Introduction

As a period characterized by salient neurobiological changes, the adolescent years present a great threat of risk-taking behavior (Steinberg, 2010). One such risk-taking behavior is substance use, which is a major public health concern as it is often linked to long-term difficulties with health and well-being, including heightened mental health concerns such as depression (Walters et al., 2018) and premature involvement in risky sexual behavior (Benotsch et al., 2013). Among the risk factors associated with the use of any substance, alcohol use in particular has been linked to neurocognitive impairments including alterations in the development of grey and white matter, attention and verbal learning, and visuospatial processing (Spear, 2018) as well as prefrontal cortex functioning involved in cognitive control which is critical to risky decision making such as substance use (Feldstein Ewing et al., 2015; Squeglia et al., 2009). Further, during adolescence, peers typically replace parents as adolescents’ main source of social support (Rosenthal & Kobak, 2010), and friendships during adolescence become increasingly important, providing companionship and a sense of self-worth (Erdley & Day, 2017). Some adolescents thrive in this new social environment, gaining widespread acceptance, whereas other adolescents find this to be a period of alienation, having experienced low-quality friendships, rejection, and even peer victimization (Erdley & Day, 2017). As discussed in the following sections, social relationship risk factors are a common catalyst for the use of substances, both in young people who have high social involvement, as well as in those who are alienated from their peers (Copeland et al., 2018; Moody et al., 2011). However, there remains a gap in the literature regarding protective factors that interface with social relationship risk factors. Thus, the present study examines whether religiousness buffers against longitudinal links between social relationship risk factors and substance use development.

Social Involvement and Substance Use

Research has demonstrated associations between social involvement and substance use such that the more popular an adolescent is among peers, the greater the likelihood of substance use (Ali et al., 2014; Moody et al., 2011). While social involvement during adolescence is often a sign of healthy adjustment, it also plays a dual function as a risk factor for deviant behavior. Bandura’s social learning theory (Bandura & McClelland, 1977) offers some explanation for a positive link between social involvement and substance use among adolescents, as it emphasizes that the social environment has the potential to affect behavior through modeling. If adolescents observe their peers showing favorable attitudes towards substance use or engaging in substance use, then they are more likely to take part in such behaviors themselves (Trucco, 2020). Peer socialization may be responsible for social learning of substance use, in that affiliation with delinquent or substance-using peers amplifies substance use in adolescence and young adulthood (Otten et al., 2017; Van Ryzin et al., 2012). In particular, Allen et al. (2005) provided evidence supporting a popularity-socialization hypothesis demonstrating that popular adolescents exhibit behavior that adheres to peer norms, resulting in increased substance use behaviors. Further, socially involved adolescents may have easier access to substances because they engage in social situations and peer interactions more frequently. Indeed, a primary source through which adolescents obtain alcohol is social resources, such as a friend or older sibling of legal age (Friese et al., 2013). Taken together, socially involved adolescents may be subject to social influences such as peer modeling and increased access to alcohol, explaining the greater prevalence of alcohol use among socially involved youth.

Social Alienation and Substance Use

Though socially involved adolescents are more likely to engage in substance use, somewhat ironically, the same also rings true for socially alienated adolescents. That is, adolescents who experience alienation from their peers have been found to engage in higher levels of substance use (Copeland et al., 2018). A possible explanation for this association could be the use of substances as a coping mechanism for social relationships that induce negative affect, such as loneliness (Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). Loneliness is an emotional experience, characterized by sadness or dejection due to lack of companionship, which is especially salient during adolescence and young adulthood due to the rapid social changes that define this period. Indeed, prior research suggests that social isolation is associated with self-medication to cope with feelings of loneliness (Osgood et al., 2014). Neuroimaging research provides further insights into the association between emotional pain derived from negative social relationships and the use of substances for self-medication by demonstrating a shared activation in the brain between physical and social pain. Specifically, the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula, areas of the brain that respond to physical pain, were activated during a computer game that simulated social rejection (Eisenberger et al., 2003, Eisenberger, 2012), demonstrating that pain stemming from social relationships stimulated the same regions of the brain as physical pain, thus supporting the notion that substances may be used as a form of self-medication against the pain of social alienation.

