Although Erikson (1968) contended that adolescents are focused on the task of identity development, he noted that this complicated task often extends into the twenties. Arnett (2015) argues that 18–29 years constitutes a distinct life stage known as emerging adulthood, which is characterized by five features: identity exploration, instability, self-focus, feeling in-between, and possibilities/optimism. With delayed entrance into adult roles of marriage and parenthood (Arnett 2015), these emerging adults, particularly those attending universities, engage in heightened exploration of their values and beliefs through curricular and co-curricular activities, including their religiousness and spirituality (R/S).

Although scholars have examined the processes that promote children’s and adolescents’ R/S (Boyatzis 2013), less is known about emerging adults’ R/S (e.g., Barry and Abo-Zena 2014). According to the Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life (2012), emerging adults claim their religious beliefs are less important across this decade than earlier, whereas their spiritual beliefs tend to increase during this time, particularly for women (Astin et al. 2011). In contrast, religious and spiritual practices tend to decline in the early part of emerging adulthood, but later increase in some, but not all emerging adults (Smith and Snell 2009). Moreover, a growing subgroup of emerging adults identifies as spiritual, but not religious (Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life 2012).

As summarized in Pargament et al. (2013), scholars agree on the multifaceted nature of R/S but disagree as to which construct is broader, with religiousness encompassing “predefined belief and rituals along with institutionally based involvement, [whereas spirituality] more often connotes an individualized, experientially based…positive values, such as connectedness, meaning, self-actualization, and authenticity” (p. 11). Still Zinnbauer et al. (1997) contend that spirituality is the forerunner of religiousness. While spirituality used to be based upon a religion, increasingly the term also may be derived from a secular source (Koenig 2008). Scholars have often struggled developing measures that assess these constructs separately, and some use the R/S terms interchangeably (Koenig 2008). In sum, religiousness and spirituality are distinct constructs but they are interrelated in many important ways (e.g., conceptually, methodologically, practically). Thus, research is needed that carefully examines R/S jointly.

Religiousness and Spirituality

The third decade of life is a unique time to examine the R/S interplay. Although emerging adults’ R/S is related to their faith activities in their home during childhood (Barry et al. 2013) through processes of intergenerational transmission of religion (Silverstein and Bengston 2018; see Nelson 2014 for review), emerging adulthood is the period in which many emerging adults begin to separate their religiousness from their spirituality (Pew Forum on Religion and Public Life 2012). For example, many emerging adults report the importance of spiritual beliefs despite their practice of religious beliefs decreasing during the early part of emerging adulthood (Smith and Snell 2009). Certainly, it is not surprising that we start to see changes in young people’s R/S given the tendency for emerging adults to engage in self-exploration in multiple and changing social contexts presented in this period of life (Arnett 2015) that have the potential to expose them to an increasing variety of values, beliefs, and practices. Because of greater exposure to a variety of ideologies, emerging adults are likely to take a cafeteria approach to forming and practicing their R/S, whereby individuals adopt a variety of views from different R/S systems into one worldview (Smith and Snell 2009). For instance, an emerging adult might seamlessly incorporate media notions, such as “The Force” from Star Wars, with both a traditional Christian doctrine, and practices of meditation from Zen Buddhism (Arnett and Jensen 2002). Given this cafeteria approach to forming beliefs, it is easy to see why many young people value spirituality, while not conforming their personal beliefs with any one religious tradition resulting in a growing number of emerging adults who identify themselves as non-religious (including those who identify as spiritual, but not religious; see Zuckerman et al. 2016). Taken together, current work points to the complex relation of two distinct but interrelated aspects of emerging adults’ identity (religiousness and spirituality); the complexity of R/S interaction only increases when we begin to examine the links of R/S with indices of adjustment and maladjustment.

