Introduction

The prevalence of obesity has increased over the last decades, making it a major public health problem worldwide. An imbalance between energy intake and energy expenditure leads to the overproduction of body fat, which may cause various metabolic disorders (Hossain et al. 2007; Mello et al. 2006). The association between this excess of body fat and cardiovascular diseases (Aghamohammadzadeh and Heagerty 2012; Kenchaiah et al. 2004; Lotufo 2000), some types of cancers (Demark-Wahnefried et al. 2012) and type II diabetes mellitus is well known (Wannamethee and Shaper 1999). Besides these classical comorbidities, there is paramount evidence of the relationship between obesity and neurological dysfunction, including neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases (Calsolaro and Edison 2016; Morales et al. 2014).

An important feature of obesity is the chronic low-grade inflammation that occurs in metabolic tissues, especially in adipose tissue. This inflammatory state, named metainflammation, is the main cause of the maladaptative responses to obesity. The increased secretion of pro-inflammatory cytokines by adipose tissue, such as tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α), interleukin 6 (IL-6) and interleukin-1β (IL-1β), can inhibit insulin signaling (Bulló et al. 2007; Maury and Brichard 2010; Shah et al. 2008). Hence, the pro-inflammatory phenotype is associated with insulin resistance, and together, these features promote a widespread repercussion on the body systems. On the other hand, it has already been demonstrated that a reduction in body weight leads to the increased expression of anti-inflammatory adipokines concomitant with a decrease in insulin resistance and in the levels of pro-inflammatory cytokines (Shah et al. 2008). Thus, strategies to contribute to weight loss are highly beneficial.

Previous studies have shown that, in obesity, inflammation in the hypothalamus may contribute to the inappropriate regulation of energy metabolism (van de Sande-Lee and Velloso 2012). However, this pro-inflammatory activity is not limited to the hypothalamus, being propagated to other brain regions and affecting a variety of brain functions (Gregor and Hotamisligil 2011). In addition, obesity can increase susceptibility to the development of neurodegenerative diseases, and neuroinflammation is a potential causal factor of the dysregulation in the central nervous system (CNS) caused by overeating. It was shown that a high fat diet can lead to the excessive production of IL-6 by activated microglia and also cause the subsequent activation of astrocytes due to the stimulation of pro-inflammatory cytokines (Tapia-González et al. 2011). These neuroinflammatory responses are harmful, since these mechanisms are associated with neuropathological processes and neurodegeneration (Ransohoff et al. 2015). The long-term inflammation could have disastrous consequences in the CNS, such as the loss of synapses, affecting cognition and executive functions (Hein and O'Banion 2009; Kohman and Rhodes 2013). Throughout life, there is a natural decline in cognitive functions; however, some environmental factors, such as obesity, may accelerate this decline (Haan and Wallace 2004). The activation of inflammatory pathways may contribute to an impairment in learning and memory, which may be related to obesity, although the mechanisms by which this occurs are still poorly understood (Hwang et al. 2010; Pistell et al. 2010).

Polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) from the omega-3 family are well known to suppress neuroinflammation and reduce oxidative stress. These PUFAs have an anti-inflammatory action, reducing the synthesis of arachidonic acid derivatives (Bazan et al. 2012). Furthermore, in the CNS, they influence enzymatic activity, cell signaling, synaptic plasticity, and neurotransmitter release, and modulate cytokines that have neuromodulatory activity (Högyes et al. 2003). It has been suggested that the consumption of unsaturated fatty acids (UFAs) can positively affect spatial learning and memory and also decrease the negative effects of physiological stress on cognitive skills (Nemeth et al. 2015).

Despite the important action of omega-3 PUFAs as anti-inflammatory molecules and their role as neuroprotective compounds, previous studies have not evaluated whether omega-3 supplementation could reverse any outcome of a high-fat diet (HFD). The present study aimed to evaluate the impact of omega-3 on metabolic parameters (weigh gain, visceral adiposity, and insulin resistance), memory and neuroinflammatory markers following diet-induced obesity in rats.