Indeed, research suggests that substance use may be a form of self-medication for those whose alienation is associated with mental health concerns. For example, social anxiety and depression have been shown to be both predictors (Biggs et al., 2012; Maughan et al., 2013) and outcomes (Katz et al., 2012; Tillfors et al., 2012) of social alienation. In turn, cross-sectional studies indicate that young people who experience mental health concerns (such as social anxiety and depression) self-medicate through the use of substances (Lemyre et al., 2019; Tomlinson & Brown, 2012). Taken together, as young people who are socially alienated may experience both feelings of loneliness as well as related mental health problems, they may use substances as a coping mechanism to alleviate these emotional concerns.

Religiousness as a Protective Factor

Given the long-term detrimental effects of substance use during adolescence (Morin et al., 2019), research identifying protective factors that may modify developmental processes underlying this association is crucial. One such factor is religiousness, which has been identified as deterring maladjustment outcomes during adolescence, including substance use (see Holmes & Kim-Spoon, 2016 for a review). Prior research has shown that more religious adolescents are less likely to engage in risk-taking behaviors, such as substance use, risky sexual behaviors, and criminal activity (Kim‐Spoon et al., 2015; Pirutinsky, 2014). Theoretically, protective effects of religiousness are in part due to its faciliatory effect on self-regulation (McCullough & Willoughby, 2009). Indeed, research has shown that more religious adolescents show better self-regulation abilities (Kim-Spoon et al., 2014b; Holmes et al., 2019). Further, there is evidence suggesting buffering roles of religiousness against detrimental effects of parental and peer risk factors (i.e., substance-using peers and harsh parenting) on substance use behaviors, in that these behaviors were attenuated among adolescents with higher religiousness compared to those with lower religiousness (Kim-Spoon et al., 2014a; Peviani et al., 2019). However, prior work examining the moderation between religiousness and substance use focused on parental relationships and peer substance use, and this study fills a gap in literature by examining the moderating effect of religiousness on the association between social relationship factors and alcohol use. Here, it is proposed that more religious adolescents may be better prepared to resist social pressures toward substance use (grown out of social involvement) as well as resist self-medication urges to cope with negative affect (grown out of social alienation).

The present study examined potentially differential roles of two distinct dimensions of religiousness: organizational and personal. Organizational religiousness encompasses an adolescent’s involvement in religious institutions (e.g. attending services or youth group) and personal religiousness encompasses the importance of faith to the adolescent’s life. Different theoretical accounts explain why organizational versus personal religiousness would affect negative health behaviors such as substance use. In the reformulated social control theory, Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) proposed that the root of delinquent behaviors is low self-control, rather than social control. According to this perspective, youth should be taught the rules of their societies by their parents and other adults to understand the full range of the consequences of their behaviors. Thus, strongly religious adolescents are more likely to internalize these rules and values through their relationships with older adults in their religious communities, and thus they are less likely to use substances facing social pressures and social rejections. Further, being an active participant in religion allows young people to define their own personal religiousness, or the personal importance of their faith. The divine interaction theory provides accounts for the beneficial effects of personal religiousness by suggesting that religious people create relationships with the divine similar to how they would form social relationships, seeking out guidance and solace from a divine being in private (Ellison, 1991; Pollner, 1989). It follows that as adolescents and young adults strengthen their personal relationship with the divine, they are expected to receive support from this relationship in a way that helps them to resist social pressures and cope with social rejections so that they are less likely to turn to substance use.

Current Study

The current study used longitudinal prospective data to examine associations between social relationships (i.e., social involvement and social alienation) and alcohol use during adolescence and into young adulthood. Factors promoting resilience towards social relationship risks were considered by testing the protective role of organizational and personal religiousness within this association. Specifically, it is hypothesized that the link between social relationships and alcohol use may be moderated by religiousness, such that the associations between social relationships and subsequent alcohol use will be weaker for those with higher organizational and personal religiousness, even after controlling for the contribution of parent–adolescent relationship quality (a protective factor within a family context) and other demographic covariates.