Religiousness and Spirituality in Psychological Adjustment

As distinct constructs, religiousness and spirituality, respectively, have been associated with emerging adults’ psychosocial adjustment (for reviews, see Yonker et al. 2012). Specifically, emerging adults’ religious beliefs have been associated with adjustment, including high levels of self-esteem (Rew and Wong 2006) and academic performance (Good et al. 2009). Additionally, religiousness has been associated with depressive symptoms (Shannon et al. 2018) and hooking up (Burdette et al. 2009). Spirituality has been associated with some adjustment outcomes, such as effective coping mechanisms (Hill et al. 2000), increased levels of moral development and psychological well-being, yet lower levels of negative moods (Powers et al. 2007) and perceived stress (Heintzman and Mannell 2003). Taken together, existing work examines the links between R/S, respectively, and indices of adjustment and maladjustment. Again, the results paint a very complicated picture as both constructs have been linked to positive and negative outcomes. Most of these studies have examined R/S as separate and distinct constructs, yet literature shows them also to be interrelated in many ways. Indeed, little, if any, work has attempted to identify the within-person relative levels of R/S in the same study (c.f., Klemmack et al. 2007) pointing to the need to examine the relations between R/S and psychological adjustment within individuals (i.e., from a person-centered approach rather than variable-centered approaches) better.

Profiles of Psychological Adjustment

Although there is ample research on R/S and psychological adjustment using a variety of variable-centered statistical approaches (e.g., factor analysis, correlational analysis, structural equation models), few researchers have taken a person-centered approach in which specific profiles or clusters of individuals are identified who share particular attributes. One such person-centered method to identify individuals who share various profiles of R/S and adjustment is cluster analysis. Cluster analysis is a procedure whereby persons are divided into separate groups or clusters based upon their similarities; this statistical technique is particularly useful when assessing R/S and psychological adjustment as it can determine how R/S and psychological adjustment variables interact with one another while additionally determining differences among these groups according to their levels of R/S and adjustment. Additionally, cluster analysis can identify unusual profiles of R/S and psychological adjustment which may be overlooked by variable-centered statistical procedures such as multiple regression analyses, correlational analyses, and structural equation models (Kristeller et al. 2011).

Person-centered approaches have been employed effectively to understand various profiles of emerging adults’ adjustment and maladjustment better, including links between various profiles of identity development and adjustment (Benson and Elder 2011; Luyckx et al. 2008). In one of the only person-centered approaches looking at religiousness, Jankowski et al. (2015) identified three profiles, or groups of young people from 30 universities in the US, based on varying levels of religious involvement, salience, and religious motivations. The three groups then differed on a variety of indices of adjustment (e.g., hazardous alcohol use). In sum, variable-centered approaches can establish links between variables (e.g., religiousness and adjustment) but person-centered approaches allow comparison of individuals who are similar in certain variables with those who differ on those same dimensions. As such and given that few studies examine the interrelatedness of R/S, we chose to employ a cluster analysis to identify different groups of emerging adults who vary along each of these dimensions (R/S and adjustment).

Current Study

Given the normative challenges faced in the third decade of life (Arnett 2015), it is essential to understand how R/S and psychological adjustment manifest in emerging adults. Emerging adults are faced with the awesome task of individuation, which in turn promotes their identity exploration (and hopefully commitment), as well as their self-worth (Arnett 2015). Amidst the instability that comes with this decade and becoming more autonomous, depressive symptoms are possible (Salmela-Aro et al. 2008). Collectively then, these three areas of adjustment (identity, self-worth, and depressive symptoms) capture salient positive and negative indicators of emerging adults’ psychological adjustment.

Identity

Identity development is an evolving and complex developmental task that becomes salient starting in adolescence, continues into adulthood, and becomes one of the central facets to positive adaptation in late adulthood (for review, see Schwartz et al. 2015). Identity can develop across multiple domains (e.g., gender, romantic relationship, career) but can also occur within an overall sense of oneself as a person. Furthermore, identity development involves two important processes, namely, exploration and commitment. Given university-attending emerging adults’ increased independence and less responsibility towards others (Arnett 2015), they often spend more time exploring than committing to their identity, particularly in their early twenties. According to Marcia and Strayer (1996), this tension between identity exploration and commitment has the capacity to shape young people’s development.

