Introduction

Organizational justice or fairness perception is critical in organizational behavior studies (Fischer et al., 2011) and has been found to be vital to a wide range of employee and organizational effectiveness outcomes (Colquitt et al., 2001) and to employee health (Greenberg, 2010). For example, a study showed that perceived fairness in the organization, such as fair treatment by the organization or supervisor, is an antecedent of employees’ loyalty to and voice within the organization (Cropanzano et al., 2007). Equity theory explains that justice is essential to employees’ organizational life because the perception of injustice or unfairness motivates individuals to behave to make things fair (Adams, 1965; Greenberg, 1987). Almost all studies have focused on the fairness perception of employees, and very few have examined the fairness perception of supervisors (with the rare exception of Liu et al., 2017), especially supervisors who perceived unfair treatment by their subordinates.

As supervisors have higher formal power over their subordinates (Yukl, 2002), researchers might consider it more likely for supervisors to be unfairly treated by the organization or higher-level supervisors than by their subordinates (Liu et al., 2017). However, supervisors could be mistreated by their subordinates (Cheng, 1995; Elangovan & Shapriro, 1998) and might react in certain ways to such unfair treatment (Tepper et al., 2006). In the workplace, subordinates might show disrespect to their supervisor or form a small group to bargain with the supervisor, and supervisors might even be betrayed by their trusted subordinates. Supervisors might react with abuse or authoritarianism to restore their sense of justice and status (Farh & Cheng, 2000; Tepper et al., 2006).

Revenge behavior involves lowering the offender’s resource or status to get even and follows the rule of retributive justice (Stuckless & Goranson, 1992). Although revenge is the quickest way to get even, this behavior could be very inappropriate for an effective leader (Tripp et al., 2007). Forgiveness is withholding the right to revenge and showing compassion and empathy toward the offender, which follows the principle of restorative justice (Bradfield & Aquino, 1999; Wenzel et al., 2008). Fehr and Gelfand (2012) showed that forgiveness can promote positive social exchange and enhance group effectiveness. Thus, we investigated the relationship between supervisors’ perception of unfair treatment by a subordinate (PUTS) and supervisors’ revenge and forgiveness. Based on the social exchange perspective (Blau, 1964), supervisors’ forgiveness promotes positive exchange and fosters cohesion and trust (Tyler & Blader, 2003), and supervisors’ revenge leads to negative exchange and distrust (Greco et al., 2019). Accordingly, this study proposes that supervisors’ revenge and forgiveness mediates the relationship between PUTS and employee effectiveness, considered as job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior (Lam et al., 2014; Owens et al., 2016).

Supervisors’ affective organizational commitment refers to their emotional attachment to the organization, which might increase the positive relationship between PUTS and revenge reactions. According to the altruistic punishment perspective (Fehr & Gachter, 2002), supervisors might feel responsible for an organizational member’s deviant behavior and use punishing behavior to make the member cooperative. Thus, this study explores the moderating effect of supervisor affective commitment on the relationship between PUTS and supervisor revenge.

The need to understand justice and fairness within law enforcement organizations is reflected in an increasing number of studies (Reynolds et al., 2018; Trinkner & Cohn, 2014; Trinkner et al., 2018). As the relationship and interaction between supervisors and subordinates is critical in law enforcement organizations (Wolfe et al., 2018), this study investigated the relationship of interest in these organizations.

Supervisors’ Perception of Unfair Treatment by a Subordinate (PUTS)

Although workplace justice has been investigated for several decades, a review by Colquitt et al. (2001) showed that most studies have focused on the employee perspective. Studies have demonstrated that employee justice perception can promote job performance, organizational commitment, job satisfaction, and organizational citizenship behavior (OCB; for a meta-analysis, see Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001). OCB is a discretionary behavior by organizational members that can benefit the organization, such as voice behavior and helping colleagues (Organ, 1988). Supervisors’ perception of fair treatment in the organization has rarely been studied. One recent study found that supervisors’ perception of unfair treatment by the organization influenced how they interacted with their subordinates, which lowered the subordinates’ prosocial and voice behavior (Liu et al., 2017). Police subcultures can exhibit unfair practices, such as brutality, authoritarianism, extreme conservatism, prejudice, and racism (Sayles & Albritton, 1999), which might make the interaction between supervisors and police officers less respectful and fair. In this way, supervisors could be unfairly treated by their subordinates (i.e., police officers) in the workplace of law enforcement organizations.