Methods

Participants

The sample used in the present study consisted of 167 adolescents (53% males) from a southeastern state in the United States of America who participated in five annual assessments across six years (with a two-year gap between Time 4 and Time 5). Adolescents were 13 to 14 years of age at Time 1 (M = 14.07, SD = 0.54 for Time 1, M = 15.05, SD = 0.54 for Time 2, M = 16.07, SD = 0.56 for Time 3, and M = 17.01, SD = 0.55 for Time 4, and M = 18.39, SD = 0.67 for Time 5). About 78% of adolescents identified as Caucasian, 14% African–American, 6% as more than one race, and 2% as other. The median annual family income was in the $35,000–$50,000 range, with varying levels of family economic status (50% “poor/near poor” and 50% “non-poor” according to income-to-needs ratio). At Time 1, 157 families participated. At Time 2, 10 families were added for a final sample of 167 parent–adolescent dyads. However, 24 families (14%) did not participate at all possible time points for reasons including: ineligibility for tasks (n = 2), declined participation (n = 17), and lost contact (n = 5) during the follow-up assessments. Attrition analyses using univariate General Linear Modeling (GLM) were performed to determine the presence of systematic predictors of missing data. Results indicated that rate of participation (indexed by proportion of years participated to years invited to participate) was not significantly predicted by age, income, sex, or race (ps > 0.307).

Procedures

Data included in the present study was collected as part of a larger longitudinal project. Adolescent participants and their primary caregivers were recruited via flyers, email announcements, and snowball sampling (word-of-mouth). Data collection was administered at university offices where participants completed a combination of self-report questionnaires, behavioral and neuroimaging tasks, and interviews by experienced research assistants. The study sessions lasted on average five hours and participants were compensated monetarily for their time. All procedures were approved by the institutional review board of the university and all participants gave written informed consent or assent.

Measures

Social involvement

Social involvement was measured at Times 1–4 via six items that make up the Social Scale of the Youth Self-Report (YSR; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). Adolescents reported (1) the names of organizations, teams or other activities they are involved in, (2) their average participation in each of the listed activities compared to others using a three-point scale including “less than average”, “average” and “more than average”, (3) the number of their close friends using a four-point scale including “none”, “1”, “2 or 3”, and “4 or more”, as well as, (4) frequency of contact with friends outside of school, (5) how well they get along with friends, siblings, and parents, and (6) how well they did things alone compared to others using a three point-scale including “worse”, “average”, and “better”. T-scores of the Social Scale each time were used in the analyses. Internal consistency was relatively low (α ranges from 0.43 to 0.52 across Times 1–4 in the current sample) but consistent with what has previously been reported by Achenbach and Rescorla (2001) (α = 0.55). The low reliability may reflect the nature of the variable, which can be seen as an index variable (Streiner, 2003). That is, the variable has multiple indicators that reflect social involvement in different areas. For instance, an adolescent having multiple close friends may not necessarily indicate that he or she would be a member of multiple organizations, yet both clearly fit within the construct of social involvement.

Social Alienation

Social alienation was measured at Times 1- 4 via the Peer Alienation subscale of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). Adolescents responded to four statements regarding their feelings of alienation from their peers using a five-point Likert scale from “1 = Almost Never or Never True” to “5 = Almost Always or Always True”. Sample items include “My friends don’t understand what I’m going through these days” and “I feel alone or apart when I am with my friends”. The mean score was calculated to create an overall alienation score, such that higher scores indicated greater feelings of alienation (α ranges from 0.60 to 0.72 across Times 1–4; all factor loadings of the confirmatory factor analyses were significant and greater than 0.44).

Religiousness

Religiousness was measured using adolescent self-report on organizational and personal religiousness subscales at Times 1–4 using six items from: the Multidimensional Measure of Religiousness/ Spirituality (Fetzer & NIA, 1999) and Jessor’s Value on Religion Scale (Jessor & Jessor, 1977). Two subscales of religiousness, organizational and personal, were used. Organizational religiousness was measured by averaging two items reflecting participation in organized religious activities (e.g., how often they attend religious services; α ranges from 0.83 to 0.87 across Times 1–4; all factor loadings of the confirmatory factor analyses were significant and greater than 0.64). Personal religiousness was assessed by averaging four items indicating the importance of religious faith (e.g., how important they think it is “to believe in God”; α ranges from 0.69 to 0.74 across Times 1–4, all factor loadings of the confirmatory factor analyses were significant and greater than 0.88).