For many emerging adults, the process of identity development may include exploring completely new areas of their lives (e.g., career) but it might also include examining the beliefs in which they were raised. Indeed, their increased autonomy may serve as a catalyst to the process of re-evaluating, adjusting, and perhaps renouncing previously held beliefs. The replacement of old values and roles with new ones requires them to adjust psychologically (Côté 2000). Hence, conceptually, it seems that varying levels of R/S may be linked to identity development. Empirically, there is emerging evidence to support this notion. For instance, college students high in quest (i.e., viewing religion as a search for truth) but low in intrinsic and extrinsic religious motivation) reported the lowest religiousness and least identity commitment compared to other groups (Cook et al. 2014). Although these results point to the notion that religiousness may be linked to identity formation, it does not consider aspects of spirituality. However, it does point to the possibility that a unique profile of R/S and identity development (i.e., identity exploration and commitment) may appear in emerging adults, which was undertaken in the current study.

Self-worth

In examining indices of adjustment, it is important to include self-worth as it has been identified as an important aspect of flourishing in emerging adulthood (Nelson and Padilla-Walker 2013). Given the instability of emerging adulthood, self-worth is likely to fluctuate (Arnett 2015) based on a variety of individual factors. Specifically, there is evidence that R/S is associated with emerging adults’ self-worth (Rew and Wong 2006) but, to date, little research has examined R/S variables simultaneously in relation to self-esteem, and none have taken a person-centered approach to see how various patterns of religiousness, spirituality, and self-worth might may be exhibited within different groups of individuals; this task was undertaken in the current study.

Depressive Symptoms

One potential barrier to positive psychological adjustment is depressive symptoms, which for some emerging adults persist at high rates across the third decade due in part to genetic predisposition and the typical age of onset (Salmela-Aro et al. 2008). Despite the increased autonomy given to emerging adults, there is heightened instability for young people in many areas of their lives including romantic partners, residences, and work (Arnett 2015). As a result, many emerging adults experience multiple internal and external stressors and pressures. Not surprisingly then, changes in mental health are likely, which can be related adversely to psychological adjustment (e.g., decreased quality of life; Lewinsohn et al. 1997).

R/S have been associated with greater levels of physical and mental health (Ellison and George 1994). Specifically concerning religiousness, church attendance, religious coping, and religious activities (e.g., prayer) have been found to be associated with lower levels of depressive symptoms (Shannon et al. 2018). Also, spirituality has been related to college students’ personal-emotional adjustment (Gilliam et al. 2008). Taken together, it appears that in work employing variable-centered methods for R/S is associated with depressive symptoms. However, little is known about the multiple ways in which individuals reflect different patterns of R/S in relation to depressive symptoms, which was explored in this study.

Overview

Given the normative challenges faced in the third decade of life (Arnett 2015), it is essential to understand how R/S and psychological adjustment manifest in emerging adults. R/S have been shown to be related positively to psychological adjustment (Yonker et al. 2012), even though little is known about how such adjustment varies by emerging adults’ religiousness relative to their spirituality. In other words, based on variable-centered methods, we know that R/S, respectively, may be linked to indices of adjustment and maladjustment, but little is known about the variation of individuals regarding R/S and adjustment. Thus, the purpose of this study was to employ a cluster analysis to identify different profiles of R/S and psychological adjustment in emerging adulthood. We hypothesized that two profiles would emerge: high R/S with positive adjustment (i.e., higher self-worth and identity, but lower depressive symptoms) and low R/S with negative adjustment (i.e., lower self-worth and identity, but higher depressive symptoms).

