Introduction

While patients with gliomas often experience impairment in physical and emotional functioning, specifically deficits in neurocognitive functioning (NCF) negatively affect daily life functioning and health-related quality of life [1]. These deficits can be caused by the tumor and surrounding edema, by epilepsy and anti-epileptic drugs, and by tumor treatment, including surgery, radiotherapy and chemotherapy [1].

There is substantial evidence that the tumor itself is the major cause of neurocognitive deficits in glioma patients. Prior to treatment, impairment in working memory [2], memory [3], executive functioning [3, 4], and language capacity [4, 5] have been observed.

Regarding surgery, usually the first step in treatment of these patients, detailed knowledge on the neuroanatomy-function associations is pivotal to attain an optimal balance between tumor control (e.g. OS) by resection and functional outcome (e.g. NCF). Although available data on the involvement of specific brain regions involved in language [5], and verbal or perceptual abilities [6] certainly are of value, more detailed information regarding the involvement of specific cortical or subcortical brain regions and tracts on NCF is needed. To identify those brain regions critically involved in NCF, voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping [7] is a promising tool that is well-established in stroke [8] and traumatic brain injury [9, 10]. However, in brain tumor patients voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping has been applied in only a few postoperative [11, 12] and preoperative [13] studies, with a focus on one specific neurocognitive function or specific brain regions. The use of so-called tumor localization maps (TLMs) in order to voxel-wise link brain tumor location to NCF, can add pertinent information to the associations known from non-tumor lesion studies, as gliomas have differences in preference location, nature and infiltrative growth pattern compared to other, acute, lesions [13]. Voxel-by-voxel lesion mapping has the advantage that a priori lesion location grouping is avoided [7, 14] and that white matter structures can additionally be identified.

The aim of the present study is to better understand the impact of brain tumor locations on distinct neurocognitive functions in glioma patients prior to surgery, by using voxel-based lesion-function mapping.

Methods

Patients

A retrospective observational cohort of consecutive de novo patients planned to undergo resective surgery for suspected glioma in Amsterdam UMC, a tertiary referral center for neuro-oncological surgery, was established. Adult patients (≥ 18 years) were included if they had a radiologically suspected glioma, no prior surgery, no radiotherapy or chemotherapy, preoperative neurocognitive assessment, and preoperative MRI including 3D FLAIR and 3D T1 gadolinium. Neuropsychological assessment and MRI were part of routine clinical care. Exclusion criteria were history of neurological or severe psychiatric disorder potentially interfering with NCF, and insufficient command of the Dutch language.

Standard protocol approvals and patient consent

Approval of the study protocol by the institutional review board of the Amsterdam UMC was not required according to the Dutch law on Medical Research in Humans. All patients gave written informed consent for using clinically derived data, including neuropsychological and MRI data, for research purposes.

Neurocognitive assessment

NCF was assessed by a battery of standardized neuropsychological tests, covering the range of neurocognitive functions known to be affected in glioma patients [6, 15]. We combined test outcomes into six cognitive domain scores, based on previous studies and consensus in neuropsychological practice: information processing speed, attention, working memory, verbal memory, visual memory and executive functioning (Online Resource 1) [15, 16].

For each test, the patient’s performance was compared to published normative data of healthy controls, corrected for age and educational level, or with performance of healthy controls from a database of the Maastricht Aging study, The Netherlands [17]. These controls where individually matched to patients according to age, gender, and educational level [18]. Individual patient test scores were converted into standardized z-scores with use of mean and standard deviation (SD) of the healthy controls on that test outcome. Domain summary measures were calculated for the patients, by dividing the total score of the test scores contributing to the domain, by the number of test outcomes of that domain. A domain score of ≥ 1.5 SD below the mean of controls was considered a clinically significant neurocognitive impairment [19].

MRI scanning

All patients were scanned using a standardized neuro-oncology protocol. The protocol included a 3D FLAIR turbo spin-echo pulse-sequence and a 3D heavily T1-weighted gradient-echo pulse-sequence obtained after double-dose administration of intravenous gadolinium. Scans were available from various 1.5 and 3.0 T scanners (including GE Signa HDXT, Toshiba Titan, Siemens Avanto, and Philips Ingenia). All MRI images were acquired with ~ 1 mm isotropic resolution.