Alcohol use

Alcohol use was measured at Times 1–5 with a question asking a typical frequency: “Which is most true for you about using alcohol?” using a 6-point Likert scale ranging from “1 = never used” to “6 = usually use every day” (α = 0.70 at Time 5).

Sex

At Time 1, adolescents completed a demographic interview which included a question asking for their biological sex, with 0 representing male and 1 representing female.

Race

At Time 1, adolescents completed a demographic interview which included a question asking for their race. This variable was then dummy coded into 0 representing White and 1 representing Non-White.

Income-to-needs ratio

At Time 1, caregivers completed a demographic interview which included questions about their and their spouse’s (if applicable) income. Total household income before taxes for the previous year was used to calculate an income-to-needs (ITN) ratio for each family. Specifically, income was divided by the poverty threshold for the given family size (according to guidelines by the U.S. Census Bureau).

Parent–adolescent relationship quality

Parent–adolescent relationship quality was added as a covariate in the models. Parent–adolescent relationship quality was measured using adolescent self-report via the mean of Parent subscale of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (Armsden & Greenberg, 1987). Adolescents responded to 12 items each regarding their feelings of attachment to both their mother and father using a five-point Likert scale from “1 = Almost Never or Never True” to “5 = Almost Always or Always True”. Sample items include “My mother/father helps me understand myself better” and “I wish I had a different mother/father”. The mean of the mother and father attachment scores was taken to represent overall parent-adolescent relationship quality at each time point, and the grand mean was calculated across Time 1–4, such that higher scores indicate a more supportive relationship (mean α for mother across Times 1–4 = 0.86; mean α for father across Times 1–4 = 0.88).

Data Analytic Plan

For all variables, descriptive statistics were used to assess for outliers and normal distributions. Skewness and kurtosis were also examined, and levels less than 3 and 10, respectively, were considered acceptable (Kline, 2011). All study variables demonstrated acceptable levels of skewness and kurtosis. Outliers (N = 6) were identified as values deviating more than 3.29 SD from the mean and were Winsorized to retain statistical power and attenuate bias resulting from elimination.

Latent Change Score (LCS) Modeling (McArdle & Hamagami, 2001) using Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) in Mplus statistical software version 8 (Muthén & Muthén, 19982018) was used to predict dynamic changes in alcohol use from repeatedly measured social relationships. One clear advantage of the LCS model was to estimate time-based dynamic relations, where the effect on change in one variable (i.e., alcohol use) depends on the state of another variable (i.e., social relationship) as well as any prior change within the system over time (i.e., proportional change which cannot be represented by other longitudinal models such as multivariate growth curve models). Additionally, the LCS model represents changes in perfectly reliable scores over a time series by partitioning true scores from measurement error, reducing the likelihood of biased change parameters while enhancing power. The hypothesized LCS model included the time-varying predictor of social relationship to examine how earlier social involvement and social alienation would predict subsequent changes in alcohol use. Further, whether religiousness moderates the associations between social acceptance and social alienation at Times 1–4 and alcohol use at Times 1–5 was tested by using a two-group SEM with the low and high religiousness groups (n = 85 low, n = 82 high for organizational religiousness, n = 84 low, n = 83 high for personal religiousness, defined by a median split). A grand mean of the religiousness variables (average of Times 1–4) was used to capture longitudinal levels of religiousness (moderator) to create high versus low religiousness groups, separately for organizational and personal religiousness.

Model fit was assessed by χ2 value, degrees of freedom, corresponding p-value, Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Confirmatory Fit Index (CFI). For RMSEAs, values less than 0.05 and 0.08 were taken to reflect good and acceptable fits, respectively; and the CFI values greater than 0.90 and 0.95 were taken to reflect acceptable and good fits, respectively (Little, 2013). Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML) estimation with robust standard errors (MLR) was used as it is known to account for missing data (three scores of substance use were missing) and non-normal distributions better than maximum likelihood (ML) estimation.