Equity theory proposes that when individuals feel that they have been treated unfairly, they are motivated to behave to decrease the feeling of unfairness (Adams, 1965). A subordinate might not reciprocate the supervisor’s support and benevolence (Cheng, 1995), might betray the supervisor’s trust (Elangovan & Shapiro, 1998), or might intentionally behave in a disrespectful manner in front of the supervisor (Pearson & Porath, 2005), thus giving the supervisor a perception of being unfairly treated by the subordinate. In response to PUTS, supervisors might take revenge or show leniency. Several items used in measures of leadership describe possible reactions to PUTS: for example, “my supervisor puts me down in front of others” (abusive supervision; Tepper, 2000), “my supervisor avenges a personal wrong in the name of public interest when he/she is offended” (moral leadership; Cheng et al., 2004), “my supervisor criticizes and blames outgroups in public” (differential leadership; Jiang et al., 2014), and “my supervisor turns a blind eye to ingroups’ mistakes” (differential leadership; Jiang et al., 2014). The above leadership studies have demonstrated that supervisors respond in ways that involve forgiveness or revenge.

Forgiveness and Revenge

Fair policing refers to police officers being willing to listen to citizens’ perspectives and treat citizens respectfully (Haas et al., 2015), for which the perception of justice between supervisors and police officers is critical (Bradford & Quinton, 2014). Furthermore, supervisors’ behavior sends crucial signals to officers regarding the moral standards of the organization (Rothstein & Stolle, 2008; Van Craen & Skogan, 2017). Therefore, supervisors’ responses to the PUTS might be influential on employee behavior, which might lead to fair policing. The responses of revenge and forgiveness reflect two perspectives of recovering justice: respectively, retributive justice and restorative justice (Goodstein & Butterfield, 2010; Strelan et al., 2008; Wenzel et al., 2008). Revenge is when someone who perceives they have been treated unfairly acts in a harmful manner in return (Stuckless & Goranson, 1992). In contrast, forgiveness is defined as reacting with compassion, benevolence, and caring, even when a person has the right to revenge (Bradfield & Aquino, 1999).

The nature of retributive justice is punishment, which involves depleting the offender’s resources, such as money, status, prestige, or freedom, to restore the balance the power between parties (Strelan et al., 2008). Reynolds et al. (2018) reported that police officers engaged in production deviance as a form of retaliation against perceived unfair treatment, such as reduced output by doing the bare minimum (Reynolds et al., 2018). The study of Aquino et al. (2006) indicated that people with greater power were more likely to act in a revengeful manner when facing unfair events. Supervisors have formal power over their subordinates and are less likely to be punished by exerting neglectful, critical, or less supportive behavior as revenge (Kremer & Stephens, 1983; Hodgins et al., 1996). Vengeance by supervisors could help them to regain a sense of justice and provide the opportunity to give feedback on subordinates’ lack of reciprocity behavior (Bies et al., 1997; Tripp & Bies, 2009). Supervisors might behave abusively, such as by engaging in hostile verbal and nonverbal behaviors (Tepper, 2000), and the long-term physiological and psychological tension inherent in police work can lead police officers to respond to minor conflicts or threats in a more aggressive manner (Griffin & Bernard, 2003). Furthermore, a recent study indicated that abusive supervision could temporarily enhance subordinates’ work engagement (Qin et al., 2018), potentially prompting supervisors who hold this belief to retaliate in a vengeful manner. Thus, supervisors might be encouraged or less hesitant to enact revenge when they experience PUTS. Similarly, supervisors might be less likely to show forgiveness when encountering PUTS.

  • H1: PUTS is positively associated with supervisor revenge behavior.