Method

Participants

Participants for this study were drawn from a study of emerging adults entitled Project READY (Researching Emerging Adults Developmental Years). This project was a collaborative, multi-site study that was conducted by a consortium of developmental and family scholars to understand the individual and relationship factors that support university students’ adjustment and transition to adulthood. The sample of emerging adults used in the current study was composed of 792 undergraduate and graduate students (547 women, 243 men, 1 transgendered person, and 1 person who identified as other). The mean age of the sample was 19.61 years (SD = 1.86), ranging from 18 to 29 years. The participants were recruited from four American universities (ns = 151, 289, 240, and 112 respectively). Religious affiliations varied as follows: 18.8% Conservative Christian, 30.3% Roman Catholic, 14% Liberal Christian, 11.5% other faiths, 6.8% Agnostic, 4.9% Atheist, and no affiliation 13.5%. Most participants were European American (79%); however, there were also participants that identified as Asian American (9%), African American (4%), Latino American (3%), and “mixed/biracial” or other (3%). The response rate was approximately 59% overall but varied from site to site (ranging from 50 to 71%).

Procedure

The participants completed the survey online through an online tool developed by a computer science consultant specifically for this project. The survey contained a battery of 448 items that ranged from demographics to behaviors, temperament, parent–child relationship qualities, and media use. Participants were recruited through announcements of the study to each institution’s undergraduate population (primarily through Psychology courses) through a flier. Informed consent was obtained online prior to beginning the questionnaires. The survey took approximately 45 min to complete, and in exchange for participants’ time, they were offered course credit or extra credit, depending upon the site.

Measures

Religiousness

Participants completed three items to assess religiousness. These items were frequency of prayer, frequency of church attendance, and attending religious/spiritual activities with others. All items were scored on a 5-point Likert scale with higher scores reflecting greater levels of religiousness. To understand the sample’s religiousness more fully, descriptives on each of these items are specified here. Participants reported higher levels of prayer (M = 3.36, SD = 1.92), moderate levels of attending church (M = 2.36, SD = 1.29), and did not frequently participate in spiritual activities with at least one other person (M = 1.92, SD = 1.36). Overall, participants’ self-reported themselves to be moderately religious (M = 2.55, SD = 1.33). All three items were averaged to yield a single score for religiousness. The Cronbach’s alpha for this measure was α = .89. Concerning validity, items of similar nature to items employed in the present study have displayed validity as evidenced by correlations with health outcomes (Bowie et al. 2017). Additionally, emerging adults with high levels of religious practices perceived morality to be a more important perceived competence than did those with low or medium religious practices (Barry and Nelson 2008). Further, retrospective accounts of childhood family faith activities predicted emerging adults’ future religious practices (after accounting for T1 religious practices; Barry et al. 2013).

Spirituality

Participants completed the 12-item adapted version of the Spiritual Involvement and Beliefs Scale (Hatch et al. 1998). The original scale was a 16-question scale that tapped into the dimensions of external/ritual, internal/fluid, existential/meditative, and humility/personal application. Since Hatch et al. (1998) found the humility scale to lack validity, it was omitted from this study. They also found spirituality to be valid among diverse religious groups, and to be correlated to spiritual well-being (r = .80), although Gilliam and colleagues (2008) found it correlated to personal-emotional adjustment (r = .20). The participants rated items on a five-point scale ranging from one (strongly disagree) to five (strongly agree). The appropriate items were reverse scored. Items were averaged to yield a single score for spirituality. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was α = 0.91. A sample question is: “I believe there is a power greater than myself.”

Self-worth

Participants completed the self-worth subscale of the Self Perception Profile for College Students scale (Neeman and Harter 1986). This subscale was adapted to be five single statements, such as “I am happy being the way I am” rather than the opposing statements that individuals had to choose which statement was most like them and then to what extent. This adaptation resulted in a similar continuous variable (with high scores indicating high levels of self-worth), even though the presentation differed slightly. The scale ranged from one (not very true of me) to four (very true of me). The appropriate items were reverse scored. Items were averaged to yield a single score for self-worth. Cronbach’s alpha for the scale was α = 0.84. This adaptation, as has been used in previous studies (Nelson et al. 2010), resulted in expected relations among variables. Specifically, they found that Mormon men who did not view pornography reported higher levels of self-worth than did those who did view it.