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlations for study variables are presented in Table 1. In the hypothesized LCS models, associations between social involvement/alienation and alcohol use were tested by estimating the effects of social relationships from Time 1 through Time 4 (manifest variables representing time series data) on changes in alcohol use from Time 1 through Time 5, separately for social involvement and social alienation (see Fig. 1 for conceptual model).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations for social involvement, social alienation, alcohol use, organizational religiousness, and personal religiousness
Fig. 1
figure 1

Latent Change Score Model of the Moderation of Religiousness between Social Relationships and Alcohol Use. Note. SR = social relationship; AL = alcohol use; T1 = Time 1 (age 14); T2 = Time 2 (age 15); T3 = Time 3 (age 16); T4 = Time 4 (age 17); T5 = Time 5 (age 18). Demographic variables sex, race, income, and parent-adolescent relationship quality are included but not depicted in conceptual model for clarity of presentation

Univariate Latent Change Score Modeling of Alcohol Use

The univariate LCS model of alcohol use change did not display acceptable model fit (χ2 = 19.53, df = 5, p < 0.001, RMSEA = 0.13, CFI = 0.94). To further improve the model fit based on modification indices, a residual correlation between alcohol use Time 2 and Time 4, was added, and the variances of alcohol use Time 1 and Time 5 were fixed to 0 to account for small, nonsignificant negative residual variances. The resulting model fit was good (χ2 = 5.12, df = 6, p = 0.528, RMSEA = 0.00, CFI = 1.00). The mean (M = 1.38, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001) and variance (σ2 = 0.34, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001) of the intercept factor were significant. Further, proportional changes were positive and significant (b = 0.42, SE = 0.13, p = 0.001), indicating that higher alcohol use at an earlier time point predicted subsequent increases in alcohol use. Next, a two-group univariate LCS models of alcohol use was tested.

The two-group LCS model of alcohol use by organizational religiousness demonstrated good fit (χ2 = 10.20, df = 12, p = 0.598, RMSEA = 0.00, CFI = 1.00). In both the low and high groups, the means (M = 1.43, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001 for the low group; M = 1.25, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001 for the high group) and variances (σ2 = 0.32, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001 for the low group; σ2 = 0.22, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001 for the high group) of the intercept factor were significant. Further, proportional changes were positive and significant in the high group (b = 0.64, SE = 0.23, p = 0.005), but not significant in the low group (b = 0.21, SE = 0.14, p = 0.138).

The two-group LCS model of alcohol use by personal religiousness demonstrated good fit (χ2 = 11.15, df = 12, p = 0.516, RMSEA = 0.00, CFI = 1.00). In both the low and high groups, the means (M = 1.43, SE = 0.06, p < 0.001 for the low group; M = 1.33, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001 for the high group) and variances (σ2 = 0.32, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001 for the low group; σ2 = 0.35, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001 for the high group) of the intercept factor were significant. Further, proportional changes were positive and significant in both the low group (b = 0.41, SE = 0.18, p = 0.022) and the high group (b = 0.50, SE = 0.19, p = 0.009).

Longitudinal Associations of Social Involvement and Social Alienation with Alcohol Use Moderated by Religiousness

Next, the hypothesis that religiousness plays a protective role against the detrimental effects of social relationship risk factors on alcohol use was tested using a two-group SEM based on the high vs. low religious groups. Specifically, religiousness was tested as a moderator for longitudinal associations between social relationships (social involvement and social alienation) and alcohol use for a total of four models. In order to test possible covariates, adolescent sex (0 = male, 1 = female) and race (0 = white and 1 = non-white), income-to-needs ratio, and parent-adolescent relationship quality were added to all four moderation models.

Organizational Religiousness as a Moderator

The two-group LCS models of the moderation of organizational religiousness between social involvement and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 83.09, df = 64, p = 0.055, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.94). As shown in Table 2, there were no significant associations between social involvement nor covariates and change in alcohol use.

Table 2 Latent change score model of the moderation of organizational religiousness between social relationships and alcohol use

The two-group LCS models of the moderation of organizational religiousness between social alienation and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 65.35, df = 64, p = 0.430, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 1.00). As can be seen in Table 2, adolescents with low organizational religiousness demonstrated a significant positive association between social alienation at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, and a significant positive association between social alienation at Time 4 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 4 to Time 5 indicating that high social alienation was associated with greater increases in alcohol use. Significant group differences were found for Time 4 social alienation effects on alcohol use change from Time 4 to Time 5 (Wald test χ2 = 5.00, df = 1, p = 0.025). However, testing numerical invariance for the significant social alienation effects between the two groups did not indicate significant group differences for Time 2 social alienation effects (Wald test χ2 = 0.81, df = 1, p = 0.369).