  • H2: PUTS is negatively associated with supervisor forgiveness behavior.

Supervisors’ Reactions and Employee Effectiveness

Social exchange theory proposes that social components, such as trust, friendship, and commitment, can be exchanged between individuals (Blau, 1964). Individuals who receive a favor from another feel obligated to reciprocate this favor, in a process called positive reciprocity (Greco et al., 2019). Whereas continuing positive social exchange can contribute to a high-quality relationship (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995), negative social components, such as hate and dislike, promote negative social exchange processes that can worsen the relationship (Greco et al., 2019). Accordingly, although revenge might make supervisors feel better and establish authority, it might trigger negative reciprocity, worsen the relationship between the supervisor and the subordinate, and influence employee effectiveness (Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007; Priesemuth et al., 2014; Qin et al., 2018). Studies on abusive supervision and authoritarian leadership have consistently shown that hostile leader behavior negatively influences elements of employee effectiveness, such as job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior (Farh & Cheng, 2000; Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007).

Restorative justice focuses on mutual benefit and long-term relationships rather than short-term benefit (Bradfield & Aquino, 1999; Exline et al., 2003; Fehr & Gelfand, 2012). Supervisors’ forgiveness could promote a positive reciprocity process (Greco et al., 2019; Witvliet et al., 2002). By expressing compassion and benevolence, a forgiving leader could make subordinates feel understanding and supportive and willing to contribute more to work to reciprocate the supervisor’s kindness (Farh & Cheng, 2000; Jiang & Cheng, 2008). Studies have indicated that supportive and benevolent leadership can promote many elements of employee effectiveness, such as job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior (Cheng et al., 2015; Podsakoff et al., 1996; Wu et al., 2012).

This study investigates three core elements of employee effectiveness: job performance (Owens et al., 2016), cooperative behavior (Tyler & Blader, 2003), and proactive behavior (Lam et al., 2014). Job performance, also called in-role performance, indicates the extent to which an employee accomplishes his/her duty correctly, efficiently, and effectively (Cheng et al., 2003). Cooperative behavior indicates the extent to which an employee complies with the rules and regulations of the organization and the group (Tyler & Blader, 2003). Employees’ proactive behavior, which refers to actively finding and resolving problems in the workplace, is critical to employee creativity and innovation (Lam et al., 2014). Recent studies have shown that these three outcomes are critical indicators of employee effectiveness when examined in relation with justice in the workplace (Colquitt et al., 2001; Tyler & Blader, 2003, 2013; Zhang et al., 2014). The study of Reynolds et al. (2018) showed that when police officers were not treated fairly, they behaved in a deviant and self-protective manner, exhibiting lower job performance, a reluctance to cooperate, and a tendency to lay low. Paoline III and Gau’s (2020) qualitative study indicated that when police officers felt supported, appreciated, and encouraged by their supervisors, they had high job satisfaction and promoted positive work behaviors. Forgiving supervisors demonstrate highly supportive behavior toward their subordinates. Thus, in line with the generalized social exchange perspective, we propose the following hypotheses:

  • H3: Supervisor revenge behavior is negatively associated with employee effectiveness (job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior).

  • H4: Supervisor forgiveness behavior is positively associated with employee effectiveness (job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior).

The Potential Mediation Effect of Supervisors’ Revenge and Forgiveness

PUTS might indirectly influence employee effectiveness through the mediation effect of supervisors’ revenge and forgiveness. From the social exchange perspective, restorative justice emphasizes the long-term relationship, which indicates a high-quality social exchange relationship (Blau, 1964; Greco et al., 2019), whereas retributive justice requires short-term fairness without considering positive social components or even introduces harmful social components as a form of revenge (Hodgins et al., 1996; Kremer & Stephens, 1983). Accordingly, PUTS might hamper employee effectiveness by increasing supervisors’ revenge and decreasing supervisors’ forgiveness. We therefore propose the fifth and sixth hypotheses as follows:

  • H5: Supervisor revenge mediates the relationship between PUTS and employee effectiveness.

