19, 37 and 7 with regions related to technical reading and reading comprehension in the right hemisphere to reveal the role of the left hemisphere in comprehension rather than in technical reading.

The group random-effects GLM analysis for sentences contrasted with words in the Sentence Picture Matching task (Fig. 5) shows bilateral activation of language regions, whereas the functional connectivity base showed a trend of greater right lateralization. This likely is due to the involvement of additional language processes for comprehension as well as syntactic and semantic processing while participating in the Sentence Picture Matching task. Bilateral activation in the STG previously has been observed in adults in response to semantically and syntactically violated sentences (Friederici et al. 2003) as well as during a narrative comprehension task along development (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2013). The fMRI task used in the current study involved a semantic and syntactic decision through an auditory processing pathway that tends to engage bilateral activation. Our data support the bilateral utilization of two white matter tracts underlies the STG (SLF, ILF) associated with percentiles for the Passage Comprehension subtest. The bilateral activation of the STG and the frontal operculum during the Sentence Picture Matching task was also observed in the GLM analyses. However, no significant connectivity in the pars opercularis or other regions of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) was found for the Sentence Picture Matching task. Combining the absence of this finding in comparison with our previous study (Horowitz-Kraus et al. 2014) suggests that the most frontal regions of the SLF are not utilized in language comprehension by younger children. The significant correlation of the middle temporal gyrus (MTG), the angular gyrus (AG), and the orbitoprefrontal cortex (oPFC) with Passage Comprehension scores, as well as the evidenced connectivity of these regions during the fMRI task, corresponds with previous reports of activation of these regions in sentence processing (Friederici 2009).

Conclusions

The current study strengthens the findings of previous studies demonstrating an association between right hemisphere activation and increased narrative comprehension (George et al. 1999; Xu et al. 2005), as well as studies highlighting the inability of those with right hemisphere damage to comprehend text in a narrative context (Delis et al. 1983; Hough 1990). It also highlights the involvement of the right hemisphere in language comprehension, which may also support reading comprehension skill (as suggested by our DTI findings). This may provide a future neuroanatomical reference for differentiating children with reading difficulties (i.e., dyslexia) from children with reading comprehension problems (i.e., SLI) and may serve as an objective tool to evaluate the effectiveness of reading intervention programs on the technical reading vs reading comprehension domains.

Limitations of the study

The findings of the current study should be interpreted with several limitations in mind. First, differences between functional connectivity in ROIs in the right hemisphere vs the homologous ROIs in the left hemisphere did not reach significance, and therefore our conclusion of qualitatively greater right lateralization related to reading comprehension should be further examined. This fact poses a critical limitation to our conclusions of greater right structural and functional connectivity being related to better reading comprehension abilities. We suspect that in a larger or older group of children or adults that are more homogeneous in their reading level (i.e., 16–18 year olds or young adults), we might obtain more conclusive and powerful results as to the laterality of reading comprehension. In line with that, due to the relatively small group of children in this study, the results of our current DTI data did not reach significance after correction for multiple comparisons. Again, we suggest that a future study enrolling a larger number of participants may result in greater power and will survive multiple-comparisons correction. Secondly, although all children participating in the study reported intact reading, we did not assess the technical reading of our participants, so we cannot demonstrate the distinct role of the right hemisphere in reading comprehension in these children. Third, when including a priori ROIs in an analysis such as those we included in the functional connectivity analysis (and also in the structural connectivity analysis using DTI) that is the same limitation for every hypothesis (ROI)-driven analysis, may lose important information regarding other regions that are involved in the process, but were not included in the analysis. An alternative approach would be to perform the functional connectivity analysis first and then choose the white matter tracts corresponding with these regions for the structural connectivity correlation with the percentiles for the Passage Comprehension task.