Identity

Participants completed the shortened version of the Ego Identity Process Questionnaire (Balistreri and Busch-Rossnagel 1995). Questions addressed identity beliefs concerning dating, occupation, and values. Four of these items were averaged to yield identity exploration (e.g., “I have tried to learn about different occupational fields to find the best one for me.”; α = 0.65), and the eight other items were averaged to yield identity commitment (e.g., “There has never been a need to question my values.”; in the current study, α = 0.72). Participants were asked to rate items on a one (strongly disagree) to six (strongly agree) scale. The appropriate items were reverse scored. With respect to validity, Balistreri and Busch-Rossnagel (1995) found identity exploration to be related negatively to authoritarianism (r = − .18), but positively related to identity commitment for men (r = .37).

Depressive Symptoms

Using an abbreviated version of the original Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale (Radloff 1977), participants rated eight statements pertaining to ‘How often did this happen in the past week?’ Sample items include “I felt sad” or “I felt lonely.” Participants rated the frequency of these items on a scale of one (never) to three (most of the time). The appropriate items were reverse scored. Items were averaged. Cronbach’s alpha was α = 0.73 for this scale. Construct validity of the scale has also been demonstrated, in that, men who did not view pornography reported lower levels of depression than did those who viewed it in a Mormon university sample (Nelson et al. 2010).

Data Analysis Plan

First, descriptive statistics (i.e., means, standard deviations) and bivariate correlations were computed to explore the nature of religiousness, spirituality, and adjustment among university-attending emerging adults and to determine if there were any anomalies within these data which might have limited the utility of cluster analysis. Second, a cluster analysis was used to determine the various profiles of religiousness, spirituality, and psychological adjustment within university-age emerging adults.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Descriptive analyses were performed on all variables, see Table 1. As each survey employed was relatively short, average values were computed for each variable instead of using a composite score to avoid any high or low endorsement of any individual question from skewing that participant’s score on the given variable. To explore the relations between adjustment variables as well as religiousness and spirituality, a series of Pearson r correlations were computed (see Table 2). Many significant correlations were observed among variables, but only one potential instance of collinearity between religiousness and spirituality (r = .76) was identified. Using the criteria specified by Sambandam (2003), the value was below the established threshold of r = .80. Nonetheless, given the theoretical differences for R/S (Pargament et al. 2013), it was advantageous to include both variables in the cluster analysis.

Table 1 Descriptives
Table 2 Pearson r correlations of key study variables

Cluster Analysis

Cluster Analysis was conducted and reported on according to recommendations provided by Clatworthy et al. (2005). A Two-Step Cluster Analysis was conducted to identify any unique profiles of religiousness, spirituality, depressive symptoms, self-worth, identity exploration, and identity commitment among emerging adults. All variables were standardized prior to analysis. Distance between clusters was computed using log-likelihood calculation. Clusters were determined using Silhouette measure of cohesion and separation. There were three distinct, yet very similar, clusters within these data, Silhouette = 0.3.

Differences in Religiousness, Spirituality, and Adjustment Among Clusters

To determine the differences between these clusters, a MANOVA was conducted to explore differences in religiousness, spirituality, depressive symptoms, self-worth, identity exploration, and identity commitment across the three clusters. Results were significant, Wilks λ = 0.16, F (12, 1564) = 199.49, p < .01, partial η2 = .61, observed power = 1.00. Table 3 presented the differences between the three clusters across all variables, and finds self-worth, identity commitment (but not identity exploration), depressive symptoms, spirituality, and religiousness significantly differed across cluster membership.

Table 3 Table of differences between the three clusters

Tukey LSD post hoc analyses were used to determine differences across clusters between variables. As shown in Table 1, self-worth was highest in cluster 2 and lowest in cluster 1. Identity commitment was highest in cluster 3 and lowest in cluster 1. Depressive symptoms were highest in cluster 1 and lowest in cluster 2. Spirituality was highest in cluster 3 and lowest in cluster 2. Religiousness was highest in cluster 3 and lowest in cluster 1. Cluster 1 displayed low self-worth, low identity commitment, high depression, moderate spirituality, moderate religiousness. Cluster 2 displayed high self-worth, high identity commitment, low depression, low spirituality, low religiousness. Cluster 3 displayed moderate self-worth, moderate identity commitment, moderate depression, high spirituality, high religiousness.