Materials and methods

Animals

Male Wistar rats (n = 40) from the animal facility of the Universidade Federal de Ciências da Saúde de Porto Alegre were used. Animals had free access to standard rat chow and water until the age of 7 months. Rats were housed in plastic cages (2–3 rats per cage) under controlled temperature (22-24 °C) and light (12 h light/12 h dark cycle; lights on at 6:00 a.m.) conditions.

All efforts were made to minimize animal suffering and to reduce the number of animals used in the experiments, which were performed in accordance with the international laws for the care of laboratory animals. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (UFCSPA, Brazil, protocol No. 261/14).

Diet and omega-3 administration

Seven month-old rats were divided into the following experimental groups: standard diet + vehicle (SD, n = 11); standard diet + omega-3 (SD + O, n = 8); high fat diet (HFD, n = 12) + vehicle; and high fat diet + omega-3 (HFD + O, n = 9). SD or HFD was administered for 20 weeks. Omega-3 supplementation was performed from the 16th to the 20th week of diet administration. Omega-3 was administered daily through gavage at a dose of 1 g/kg.

SD and SD + O groups were fed with Nuvilab® CR-1 standard rat chow (NUVITAL®, Curitiba, PR, Brazil) providing a total energy content of 3.4 kcal/g (63% carbohydrates, 26% protein, 11% fat). Animals from HFD and HFD + O groups were fed a high fat diet (Pragsoluções Biociências, Jaú, SP, Brazil) providing a total energy of 4.5 kcal/g (35.7% carbohydrate, 19.2% protein, 45.1% fat). Nutritional information was provided by manufacturers.

Object recognition test

In the last week of the diet administration, animals were submitted to the object recognition test. On the first day, rats were habituated in an acrylic box (40 cm × 40 cm) delimited by four walls with a height of 20 cm. Twenty-four hours after habituation, the training session was conducted. Animals were placed individually in the left rear quadrant of the box containing two different objects (A and B), and were allowed to freely explore them for 5 min. The retention test was performed 24 h after the training session. For the test, rats were individually reintroduced into the box where one of the objects presented during training was randomly replaced by a new object (C). Exploration of an object was defined as directing the nose towards the object at a distance less than 2 cm and/or when the animal touched the object with their nose (Ennaceur and Delacour 1988). The exploration time of the familiar and new objects were counted.

Tissue and blood collection

At the end of experimental time course, animals were decapitated for blood and tissue collection. Trunk blood was collected and, after centrifugation (1500 x g for 10 min at 4 °C), serum was separated and stored at −80 °C for later analysis. The brain was quickly removed and the right and left hemispheres were separated. The right hemisphere was put in a fixative solution for histological analyses. The cerebral cortex was dissected from the left hemisphere and quickly frozen in liquid nitrogen, as previously described in de Moura et al. (2015).

Blood analysis

Serum levels of glucose were analyzed using colorimetric kits (LabTest), according to the manufacturer’s instructions. An enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) commercial kit (Millipore) was used to analyze serum levels of insulin according to the manufacturer’s instructions.

HOMA index

The HOMA (homeostasis model assessment) index was used in order to quantify insulin resistance and beta cell function in the pancreas (Matthews et al. 1985). The index is calculated based on blood levels of glucose (mmol/L) and insulin (mU/L), using the following formula: (glucose blood levels x insulin blood levels) / 22.5 (Nattiv et al. 2007).