Voxel-based lesion mapping

For tumor localization mapping, we used the methodology described elsewhere [20]. Tumor regions were segmented in 3D on FLAIR images (smart brush tool of iPlan v3.0 software; BrainLAB AG, Feldkirchen, Germany) in case of a suspected low-grade glioma (i.e. hyperintense signal abnormality) and on T1 post contrast images in case of a high-grade glioma (i.e. contrast-enhancement abnormality). The segmented volumes were verified and adjusted in reconstruction planes by two observers (EH and PW). The FLAIR scans were linearly registered to the T1 scans for further analysis using the image fusion tool. Perilesional edema was excluded from the segmentations. Subsequently, the segmented volumes were exported as binary masks registered to the T1 images. The resulting patient-specific T1 volume was non-linearly registered to 2 × 2 × 2 mm Montreal Neurological Institute brain template (MNI-152 space), i.e. standard brain space [21]. This registration was done in sequential steps, with rigid, affine, B-spline regularization and symmetric diffeomorphic registration with cross-correlation as similarity metric [22]. The registration was visually verified and repeated when necessary. The results were preoperative TLMs of all patients grouped together, aligned to MNI space for further analysis. The probability of tumor was calculated for each voxel that covered MNI space, by dividing the number of patients with a glioma at that voxel by the total number of patients.

Statistical analysis

For a detailed description of the statistical analysis of the TLMs we refer to our previous publication [20]. In short, to identify brain regions associated with cognitive impairment, for each neurocognitive domain we compared TLMs of patients with and without neurocognitive impairment in the presence of a glioma at a specific voxel using a voxel-wise randomization test. We restricted the analyses to brain regions affected by tumor in at least three patients. As test statistic per voxel, the fraction of patients with a tumor region among those with an impairment was divided by the fraction of patients with a tumor region among those without, providing a ‘relative risk’. A higher test statistic corresponded to a higher rate of tumor location regions among patients with an impairment than among those without.

To evaluate the probability of the test statistics, we used a randomization test. The spatial dependency of the data was addressed by estimating the empirical null-distribution of relative risks for each voxel based on a permutation with relabeling of patients to one of the two cohorts. A subset of 4000 randomizations was randomly drawn without replacement to provide a reasonable estimate, as has been applied to analyze functional neuroimaging [23,24,25]. The adjusted one-sided p-value per voxel was calculated by counting the number of equal or more extreme test statistics in the randomized null distribution, and dividing by the number of randomizations. Multiple hypothesis testing was controlled for type I error using the false discovery rate, expressed as q-value for each voxel. These q-values are the proportion of voxels with false differential probability of a glioma among all voxels declared differentially infiltrated, when the p-value of a voxel is called significant. To obtain the q-value for a voxel, we divided the estimated number of false discoveries by the number of voxels declared significant. The estimated number of false discoveries given a specific adjusted p-value threshold was determined from the empirical null-distribution of 4000 randomizations per voxel. The number of voxels declared significant was determined from the distribution of observed p-values [23, 25]. This resulted in p- and q-value maps of differentially infiltrated brain regions that were superimposed on MNI-152 space for visual inspection. Because of the exploratory nature of our study, we considered q < 0.4 as informative, and q < 0.2 as significant. The correlation between the number of tumor voxels and the neurocognitive domain score (impaired versus not impaired) was analyzed using a non-parametric Spearman test. For further interpretation of the brain locations, we aligned our results with standard cortical [26] and white matter anatomy [27]. Statistical procedures were customized in R, v3.3 [28].

Language functioning as reference measure

To ensure the validity of our map comparisons, as the use of TLMs is a relatively new approach in glioma patients, we used language functioning as a reference measure. A map calculating the association between preoperative tumor location and language impairment was added. Language function was tested during preoperative neurological examination and was characterized mainly by naming and (subjective) word finding problems.

Results

Sociodemographic and clinical data

Seventy-two patients were included (Table 1). Excluded were ten cases: three patients with incomplete neuropsychological assessment, two patients with a history of previous surgery, one patient with both previous surgery and radiotherapy, two patients with a cavernous malformation, one patient with an epidermoid and one patient with a hypothalamus hamartoma. The majority (60%) of patients, with a mean age of 40 years, was diagnosed with a glioma WHO grade II. Forty-one tumors were located in the left, and 31 in the right hemisphere. Epilepsy was the most frequently presenting symptom (62%), in 10% of cases in combination with neurocognitive impairment. Tumors were predominantly located in frontal, temporal and insular brain regions (Fig. 1). In two patients with right sided tumor and left handedness, additional fMRI revealed right hemispheric dominance for language functions. In two left handed patients with tumor in the left hemisphere, fMRI showed right hemisphere dominance for language. For all other patients, left hemisphere was language dominant. The mean interval between preoperative MRI and neurocognitive assessment was 0.4 weeks (SD ± 9.2 weeks).