  • H6: Supervisor forgiveness mediates the relationship between PUTS and employee effectiveness.

The Moderating Effect of Supervisor Affective Commitment

Tyler and Blader’s (2003) proposed group engagement model posits that groups use sanctions to promote cooperation and engagement among their members. The question remains, however, of which group member is responsible for the sanctioning decision and behavior. Organ (1988) argued that a committed employee would be proud of being an organizational member and act like a corporate soldier, protecting the organization’s benefit. Mayer and Allen (1991) and O’Reilly and Chatman (1986) proposed that affective organizational commitment represents an employee’s attachment to the organization, which means that the individual identifies and internalizes the organization’s vision and values. Thus, a committed supervisor might be more likely than one who is less committed to prioritize the welfare of the organization and the workgroup. This proposition is supported by recent findings that committed employees exhibit high levels of voice and proactive behavior (Loi et al., 2012; Parker et al., 2006; Van Dyne & LePine, 1998).

A committed supervisor will focus on the cohesiveness of the workgroup and might see PUTS as deviant behavior toward herself/himself and the workgroup as a whole. Fehr and Gächter (2002) proposed that group members engage in altruistic punishment, which involves punishing uncooperative members to maintain the group’s cohesiveness. This means that a committed supervisor might be prone to enact revenge for the workgroup’s sake. The same study showed that uncooperative members behaved cooperatively after their punishment (Fehr & Gachter, 2002). Thus, according to the altruistic punishment perspective, we expect that supervisors with high affective commitment to the organization tend to emphasize the benefit of the workgroup and the organization, and thus might be more likely to enact revenge for PUTS as they see it as a threat to the cohesiveness of the workgroup and the organization. As the altruistic punishment perspective explains punishment behavior within a group, supervisor affective commitment is less likely to moderate the relationship between PUTS and forgiveness. Accordingly, our final hypothesis is as follows and Fig. 1 shows the hypothesized study framework:

Fig. 1
figure 1

Proposed relationship framework of the study

  • H7: Supervisor affective commitment moderates the relationship between PUTS and supervisor revenge, such that the positive relationship is greater when supervisor affective commitment is high than when it is low.

Methods

A group-based dyadic questionnaire survey was administered in January 2022 to 120 supervisors and 681 subordinates from a Taiwanese law enforcement organization. Ninety-three supervisor questionnaires and 389 subordinate questionnaires were returned, for a response rate of 83.03% and 57.12%, respectively. Approximately 92% of the supervisors and 83% of the subordinates were male. The mean number of subordinates under a supervisor was 4.18. The mean subordinate age was 41.46 years (SD = 10.81) and the mean supervisor age was 50.75 years (SD = 6.82). The mean supervisor and subordinate coworking period was 2.05 years (SD = 2.72). This study was approved by the institutional review board of the National Chung-Cheng University (CCUREC110051801).

Procedure

The researchers contacted a Taiwan law enforcement organization to invite them to join this study, and the organization accepted the proposal. Questionnaires with guidance were pre-coded to ensure anonymity and delivered with the help of the organization’s staff. After completing the questionnaire, the participants sealed it in an envelope prepared in advance, and the researchers collected the sealed questionnaires directly from the participants.

Measures

The translation–back-translation procedure was used to create a Traditional Chinese version of all measures (Brislin, 1986). The supervisor questionnaire comprised three sections, measuring PUTS (11 items), affective commitment (6 items), and demographic variables. The subordinate questionnaire comprised five sections, measuring the supervisor’s revenge and forgiveness behavior (11 items), job performance (4 items), cooperative behavior (15 items), proactive behavior (10 items), and demographic variables. All items were rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree.

Perceived Unfair Treatment by a Subordinate (PUTS)

The measure of PUTS was adapted from the justice perception measure of Niehoff and Moorman (1993) and the betrayal measure of Grégorie and Fisher (2008) and consists of an 11-item scale. Sample items are “When this subordinate reports difficulties with work tasks to me, he/she is honest and transparent without deception” (reversed coded), “I feel deceived by this subordinate,” and “I feel betrayed by this subordinate.” The supervisors were asked to rate each of their subordinates on this scale. Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.91.