Immunohistochemical staining (IHC)

For immunohistochemistry, the left hemisphere of the brain was fixed in a zinc buffer solution (pH 7.4) for 48 h at room temperature. After this period, tissues were dehydrated, embedded in paraffin and sectioned (8 μm thick) using a microtome. Sections were treated with 3% hydrogen peroxide in 10% methanol for 30 min, washed in PBS for 30 min and incubated for 30 min in 3% normal goat serum in PBS containing 0.4% Triton X-100 (PBS-T). Then, sections were incubated for 48 h at 4 °C with a monoclonal anti-GFAP antibody (Millipore), diluted 1:750. Sections were incubated with anti-mouse IgG peroxidase-conjugated secondary antibody (Sigma), diluted 1:500 in PBS-T, for 90 min at room temperature. Immunoreaction was developed using a solution of 0.06% 3,3′-diaminobenzidine tetrahydrochloride (Sigma) and 0.005% hydrogen peroxide in PBS. Sections were counterstained with hematoxylin, dehydrated, and covered with Entellan (Merck) and coverslips. Digital images were acquired using a digital camera coupled to an Olympus BX-41 microscope using a 20× objective lens. The images were analyzed using ImageJ software (http://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). Three randomly selected fields were analyzed in six nonadjacent sections. Five animals per group were analyzed.

Molecular analyses

RNA extraction

RNA was extracted from the samples with Trizol (Invitrogen) which was used according to the manufacturer’s recommendations. The structures of the brain were homogenized in the presence of Trizol (1:5, v/v); aqueous phase was obtained by centrifugation (12,000 x g, 15 min). The RNA was precipitated with isopropanol, for 15 min at room temperature, followed by centrifugation at 12000 x g for 10 min. The pellets were resuspended in 0.1% DEPC-treated water. The concentration of total RNA was determined by measuring the optical density at 260 nm and purity of the RNA was evaluated on the basis of the 280 nm/260 nm ratio and electrophoresis on agarose gel (Langnaese et al. 2008).

cDNA synthesis

Total RNA (1 μg) was used as a template to synthesize cDNA. The RNA was incubated for 5 min at 60 °C with 1 μl oligo (dT) (0.5 μg/μl, Invitrogen), 1 μl 10 mM dNTP and DEPC-water to a final volume of 12 μl, and then for 1 min on ice. The following reagents were then added to a final volume of 19 μl: 4 μl of RT buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 8.3, 75 mM KCl, 3 mM MgCl2), 2 μl 0.1 M DTT, and 1 μl RNaseOUT (40 U/μl, Invitrogen). After 2 min incubation at 37 °C, 1 μl M-MLV-RT (200 U/μl; Invitrogen) was added and cDNA synthesis was carried out at 50 °C for 1 h. The reaction was inactivated by incubation at 70 °C for 15 min.

Real-time PCR (qPCR)

The expression of TNF-α, IL-6 and IL-10 was assessed. The housekeeping genes beta-actin (ActB) and Cyclophilin A (CypA), which have been shown to be stable in each brain area, were used as controls. Primer sequences are presented in Table 1. Amplification was carried out using 7.5 μL of SYBR Green PCR Master Mix (Applied Biosystems), 0.5 μL of forward and reverse primers (0.33 M each), 100 ng of cDNA and nuclease-free water, in a total volume of 15 μL. Reactions were performed in an optical 96-well plate, using a StepOnePlusTM thermocycler (Applied Biosystems). After an initial denaturation step at 95 °C for 10 min, amplification was performed in 40 cycles of denaturation at 95 °C for 30 s, annealing at 60 °C for 40 s and extension at 72 °C for 40 s. Amplification was followed by a melt curve analysis to confirm PCR product specificity. No signals were detected in the no-template controls. The experimental Ct (cycle threshold) was calculated using the algorithm enhancements provided by the equipment. All samples were run in duplicate and the mean value of each duplicate was used for all further calculations (Cook et al. 2010; Langnaese et al. 2008; Nelissen et al. 2010). The Ct value of each reaction was used to calculate the level of mRNA expression of each specific gene, after normalizing it to the expression of the control housekeeping gene (HKG) genes analyzed in the same reaction plate (Zimmermann-Peruzatto et al. 2016).

Table 1 Primer sequences used in the study

Statistical analyses

The data are expressed as mean ± SEM, except for weight gain, which was expressed as median and interquartile range. Data were analyzed by two-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post-hoc. Treatment and diet were used as factors for all ANOVA analyses, except for the object recognition test, in which experimental group and object (familiar and new) were used as factors. GraphPad Prism 5.0 was used for the statistical analyses. The significance level was set at p < 0.05.