In the social alienation model, some effects of family income and parent-adolescent relationship quality were found (see Table 2). In the low religiousness group, there was a significant negative association between parent-adolescent relationship quality at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, indicating that lower relationship quality was associated with greater increases in alcohol use. In the high religiousness group, there was a significant positive association between income-to-needs ratio at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, indicating that higher family income was associated with greater increases in alcohol use. In addition, there was a significant positive association between parent-adolescent relationship quality at Time 4 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 4 to Time 5, indicating that higher parent-adolescent relationship quality was associated with greater changes in alcohol use. Thus, the data indicated differential effects of the parent-adolescent relationship quality between low and high religiousness groups.

Personal Religiousness as a Moderator

The two-group LCS of the moderation of personal religiousness between social involvement and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 88.83, df = 63, p = 0.018, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.92). As can be seen in Table 3, there were no significant associations between social involvement and change in alcohol use.

Table 3 Latent change score model of the moderation of personal religiousness between social relationships and alcohol use

The two-group LCS of the moderation of personal religiousness between social alienation and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 81.90, df = 62, p = 0.046, RMSEA = 0.06, CFI = 0.94). As can be seen in Table 3, adolescents with low personal religiousness demonstrated a significant positive association between social alienation at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, and a significant positive association between social alienation at Time 4 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 4 to Time 5 indicating that high social alienation was associated with greater increases in alcohol use. However, testing numerical invariance indicated non-significant group differences for Time 2 (Wald test χ2 = 0.581, df = 1, p = 0.446) or Time 4 (Wald test χ2 = 2.208, df = 1, p = 0.154) social alienation effects, thus these results should be interpreted with caution.

As can be seen in Table 3, in both the social involvement and the social alienation models, there was a significant positive association between income-to-needs ratio at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3 in the high personal religiousness group, indicating that higher income was associated with greater increases in alcohol use. Additionally, in the social alienation model, there was a significant negative association between parent–adolescent relationship quality at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3 in the low religiousness group, indicating that lower relationship quality was associated with greater increases in alcohol use.

Sensitivity Analyses

An alternative approach was taken to examine the robustness of the findings. In contrast to a median split as used in the primary analysis, the data were split by level of religiousness by contrasting the lowest 25% of religiousness (n = 40 for organizational religiousness; n = 42 for personal religiousness) against the remaining 75% (n = 127 for organizational religiousness; n = 125 for personal religiousness) to evaluate whether the buffering effects of religiousness are beneficial to most people while leaving those with notably low levels of religiousness particularly vulnerable to increasing use of alcohol. Results of these analyses are presented in Appendix A.

First, for organizational religiousness, the two-group LCS of social involvement and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 95.64, df = 64, p = 0.006, RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.90). As can be seen in Table 4, consistent with the two-group LCS based on the median split, there were no significant associations between social involvement and change in alcohol use. The two-group LCS of the moderation of organizational religiousness between social alienation and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 96.49, df = 64, p = 0.005, RMSEA = 0.08, CFI = 0.89), and findings were consistent with the two-group LCS based on the median split. Specifically, as can be seen in Table 4, adolescents with low organizational religiousness demonstrated a significant positive association between social alienation at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, and a marginally significant association between social alienation at Time 4 and alcohol use change from Time 4 to Time 5.

Next, for personal religiousness, the two-group LCS of the moderation of personal religiousness between social involvement and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 90.25, df = 63, p = 0.014, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.91). As can be seen in Table 5, consistent with the two-group LCS based on the median split, there were no significant associations between social involvement and change in alcohol use. The two-group LCS of the moderation of personal religiousness between social alienation and alcohol use change displayed acceptable fit (χ2 = 86.99, df = 62, p = 0.020, RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0.92), and the findings were consistent with the two-group LCS based on the median split. Specifically, significant positive associations were found between social alienation at Time 2 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 2 to Time 3, as well as between social alienation at Time 4 and the subsequent change in alcohol use from Time 4 to Time 5 (see Table 5).

To summarize, results from sensitivity analyses solidified how religiousness and the risk factor of social alienation interface to predict developmental changes in substance use behaviors during adolescence into young adulthood, although statistical significance (i.e., p levels) was not always consistent with the main analysis results, likely due to the nature of decreased sample size for the lowest religiousness group (i.e., 25% of the sample).