Supervisor Affective Commitment

Affective commitment was reported by the supervisor on a measure adapted from the 6-item affective organizational commitment scale of Meyer at el. (1993). Sample items are “I would be very happy to spend the rest of my career with this organization” and “I really feel as if this organization’s problems are my own.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale is 0.84.

Supervisor Revenge

For the revenge measure, six items on revenge from Bradfield and Aquino (1999) were modified by changing the referent from “I” to “My supervisor” and the target from “them” to “me.” Sample items are “When I did something bad to my supervisor, he/she supervisor tried to make something bad happen to me,” “When I hurt my supervisor, he/she did something to make me pay,” and “When I didn’t treat my supervisor fairly, he/she got even with me.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.95.

Supervisor Forgiveness

For the forgiveness measure, five items from Bradfield and Aquino (1999) were modified by changing the referent from “I” to “My supervisor” and the target from “them” to “me.” Sample items are “When I made mistakes, my supervisor gave me back a new start, a renewed relationship,” “My supervisor accepted my humanness, flaws, and failures,” and “Even when I made mistakes, my supervisor did his/her best to put aside the mistrust.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.96.

Job Performance

The job performance measure was adapted from Farh and Cheng’s (1997) 4-item measure. Sample items are “I make an important contribution to the overall performance of our work unit” and “I am one of the excellent employees in our work unit.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.90.

Cooperative Behavior

The cooperative behavior measure was adapted from Tyler and Blader (2001) and comprised an 8-item compliance measure and a 7-item deference measure. Sample items are “I follow work rules,” “I comply with company regulations,” and “Even if no one is around, I execute my supervisor’s order accurately.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.80.

Proactive Behavior

The proactive behavior measure was adapted from Grant et al. (2009). This 10-item measure consists of three domains: voice, rational issue-selling, and taking charge. Sample items are “gets involved in issues that affect the quality of work life here in the group,” “goes about changing your mind to get you to agree with them,” and “tries to implement solutions to pressing organizational problems.” Cronbach’s alpha of this scale was 0.96.

Control Variables

Supervisors’ and subordinates’ age and gender were included as control variables. This study also controlled the effect of the coworking period with the supervisor.

Results

Before examining the proposed hypothesis, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to demonstrate discriminant validity among the studied variables. The confirmatory factor analysis showed a good fit for a six-factor model comprising PUTS, supervisor forgiveness, supervisor revenge, supervisor revenge, job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior, χ2 = 209.27, df = 120, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) = 0.04, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.98, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = 0.98, standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) = 0.03. Table 1 shows the confirmatory factor analysis results and indicates that the six-factor solution had the best fit.

Table 1 Results of confirmatory factory analyses

Table 2 shows the correlations among the studied variables. PUTS was negatively associated with supervisor forgiveness (r = −.27, p < .01) and positively associated with supervisor revenge (r = .12, p < .05). Supervisor forgiveness was positively related to job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior (rs = 0.22–0.33, p < .01). Supervisor revenge was negatively related to job performance (r = −.15, p < .01) (Table 2).

Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and correlation coefficients among studied variables

Mplus Version 8.3 was used to perform multi-level path analysis to examine the proposed hypothesis. We adopted group-mean centering for Level 1 variables and grand-mean centering for the Level 2 variable. Figure 2 shows the analysis results. The path coefficient from PUTS to supervisor revenge was non-significant (β = 0.11, p > .05) and the coefficients from supervisor revenge to job performance (β = −0.02, p > .05), cooperative behavior (β = 0.00, p > .05), and proactive behavior (β = 0.06, p > .05) were non-significant. Thus, H1 and H3 were not supported. The path coefficient from PUTS to supervisor forgiveness was significant (β = −0.24, p < .01) and the coefficients from supervisor forgiveness to job performance (β = 0.28, p < .01), cooperative behavior (β = 0.12, p < .05), and proactive behavior (β = 0.35, p < .01) were significant. Thus, H2 and H4 were supported.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Results of path analysis. *p < .05. **p < .01

Table 3 shows the mediation analysis. The results indicated that supervisor revenge did not mediate the relationship between outcomes (indirect effects = 0.00–0.01, p > .05). Thus, H5 was not supported. However, the results indicated that supervisor forgiveness significantly mediated the relationship between PUTS and job performance (indirect effect = −0.08, p = .01) and between PUTS and proactive behavior (indirect effect = −0.10, p = .01). Thus, H6 was partially supported.