Results

Animals fed a HFD gained significantly more weight over time than the animals fed a standard diet (diet effect, F 1,36 = 24.82, p < 0.0001). Omega-3 treatment (HFD + O group) did not revert this parameter and Bonferroni post-hoc showed that weight gain in the HFD + O group was not significantly lower than in the HFD group (p > 0.05). However, when visceral adiposity was evaluated, HFD caused a significant increase in visceral fat compared to standard diet (F 1,36 = 12.49, p = 0.001). Post-hoc analysis showed that omega-3 supplementation is able to prevent visceral adiposity in obese rats (p < 0.05). Glycemia and plasma insulin did not show any differences between groups (p > 0.05). Also, HOMA-IR, which provides an estimation of insulin resistance, did not show a diet effect as analyzed by two-way ANOVA (F 1,36 = 3.178, p > 0.05). However, post-hoc comparison between HFD and HFD + O showed a significant decrease in HOMA-IR after omega-3 supplementation (p < 0.05). This result proves that omega-3 PUFA improves insulin sensitivity in obesity (Table 2).

Table 2 Effects of high fat diet and omega-3 treatment on weight gain, visceral fat depot, glycemia, plasma insulin and HOMA-IR

In order to assess whether HFD-induced obesity can interfere in animal recognition memory, object recognition test was performed. This task evaluates long-term memory through the ability to memorize and recognize both new and already known objects, 24 h after the training session (Caletti et al. 2015; Rossato et al. 2007). When comparing the total exploration time during the test session, there were no differences between groups (p > 0.05, Fig. 1A). However, when comparing the time exploring the familiar or new objects during the test session, a significant effect of object (familiar versus new) was found (F 1,42 = 4.932, p = 0.03). In the SD and SD + O groups, animals spent significantly more time exploring the new object during the test session (p < 0.05), which means that these rats recognized familiar and non-familiar objects. On the other hand, Bonferroni post-hoc showed that animals fed a HFD did not exhibit differences in the exploration time between familiar and new objects (p > 0.05) showing that obesity causes long-term memory impairment. These results also demonstrate that omega-3 did not exert a protective effect on the recognition memory (Fig.1B). We also analyzed the motor activity of the rats and found no differences between groups regarding the duration of locomotion (data not shown).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a. Total time spent exploring both objects during the object recognition test. No significant differences were found among experimental groups. b. Time spent exploring a familiar and a new object during the object recognition test. In SD and SD + O groups, animals spent more time exploring the new object during the retention session. HFD and HFD + O groups did not show differences in the exploration time of familiar and unfamiliar object. Omega-3 did not exert a protective effect on recognition memory. *p < 0.05, familiar versus new object. n = 7–8 for each group. Data are shown as mean ± SEM

GFAP was assessed by immunohistochemistry in order to determine the HFD effect on astrogliosis. GFAP is an astrocyte marker and GFAP labeling is widely used for the detection of tumors of astrocytic lineage (Liedtke et al. 1996), being the most widely used immunohistochemical marker for the identification of astrocytes (Catalani et al. 2002). Astrogliosis is represented by an increased number of GFAP-positive cells, which is found after several types of injury and it also indicates astrocytic activation and neuroinflammation (Kang et al. 2014; Sun et al. 2008; Toklu et al. 2017).