Table 1 Sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the patient cohort
Fig. 1
figure 1

Glioma locations within the brain. Transversal sections are shown superimposed on standard brain space (MNI). The legend refers to the number of patients with glioma tissue at a voxel, with lighter yellow indicating a higher number of patients

Neurocognitive outcome

Attention and working memory capacity were the most frequently affected domains, in 30% and 20% of patients respectively. Fewer patients experienced impaired visual memory (14%), verbal memory (13%), information processing speed (11%), and executive functioning (8%) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of neurocognitive functioning scores per domain (n = 72). Each bar represents one patient, with darker colors indicating scores below 1.5 SD of healthy controls (horizontal dotted line at − 1.5 SD)

Brain regions associated with neurocognitive functions

Reference measure: language functioning

Only regions in the left hemisphere were found to be significantly associated with language functioning, including the frontal operculum, supra-marginal gyrus, arcuate fasciculus, superior- and middle frontal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and insular cortex (Online Resource 2). As the significant language regions and tracts in our sample are in accordance with earlier studies on naming and word retrieval processing in the brain [29, 30], tumor location mapping can be considered a valid method to link NCF to brain regions.

Neurocognitive functions

Attentional performance was significantly associated with regions of the left frontal and parietal cortex (Fig. 3a; Online Resource 3), including precentral and postcentral gyrus, central and parietal operculum (Fig. 4). Moreover, significant regions were observed in the insular cortex, Heschl’s gyrus and the planum temporale, and in white matter tracts (arcuate fasciculus and corticospinal tract). In the right hemisphere, only a focal area of voxels in the parahippocampal gyrus exceeded the critical q-value.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Tumor localization maps for the attention (a) and working memory (b) domains. For both domains, rows represent (1) TLM of patients with a tumor at that voxel without a deficit in the particular domain, (2) TLM of patients with a tumor and a deficit in that domain (lighter yellow indicates higher percentages), (3) relative risk map of tumor probability, i.e. the odds ratio at each voxel between tumor probabilities (1) and (2), with darker green indicating more deficits in presence of a tumor. The observed relative differences were voxel-wise tested for significance, resulting in an adjusted p-value map (4), taking spatial dependence into account (lighter yellow indicates lower p-values); (5) corrected q-value map to address multiple testing, displayed as the log odds ratios for the voxels with q-values below 0.4 presented in red and q-values below 0.2 in yellow. Voxel clusters with q<0.4 were considered significant

Fig. 4
figure 4

Heat map with the association between anatomical brain structures and neurocognitive domains. The neurocognitive domains are shown in columns, grouped per hemisphere and the midline. Anatomical structures are displayed in rows, grouped per cortical region, subcortical white matter structure and subcortical nucleus. Horizontal orientation shows the percentage of significant voxels (with q < 0.4) per domain of the total number of significant voxels per anatomical structure, with darker shades of green indicating a stronger association. Absence of information in anatomical regions is displayed as white, while information of 0% is displayed as light yellow

For working memory, no significant clusters were identified (Figs. 3b, 4; Online Resource 4). P-values indicate bilateral parietal and frontal regions, predominantly in left hemisphere, possibly involved in working memory performance, but these were not identified in our q-value maps.

Visual memory, verbal memory, information processing speed and executive functioning were also analyzed for associated brain regions, although these were involved in fewer patients. Overall, for these four domains, regions in left white matter structures, including arcuate fasciculus, corticospinal tract, and corpus callosum, and in left frontal and parietal cortex, including pre- and postcentral gyrus, were found to be significantly associated with performance (Fig. 4; Online Resources 5–8). In addition, visual and verbal memory impairments were associated with regions in the left cingulum, left posterior cingulate gyrus and precuneus. For verbal memory and information processing speed, the hippocampus showed significant voxels. Many other regions were identified to be significantly associated with executive functioning, most importantly the inferior fasciculus and superior parietal lobe. Right-sided regions, including temporal pole, fusiform cortex and basal ganglia, were only associated with verbal memory.

Discussion

We aimed to better understand the association between tumor location and distinct neurocognitive functions in glioma patients prior to surgery. Attention and working memory were the most affected domains. Using voxel-based lesion mapping we observed brain structures associated with attention impairments to be localized in the left frontal and parietal cortex and left-sided white matter tracts. The neural basis for attentional functioning has been extensively studied. One of the most influential models is the three attentional networks model (alerting, orienting and executive control) [31, 32], which involves an extensive anatomical distribution including frontal and parietal regions with interconnecting pathways, the anterior cingulate cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and the thalamus with connections to the superior parietal lobe, temporo-parietal junction, the superior temporal lobe, and the frontal eye fields [31, 33, 34]. Our results comply to some extent with this anatomical attentional network, i.e. we found significant regions in prefrontal and parietal areas. However, we could not replicate the involvement of the anterior cingulate cortex in executive control [34, 35], nor did we observe regions associated with the orienting and alerting networks of attention [31, 35]. This could be due to an underrepresentation of gliomas in these regions or by the use of other attentional tasks than the ones used in Posner’s model [34] that thus might have provoked activation in different brain regions. Contrasting with earlier studies, apart from the parahippocampal gyrus, we did not identify regions associated with aspects of attention in the right hemisphere [31, 36], including the inferior frontal gyrus and medial temporal cortex found in stroke patients [37] and the caudal anterior cingulate in traumatic brain injury patients [38]. Nor did we observe bilateral activation in the thalamus [35, 39] and basal ganglia [39]. Perhaps we did not find associated regions in the right hemisphere because our testing protocol consisted of tasks requiring a verbal response, and did not assess spatial attention.