Table 3 Mediation effects of supervisor forgiveness and revenge

Figure 2 shows that supervisor affective commitment significantly moderated the relationship between PUTS and supervisor revenge (β = 0.33, p < .05). Figure 3 shows the moderation effect: PUTS was positively associated with supervisor revenge when commitment was high and negatively associated with supervisor revenge when commitment was low. Thus, H7 was supported.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The moderation effect of supervisors’ affective commitment on the relationship between perceived unfair treatment by a subordinate and supervisor revenge

Discussion

This study proposed that supervisors might experience being unfairly treated by their subordinates. The results showed that PUTS did not lead supervisors to revenge but did decrease supervisor forgiveness. Ultimately, PUTS also decreased employee effectiveness via its negative impact on supervisor forgiveness. The results further indicated that PUTS did not cause supervisor revenge and that revenge had no significant effects on outcomes. The moderation analysis demonstrated that supervisor affective commitment tended to make the supervisor stress the unity of the workgroup and make them more likely to act in a revengeful manner in response to PUTS.

This study demonstrates that PUTS is a valid construct and that supervisors do react to PUTS. It suggests that in law enforcement organizations, supervisors would be less likely to take revenge in response to their subordinates’ unfair treatment but more likely to show a decrease in forgiveness to their subordinates. Contrary to our expectations, PUTS did not predict supervisor revenge. We offer three possible explanations for this finding. First, police officers are expected to respond with kindness and benevolence toward others, even in unfair situations (Tyler, 2011; Tyler & Huo, 2002), and as supervisors bear the role of modeling this behavior (Van Craen & Skogan, 2017) they might suppress their revenge reactions. Second, Chinese culture emphasizes the benevolence of leaders toward their subordinates (Farh & Cheng, 2000; Jiang & Cheng, 2008), and supervisors embedded in this culture might therefore be reluctant to use revenge to get even. Third, organizations might prohibit supervisors’ revengeful behavior, and supervisors have the option of under-rating subordinates in performance appraisals without showing revenge. However, supervisors who are highly committed to the organization might believe their acts are benefiting the company, like a soldier for the company, and thus be more inclined to control the behavior of a wrongdoer in the workgroup (Fehr & Gachter, 2002; Organ, 1988; Wenzel et al., 2008).

As predicted, PUTS decreased supervisor forgiveness, which promoted subordinates’ job performance, cooperative behavior, and proactive behavior. The finding that supervisor forgiveness was critical to employee effectiveness is consistent with the results of Fehr and Gelfand (2012). Supervisor forgiveness can significantly impact a workgroup’s forgiving climate, and a recent study indicated that a forgiving climate promoted subordinates’ forgiving behavior with benefits for job performance (Radulovic et al., 2019).

Surprisingly, supervisor revenge did not predict employee effectiveness, although studies have shown that police officers who are not treated fairly and respectfully will respond negatively (Maria et al., 2018; Reynolds et al., 2018). Silin (1976) found that employees showed loyalty to the authoritarian leader in a Chinese family enterprise. Jiang and Cheng’s (2008) study indicated that subordinates felt they should behave loyally to their supervisors in a Chinese organization, even though they might not treat them fairly. These results suggest that a culture of loyalty might prevent subordinates from reacting to revenge by their supervisors, and this might not be the case in other cultural contexts. Further studies are needed to investigate why supervisor revenge did not harm subordinates’ performance.