There was an increase in the number of GFAP-positive cells in the cerebral cortex, which is dependent on diet, showing that obesity leads to astrogliosis (F 1,60 = 9.369, p = 0.003) (Fig. 2). Omega-3 treatment decreased the number of GFAP-positive cells (treatment effect, F 1,60 = 8.550, p = 0.004) and Bonferroni post-hoc also showed that the number of positive cells in the HFD + O group was significantly lower than in the HFD group, which indicates that omega-3 may exert a protective role in the cerebral cortex by diminishing the activation state of astrocytes. Interestingly, there was no significant difference between groups in the number of GFAP-positive cell in the hippocampus (p > 0.05, Fig. 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

GFAP immunoreactivity in the cerebral cortex. The number of GFAP-positive cells was significantly higher in HFD group as shown in the representative photomicrographs of GFAP immunoreactivity in the cerebral cortex (left panel, lower magnification; right panel, higher magnification). Graph shows the number of GFAP-positive cells from two to three random fields analyzed from each of six nonadjacent sections of the cerebral cortex. *p < 0.05 when compared with HFD + O group. n = 5 animals for each group. Data are shown as mean ± SEM

Fig. 3
figure 3

GFAP immunoreactivity in the hippocampus. There was no differences in the number of GFAP-positive cells among groups as shown in the representative photomicrographs of GFAP immunoreactivity in the hippocampus (left panel, lower magnification; right panel, higher magnification). Graph shows the number of GFAP-positive cells from two to three random fields analyzed from each of six nonadjacent sections of the cerebral cortex. n = 5 animals for each group. Data are shown as mean ± SEM

The gene expression of the neuroinflammatory markers TNF-α, IL-6 and IL-10 was also determined in the cerebral cortex. We found a significant increase of the pro-inflammatory cytokines TNF-α (diet effect, F 1,29 = 5.844, p = 0.02; treatment effect, F 1,29 = 10.76, p = 0.002) and IL-6 (treatment effect, F 1,21 = 4.794, p = 0.04) gene expression in the HFD group. Moreover, HFD + O rats exhibited a reduced level of TNF-α and IL-6 compared to HFD rats, demonstrating that omega-3 may control obesity-induced neuroinflammation. The anti-inflammatory cytokine IL-10 did not show differences in gene expression between groups (p > 0.05, Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Gene expression of TNF-α, IL-6 and IL-10 in the cerebral cortex. Pro-inflammatory cytokines TNF-α and IL-6 gene expression increased in HFD group. IL-10 did not show differences among groups. *p < 0.05 versus HFD + O. n = 5–11 animals for each group. Data are shown as mean ± SEM

Discussion

In the present study, we showed that omega-3 supplementation is able to reverse insulin resistance and the increase in visceral adiposity caused by HFD. It is well known that increased visceral fat is a causal factor of metabolic syndrome (Després and Lemieux 2006). The development of visceral adipose tissue also contributes to a heightened secretion of pro-inflammatory mediators, contributing to the systemic state of inflammation found in obesity (Gregor and Hotamisligil 2011). Although we did not find any significant differences in plasma insulin and glycemia, HOMA-IR was significantly higher in the HFD group. Omega-3 supplementation ameliorates insulin sensitivity since the HFD + O group showed a similar HOMA-IR to rats fed a standard diet. Our findings are in agreement with previous studies that demonstrated a reduction in adiposity and an improvement in insulin sensitivity when a high fat diet is associated with omega-3 supplementation (de Sá et al. 2016; Rokling-Andersen et al. 2009; Storlien et al. 1987). In the present study, we showed that HFD induced greater weight gain than SD, independent of omega-3 administration. LeMieux et al. (2015) also reported that omega-3 did not interfere with total body weight in HFD-fed mice. However, they found a reduction in adipocyte size and adipogenesis following omega-3 supplementation (LeMieux et al. 2015). Then, omega-3 exerts an important role in the maintenance of metabolic tissues homeostasis.

In spite of the important metabolic repercussions of omega-3 supplementation, we could not show any improvement in long-term memory related to omega-3 treatment. Our findings are in agreement with previous studies showing memory impairment in experimental models of obesity in rodents. Molteni et al. (2002) reported spatial memory deficits in female rats 2 months after they were fed a high saturated fat and refined sugar diet (Molteni et al. 2002). A similar diet administered for 5 months produced deficits in spatial learning and long-term memory evaluated in the 8-arm radial maze in adult male rats (Ullrich et al. 2010). In our study, 20 weeks of HFD with 45% of energy from fat also promoted memory impairment, as shown by the object recognition task. However, shorter HFD administration is already known to be sufficient to induce cognitive deficits, as demonstrated elsewhere. It was previously shown that four to 5 weeks of a high fat diet also produces spatial memory deficits in young rats (Gergerlioglu et al. 2016; Pathan et al. 2008).