Surprisingly, we also observed sections of the white matter tracts, especially the arcuate fasciculus and the corticospinal tract, to be involved in attentional functioning. The corticospinal tract is mainly involved in motor function, which was not required in the applied tasks. The arcuate fasciculus is known for its role in language processing [30], but scarce data also suggest a role in attentional processes and executive functioning, for example in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, using diffusion spectrum imaging tractography [40]. As the involvement of the arcuate fasciculus was also found for memory, speed and executive functioning, and most tasks were presented verbally and/or required a verbal response, glioma infiltrating in the arcuate fasciculus could have a subtle impact on neurocognitive performance.

No specific regions were identified to be associated with working memory, perhaps due to low statistical power. In the literature, many different structures within the prefrontal-parietal network have been associated with working memory performance in healthy and brain injured patients, partly depending on the type of working memory task. For example, impairments in complex span tasks were associated with lesions in the left inferior frontal gyrus [41]. Other relevant regions, found in fMRI studies, include the bilateral posterior parietal cortex [42, 43] premotor areas and posterior cingulate [42], insular cortex and middle temporal gyrus, the inferior longitudinal and fronto-occipital fasciculi, uncinate, and corpus callosum [43].

Our study evidently has some limitations. First, including low-grade glioma might have complicated the analysis, as these tumors do not show contrast enhancement on MRI and hence tumor boundaries are more difficult to identify. Also the behavior and biology of low- and high-grade glioma might affect the infiltration in brain tissue in a variable way [44], complicating lesion mapping and therefore, according to some authors, makes this method not applicable in glioma patients [45]. However, the tumor maps were also based on FLAIR images, and in accordance with others [13, 46] we are convinced that voxel-based lesion mapping is applicable in gliomas. Second, differences in the concept of brain plasticity might hamper interpretation of our results. For example, the higher growth rate associated with isocitrate dehydrogenase 1 wild type (IDH1-WT) tumors compared to IDH1-mutant tumors, might result in less efficient brain networks and less time for brain plasticity to compensate for functional loss [47]. Also, functional compensation might be better for cognitive functions that are represented in more widespread brain networks, such as language [48], than for more basic functions such as spatial attention [13], although results in literature are partially contradictory [49]. As a result, we might have missed brain regions involved in specific neurocognitive functions seen in acute lesions. However, this does not negate that the regions we found to be associated with NCF are still of relevance. Third, due to unevenly distributed glioma over cortical and subcortical areas, not all regions could be included in our analysis. Therefore, perhaps not all brain structures relevant for the tested neurocognitive functions could be identified. Fourth, the interval between cognitive assessment and MRI scanning varied. In patients with a relatively long interval, the location, size and infiltration pattern of glioma tissue, as observed on MRI, could have been different from the glioma at time of the neurocognitive assessment. However, especially high-grade gliomas show rapid change and these patients were scanned within a relatively short interval. Fifth, we did not calculate Jaccard/Dice overlap scores between the two observers (EH and PW). The interrater agreement for tumor segmentation before surgery in general is known to be non-trivial [50]. Sixth, neuropsychological assessment was more routinely performed in WHO grade II glioma patients, who are typically young adults with an anticipated survival of many years. Cognitive performance is important for social and professional functioning during these years. For patients with WHO grade IV glioma, who still have a relatively short survival, extensive neuropsychological assessment was less well possible due to physical status or the necessity of planning the surgery at short notice. For this reason, high-grade glioma patients are underrepresented in our study cohort. Finally, because of the explorative nature of our study design, we did not examine the influence of epilepsy, use of anti-epileptic drugs, emotional changes, depression, anxiety and fatigue on NCF, while these factors are relevant in glioma patients and have an impact on cognition [51,52,53,54].

Our study has several implications. First, our finding regarding the involvement of the corticospinal tract in attention adds some information to the field of neuroscience. This observation should be confirmed in larger series. Second, as preoperative neurocognitive impairment in glioma patients is common, and mainly associated with disruption of the left frontoparietal network, assessment of attention and working memory prior to surgery, and informing patients regarding these impairments, should be incorporated in clinical care. Third, during awake surgical procedures resective surgery can be optimized by intraoperative monitoring of attentional function. However, rigorous paradigms to test these functions are yet to be developed. Finally, with information regarding preoperative neurocognitive impairment, cognitive rehabilitation can be applied in an early stage and tailored to the individual patient.