Discussion

Adolescence is a developmental period characterized by heightened susceptibility to social influences, particularly by peers (Albert et al., 2013). Adolescents are more sensitive to acceptance and rejection by peers, and their risk-taking behaviors are also influenced by peers (Dishion & Tipsord, 2011). Yet, some adolescents are more vulnerable than others to the effects of social relationships. As such, the identification of factors that may buffer against adverse effects of social relationship risks is helpful in informing preventive intervention efforts to reduce adolescent substance use and prevent its associated outcomes (Spear, 2018). The present longitudinal study documents the protective role of religiousness in the link between the effects of social relationship risk factors (both being involved with and alienated from social relationships) on substance use across adolescence into young adulthood.

The effect of social alienation on alcohol use was consistent with prior work suggesting a link between loneliness and social isolation with greater substance use (Copeland et al., 2018; Osgood et al., 2014). Importantly, the findings of the function of social relationships align with Cooper’s (1994) four factor model of alcohol use motivators: Those who are socially alienated with high alcohol use can be internally motivated by coping drinking motives (e.g. to forget about their problems) or enhancement motives (e.g., to get high). Internally motivated alcohol users have different clinical implications. For example, drinking as a form of self-medication is often done alone. These solitary drinkers—who are internally motivated—are more concerning, as solitary drinking motivated by the need to cope with emotional pain presents a greater risk of substance use disorders in later life (Mason et al., 2020; Skrzynski & Creswell, 2020). It follows that identifying protective factors against social alienation related drinking is particularly important.

However, the nonsignificant effect of social involvement on alcohol use was not in line with previous research indicating a link between sociometric popularity and substance use culminating in socially motivated drinking behaviors (Ali et al., 2014; Allen et al., 2005; Hussong et al., 2020). The discrepancy between the previous and the current findings may be due to the nature of the social involvement measure used in the current study that mainly captured involvement in groups as well as number of friends and frequency of contacts with friends. Our finding suggests that these quantitative aspects of social involvement may not be a critical predictor of alcohol use. Instead, as alluded by social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), adolescents’ risky decision making may depend on the social norms espoused by their social groups such that the attitude that those friends and groups hold toward alcohol use may matter more than quantity of social interactions with respect to influencing adolescents’ alcohol use behaviors.

The study hypothesized that both organizational and personal religiousness would act as a protective factor against social relationship effects on alcohol use, and the findings provide supportive evidence of the protective role of religiousness. Specifically, the alcohol use of adolescents who were low in organizational and personal religiousness was more heavily impacted by the social relationship risk factor of alienation than those high in organizational and personal religiousness. Organizational religiousness is characterized by an adolescent’s engagement in religious institutions, such as attending church services or youth group. The reduced social alienation effects among adolescents with high organizational religiousness are consistent with the reformulated social control theory, as strongly religious adolescents are more likely to internalize these rules and values through their relationships with older adults in their religious communities (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990) and thus are less likely to engage in substance use as a coping mechanism.

Personal religiousness is characterized by the personal importance of faith to an adolescent. Consistent with the divine interaction theory (Ellison, 1991; Pollner, 1989), an adolescent who has high personal religiousness forms a relationship with a divine being similar to that of a social relationship, thus seeking out guidance and solace from the divine. The reduced social alienation effects among adolescents with high personal religiousness suggest that their experience of a supportive relationship with a divine being can then help them to resist desires to drink driven by coping motives, such as drinking to achieve the negative, internally generated reinforcement of forgetting pain from feeling lonely. The current finding of the buffering role of personal religiousness is also consistent with prior work suggesting the role of personal religiousness in promoting self-regulation and in turn reducing substance use among adolescents (Kim‐Spoon et al., 2015).

Collectively, our findings suggest that both an adolescent’s frequent involvement in religious institutions, as well as an adolescent’s personal relationship with a divine being, serve as protective factors against alcohol use fueled by the risk factor of social alienation. However, it is important to note that, because the rigorous numeric invariance tests of group differences regarding the social alienation effects at Time 2 for organizational religiousness and the social alienation effects at Time 2 and at Time 4 for personal religiousness were not statistically significant, the results should be interpreted with caution and replication of the findings is warranted.