In our study, omega-3 was administered for 4 weeks after 16 weeks of HFD, in order to evaluate whether the effects caused by a HFD could be reversed by omega-3. In this case, the results have shown that omega-3 did not exert a protective effect on animal recognition memory. We can assume that 4 weeks of omega-3 administration was not sufficient to cause an improvement in the recognition memory of these animals following a HFD. It was shown that 12 weeks of administration of docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), a fatty acid from the omega-3 family, promotes an improvement in spatial cognition (Tanabe et al. 2004). Besides, in our study, we used a higher dosage of omega-3 for a shorter period of time. We can speculate that a lower dose of omega-3 over a longer period may have a more positive effect on memory. On the other hand, Sopian et al. (2015) found an improvement in cognitive performance after 3 weeks of fish oil supplementation, with a standard diet enriched with omega-3 PUFAs (Sopian et al. 2015). Another reasonable explanation for our results is that omega-3 is unable to recover the impairment in cognitive function caused by HFD. In Alzheimer’s disease transgenic mice, omega-3 treatment did not promote any beneficial effect on cognition, suggesting that the supplementation of these fatty acids does not protect against Alzheimer’s disease in susceptible individuals (Arendash et al. 2007). However, we cannot exclude their potential in the prevention of cognitive decline. When eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), also from the omega-3 family, was administered prior to amyloid-β peptide infusion, there was an improvement in the learning ability of rats. Since the deposition of amyloid-β peptide occurs mainly during aging, this finding indicates that omega-3 supplementation may be a strategy to prevent age-related cognitive decline (Hashimoto et al. 2009).

Increasing evidence demonstrates that besides cognitive impairment, HFD can promote insulin resistance, and immune and synaptic changes in different brain areas (Liu et al. 2015; Petrov et al. 2015). Such alterations may promote tissue lesions to which the astrocytes react, elevating GFAP expression. These changes are directly related to neuroinflammation, since astrocytes, in association with microglia, are able to release inflammatory mediators, directly interfering with the nervous system milieu (Theodosis et al. 2008). In our study, we did not address microglia since their recruitment and activation in diet-induced obesity is well demonstrated in the literature (Maldonado-Ruiz et al. 2017). It was also showed that omega-3 inhibits microglial response in neuroinflammatory conditions (Chang et al. 2015; Kurtys et al. 2016), including high-fat diet exposure (Oksman et al. 2006; Cintra et al. 2012; Johnson 2015). Although there have been some studies showing astrogliosis in obesity-induced neuroinflammation, less attention has been paid to these cell responses following a high-fat diet. Thus, in the present study, we focused on GFAP immunostaining to evaluate astrogliosis. In obesity, neuroinflammation is well described in the hypothalamus, which is a primary site where signals coming from the periphery arrive in the CNS (Cavadas et al. 2016; Miller and Spencer 2014). However, there are a few studies focusing on obesity and neuroinflammation in other brain regions. In the present study, we showed an increase in astrocytic activation in the cerebral cortex of the HFD group. However, when evaluating the effect of HFD on GFAP-immunoreactivity in the hippocampus, we did not find any difference between experimental groups. In mice, it was demonstrated that the hippocampal dysfunction caused by high-fat diet is dependent on strain (Xue et al. 2015). Wistar rats show conflicting results, demonstrating a decrease in GFAP immunostaining in the hippocampus following prolonged hypercaloric diet (Gzielo et al. 2016). On the other hand, 4 weeks of a high-fat diet did not change GFAP expression in either the cerebral cortex or the hippocampus (van der Harg et al. 2015). An increase in GFAP immunostaining was shown in transgenic obese rats (Zucker rats) in the cerebral cortex and hippocampus (Tomassoni et al. 2013). Additional studies are necessary to clarify these brain region differences in astrocyte responses to obesity. Thus, despite the conflicting results on the brain areas that exhibit astrocytic activation in response to obesity, certainly this is an important neuroinflammatory response in this condition.