Three Adjustment Profiles

Given the results of the Cluster Analysis combined with the results of the MANOVA, three distinct but similar profiles of adjustment were observed. Two profiles of positive psychological adjustment were seen. Cluster 2 was called the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual group as individuals within this group displayed high self-worth, high identity commitment, low depression, low spirituality, low religiousness. Cluster 3 was labeled the Adjusted high religious and high spiritual group as individuals within this group displayed high spirituality, high religiousness, moderate self-worth, moderate identity commitment, and moderate depression. There was one profile of low psychological adjustment. Accordingly, Cluster 1 was labeled the Low-adjusted moderate religious and moderate spiritual group as individuals within this group displayed high depression, moderate religiousness, moderate spirituality, low self-worth, and low identity commitment.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to employ a cluster analysis to identify different profiles of R/S and psychological adjustment in emerging adulthood. Although past lines of research employing variable-centered approaches have found links between R/S and indices of adjustment and maladjustment, no study has attempted to take a person-centered approach in attempting to examine the complexity in the relations of R/S and adjustment. Hence, in identifying three different profiles of R/S and psychological adjustment, the results contribute significantly to our understanding of R/S during the third decade of life. Specifically, the present study found distinct profiles that point to two patterns of positive adjustment and one that reflected low levels of adjustment. Individuals from the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual group displayed high levels of psychological adjustment (lower depressive symptoms, higher self-worth, and higher identity commitment). Conversely, the Adjusted high religious and high spiritual individuals displayed greater psychological adjustment (i.e., moderate levels of depressive symptoms, self-worth, and identity commitment). Lastly, one pattern of individuals with negative psychological adjustment was found; individuals from the Low-adjusted moderate religious and moderate spiritual were moderately religious and spiritual but displayed the highest levels of depression and lowest levels of identity commitment, and low self-worth.

Comparisons Across Profiles

Although we expected to find a large variance in our measures of emerging adults’ R/S, particularly due to the rising proportion of emerging adults in the US who consider themselves “more spiritual than religious” (Smith and Snell 2009), we found only groups of emerging adults with comparable R/S levels. Few individuals displayed divergent profiles of R/S. In terms of the Low-adjusted moderate religious and moderate spiritual cluster, these individuals displayed the highest levels of depressive symptoms, and lowest levels of self-worth and identify commitment. Surprisingly, these individuals displayed moderate levels of R/S compared to the other two clusters who exhibited the highest levels of adjustment either had high or low levels of R/S but the profile faring the worst had moderate levels of both R/S. It may be that these individuals from Cluster 1 (Low Adjustment) are comparable to what Smith and Snell (2009) refer to as the sporadic (17.9% of their total sample from the National Study of Youth and Religion) who attend services a few times a year, whose faith is somewhat, not very, or not important, and closeness to God, prayer, and scripture reading is quite variable. This profile of emerging adults, therefore, has a basis of their faith tradition from which can guide their future behavior. Therefore, they fared worse because their faith is somewhat a part of their lives, but not fully integrated, which results in potential areas for cognitive dissonance between their beliefs and likely their behavior. This is akin to a study by Nelson et al. (2010) where emerging-adult Mormon men who engaged in behavior opposed by their religion fared psychologically worse than comparable Mormon men who did not engage in such behavior. Scholars may seek to replicate this unusual profile of religiousness, spirituality, and adjustment.

In terms of the two-positive adjustment profile, individuals from the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual cluster and the Adjusted high religious and high spiritual profiles both displayed greater levels of psychological adjustment than individuals in the Low-adjusted moderate religious and moderate spiritual profile. It is difficult to say which profile of adjustment (Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual vs. high religious and high spiritual) is most positive given the difference between these two profiles is very low as seen by the silhouette measure of cohesion value of 0.3. The Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual profile displayed greater self-worth, lower identity commitment, and lower depressive symptoms than Adjusted high religious and high spiritual profile. Thus, the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual profile has more positive adjustment in two of three adjustment areas relative to the high religious profile. This is not enough at this juncture to say that the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual profile is significantly preferable to the Adjusted high religious and high spiritual profile.