In light of extant literature implying potential developmental differences with respect to the influences of social relationships, it is important to examine longitudinal analyses to explore differential timing of social relationships contributing to substance use behaviors. The pattern of findings suggested significant effects of social alienation in early adolescence as well as late adolescence into young adulthood. Early adolescence is a time characterized by both social and school changes, as youth transition from middle to high school, spend more time with peers than they do parents, and are exposed to social groups with older peers (Blum et al., 2014). Similarly, late adolescence into young adulthood is a time characterized by social role transitions, including graduating high school and going to college or joining the workforce or military (Shanahan, 2000). These social transitions open the door to increased substance use behaviors within the social context, especially for those who feel alienated from their peers. Stress during a time of social transition (e.g., starting a high school or leaving home to attend college) can prompt self-medication via alcohol use as a way of coping (Creswell et al., 2014; Tomlinson & Brown, 2012). Further, in line with existing literature indicating that the transition to high school is a time often associated with increased loneliness (Benner et al., 2017) and that young adults are particularly sensitive to the effects of loneliness and social isolation (Murthy, 2020), the data suggest that the influence of social alienation on alcohol use is prominent during high school years and for those transitioning into young adulthood.

The contributions of the current study should be considered in light of several limitations. First, study variables were assessed through self-reports. There is evidence that self-report measures are particularly revealing for behaviors that are related to private or internal experience (e.g., Kendall et al., 1989) such as certain aspects of religiousness, alcohol use, and social relationships. Yet, the associations among the study variables may have been enhanced due to method variance and within-subject bias, thus replications of the findings utilizing multiple-informant multi-method assessments are warranted. In particular, sociometric assessment of popularity may represent some important aspects of social involvement (e.g., popularity) in relation to alcohol use. Further, including an assessment of peer substance use or peer delinquency to capture the nuanced role of social involvement would enhance clearer understanding of the role of adolescents’ social network. Indeed, a rich body of empirical work suggests that affiliation with substance-using peers increases substance use throughout adolescence and young adulthood (e.g., Otten et al., 2017). Second, it is important to state the potential limitations of the sample used in this study. The sample involved a homogeneously aged sample of both male and female adolescents who were annually assessed throughout adolescence. Such an intensive longitudinal data collection provided suitable data for examining fast-moving changes in social relationships and substance use behaviors. Yet, given the relatively small sample size and the nature of being regionally representative, future research should replicate the results with larger, nationally representative samples.

These limitations notwithstanding, the current study had notable strengths, including five waves of data with a prospective design that allowed testing timing of the social relationship effects and latent change score models that permitted more precise estimation of the dynamic change in alcohol use affected by preceding social relationship risk factors. The findings have implications useful for health professionals to assist youth at risk for substance use. First, one way to utilize the social effects toward preventing alcohol use is to guide young people to healthy social affiliations. For example, studies using at-risk adult populations have reported positive effects of social network intervention in which participants examine their own social networks to identify people who trigger their substance use in comparison to people who support healthier choices (Kennedy et al., 2018), and actively choose to spend more time with the latter (Eddie & Kelly, 2017). Second, the data provide preliminary support suggesting important roles of religiousness in shaping substance use behaviors among young people. For example, positive religious coping mechanisms (e.g., seeking social support from faith groups) have been found to be associated with positive affect and greater life satisfaction in adolescents (Van Dyke et al., 2009). Indeed, this is consistent with the use of religious and spiritual practices in groups such as Alcoholics Anonymous that have provided strong evidence for the potential of religious values in overcoming substance use (Hahn, 2019) as well as loneliness (Murthy, 2020).

Conclusion

Within the current literature, the moderating effect of religiousness on specifically peer-related social relationship factors is not well known. The present study presents a longitudinal investigation seeking to elucidate how social relationship risk factors and religiousness protective factors interface to predict developmental changes in alcohol use during adolescence and into young adulthood. The findings demonstrate protective effects of organizational and personal religiousness against the detrimental effect of social alienation risk factor that promotes alcohol use during adolescence into young adulthood. The results highlight the protective roles of religiousness acting as additional sources of social engagement experiences to modulate the effects of social alienation predicting alcohol use progression and provide further evidence for the positive impact religiousness has on healthy adolescent development.