In the present study, we observed that omega-3 exerts a protective effect in the brain by decreasing astrogliosis in the cerebral cortex of the HFD + O group. This finding demonstrates that even for a short period of supplementation, omega-3 is able to exert anti-inflammatory function. In addition to interfering in the inflammation processes, omega-3 can be incorporated into the phospholipids of inflammatory cell membranes, altering inflammatory processes. This change in fatty acid composition allows membranes to maintain its fluidity and to modify the formation of lipid derivatives, being able to influence, in this way, the function of cells involved in the inflammation (Yaqoob and Shaikh 2010). The role of omega-3 PUFAs on the inhibition of nuclear factor κB (NFκB) activation, which is a pro-inflammatory transcription factor, has been reported (Calder 2015). Also, omega-3 can positively modulate an anti-inflammatory gene expression by activating the peroxisome proliferator activated receptor γ (PPARγ) (Heras-Sandoval et al. 2016). In microglial cells, omega-3 treatment decreases the LPS-induced release of TNF-a and IL-6. This effect is mediated through activation of the sirtuin-1 (SIRT1) pathway, which causes NF-kB inhibition due to p65 deacetylation (Inoue et al. 2017). DHA anti-inflammatory effect was also showed in astrocytes followed by saturated fatty acids treatment (Gupta et al. 2012). Thus, these fatty acids have the ability to shift a pro-inflammatory M1 phenotype to M2, which is anti-inflammatory.

The anti-inflammatory effects of omega-3 PUFAs are well known. In obesity, omega-3 prevents macrophage infiltration and decreases inflammatory gene expression in adipose tissue (Todoric et al. 2006). Labrousse et al., found that a 2-month treatment with EPA/DHA prevented the increase in pro-inflammatory cytokine expression in the hippocampus (Labrousse et al. 2012). In the present study, we found an increased mRNA expression of pro-inflammatory cytokines TNF-α and IL-6 in the cerebral cortex after HFD. This pro-inflammatory profile in the brain caused by obesity is already shown in previous studies (Sánchez-Sarasúa et al. 2016; Toyama et al. 2015). Astrocytes and microglia are both important sources of cytokines such as TNF-α and IL-6 during pathological conditions (Baek et al. 2016; Urrutia et al. 2017). In the present study, it was not possible to confirm whether the increased pro-inflammatory cytokine expression is attributed to microglia or astrocytes. However, we believe that astrocytic activation may importantly contribute to this finding, since we also showed a concomitant increase in the number of GFAP-positive cells in the cerebral cortex after HFD. In addition, the neuroinflammatory status has been related to cognitive decline in obesity (Miller and Spencer 2014). In the present study, omega-3 supplementation following the induction of obesity was able to restrain the increase in cytokine expression in the cerebral cortex. Despite this, omega-3 did not improve long-term memory. This result is probably explained by the fact that omega-3 acts by preventing inflammation and consequently, cognitive decline, but it is not able to revert an already established neuronal and/or synaptic damage. This assumption is reinforced by studies focused on aging, since there is a neuroinflammatory profile in this situation.

In conclusion, the supplementation of omega-3 fatty acid for 4 weeks following HFD was able to improve insulin sensitivity, decrease visceral adiposity and diminish the neuroinflammatory profile. These results emphasize the anti-inflammatory role of this family of PUFAs, not only in the periphery, but also in the CNS. However, the lack of a protective effect on the long-term recognition memory after HFD suggests that omega-3 fatty acids are not able to revert already established cognitive impairment. Thus, omega-3 supplementation plays an important role to prevent rather than revert neuronal function impairment.