From the perspective of supervisors, this study explored the responses they exhibit when subjected to unfair treatment from their subordinates. However, the way subordinates treat their supervisors may also be influenced by the supervisors’ behavior (Mitchell & Ambrose, 2007). When supervisors treat their subordinates disrespectfully or unfairly, subordinates might respond in kind (Van Craen & Skogan, 2017). Although future research could further explore this reciprocal relationship, supervisors are more likely than subordinates to seek to restore a sense of fairness when subjected to unfair treatment (Aquino et al., 2006). More importantly, the responses of supervisors are likely to have a more direct impact on subordinates than the reverse.

Policy Implications

Police officers bear responsibilities and experience pressure from multiple sources, including work tasks, public demands, and the need for crime prevention. Consequently, the mental and physical well-being and stress management of police officers are always been a critical focus of both research and practice (Bishopp et al., 2019; McCarty & Skogan, 2013). This study highlights that supervisors in law enforcement units can experience unfair treatment from their subordinates, creating further burdens for those already under multifaceted pressures (Martinussen et al., 2007). Therefore, law enforcement agencies should consider providing leadership training for supervisors to enhance their ability to manage and respond to deviant behavior by subordinates (Sadri, 2012). Furthermore, emotional intelligence training is recommended as it can improve supervisors’ skills and capabilities in handling emotional incidents, including managing their own emotions and those of their subordinates (Salovey & Mayer, 1990).

For law enforcement agencies, it may also be necessary to consider reducing the group loyalty effect, which describes the tendency of members with high loyalty to a group to spontaneously punish those who are disloyal (Fehr & Gächter, 2002). The results of the present study suggest that when supervisors have a high degree of loyalty to the police department, they are more likely to exhibit retaliatory behaviors when mistreated. However, it seems that such behavior does not improve their subordinates’ work performance, cooperative behavior, or proactive behavior. Therefore, while encouraging supervisors’ commitment and loyalty to the organization, law enforcement agencies should also promote inclusivity by encouraging supervisors to accept different approaches and perspectives (Nembhard & Edmondson, 2006), thereby increasing the positive participation of members with diverse characteristics and reducing the potential group loyalty effect.

Limitations and Future Studies

This study applied a group-based dyadic sample, which allowed us to control for common group effects and thus minimize potential common method variance (CMV; Podsakoff et al., 2003). However, a survey study has a limited ability to examine causal relationship, and longitudinal and experimental studies are required to further examine causality. Future studies could also investigate critical moderators in the relationship between PUTS and supervisor forgiveness and revenge and between supervisor revenge and forgiveness and outcomes. Among the supervisor participants, 55% were first-line supervisors and 45% were mid-level supervisors. Although this study did not address the potential influence of supervisor rank, it is plausible that the more powerful mid-level supervisors may be more inclined than first-level supervisors to exhibit retaliatory behaviors (Aquino et al., 2006). Further investigations with attention to supervisor rank could yield insightful findings.

As the law enforcement culture might influence how police officers perceive and react to interpersonal conflicts and managerial issues, it would be interesting to further investigate the relationship between organizational culture and justice-related behavior within law enforcement organizations (Game & Crawshaw, 2017). People normally expect law enforcement organizations should adhere to higher justice standards than business enterprises. This study’s results showed that police officers are conservative in enacting revenge. The self-control of those in managerial roles might be the critical element in managing justice and police officers’ misconduct in police departments (Donner & Jennings, 2014; Restubog et al., 2012; Wolfe et al., 2018). Chinese cultural values, such as harmony (Westwood, 1997), might make supervisors in Taiwanese law enforcement organizations hesitate to engage in acts of revenge and more inclined to demonstrate benevolence and forgiveness. Therefore, similar investigations are needed in Western societies to examine the potential influence of cultural values. Compared with law enforcement organizations, the power distance between supervisors and subordinates in private business organizations tends to be lower and more flexible, with higher personnel turnover (Engelson, 1999; Shavell, 1993). As a result, supervisors in business settings may be more inclined to engage in retaliatory actions to exclude non-conforming or disliked subordinates. Further exploration of how supervisors in business organizations respond to unfair treatment from subordinates could provide valuable insights into these dynamics.