More importantly, the current study’s findings illustrate that adjustment can take numerous forms. Although emerging adults’ R/S often is associated with adjustment (Magyar-Russell et al. 2014), emerging adults who are “nones” have often been found to fare well psychologically, particularly among those who have done the important work of meaning-making (Zuckerman et al. 2016). Thus, many pathways can exist for emerging adults to experience positive psychological adjustment. It is important though for scholars to investigate these profiles as it bears on other indices of adjustment and to follow these individuals of differing profiles across the full decade of emerging adulthood to determine if one profile is in fact preferable to the other.

Mechanisms Underlying Adjustment and R/S Profiles

Although many different and unusual adjustment profiles were observed in the present study, we strive to understand the possible mechanisms to explain the connection between R/S and adjustment. Oleckno and Blacconiere (1991) noted that religiousness was associated with positive health outcomes, yet Zuckerman (2009) noted that a lack of religiousness also yielded positive outcomes.

It is unclear why individuals who reported moderate R/S levels display the poorest psychological adjustment, which could be explained by Pargament’s (2002) finding that the impact of religion is associated with one’s religious commitment. Individuals who are intrinsic display greater well-being compared to those who are superficially religious. This Low-adjustment cluster may reflect this group of individuals who do not display a high degree of religious commitment to benefit from the positive aspects of religion (as seen in cluster 3) yet barely affiliated with the religion to experience the drawbacks of religion (which were avoided in cluster 2).

Among the Adjusted high religious and high spiritual profile, religious individuals who simultaneously report high levels of spirituality often can be characterized as having a deeper penetration of their faith into their being, which in turn promotes their abilities for self-control and self-regulation to manage life’s difficulties and live well (i.e., better adjustment; McCullough and Carter 2013). Moreover, highly religious emerging adults have been shown to engage in high levels of coping (Cook et al. 2014). Further, those with high levels of R/S beliefs are likely to be exposed regularly to religious communities and practices wherein religious models promote their social capital; in turn, these experiences can promote their adjustment (King 2008). Thus, scholars should investigate such constructs as self-control, self-regulation, coping, and religious contextual factors while using this exploratory approach to understand profiles of emerging adults’ R/S and adjustment.

Among the Well-adjusted non-religious and non-spiritual persons, however, there may be alternative mechanisms explaining this relation. For non-religious individuals, Weber et al. (2012) have identified numerous sources of distress such as negative views stemming from being atheist; yet, a person’s conviction in their non-theistic beliefs was associated with high mental health. As such, the degree to which atheist beliefs are held may be a mechanism through which low R/S individuals are associated with positive adjustment, which we suspect is due to their high levels of meaning-making to discern their atheist worldview and in many cases to reject organized religion. In so doing, they often have come to identify their values and beliefs that guide their daily lives (e.g., Zuckerman et al. 2016). As such, the degree to which atheist beliefs are held may be a mechanism through which low R/S individuals experience positive adjustment. For low R/S emerging adults who do not espouse atheism, explanations for these results are less clear. Since many of these young emerging adults are often involved in heightened exploration of values, beliefs, and worldviews (Arnett 2015), many disengage from organized religion as they focus on the immediate transitions of school, work, and peer group. Thus, in the short term their adjustment may be acceptable; however, Magyar-Russell et al. (2014) would suggest their adjustment in the long-term would not remain high. Indeed, future work is needed among this subgroup of individuals to understand better the mechanisms by which positive adjustment can occur.

Limitation and Future Directions

Since few individuals displayed divergent profiles of R/S, we attribute this absence in part to the R/S measures used, but also to the sample’s age. As younger emerging adults, their levels of spirituality were less than what they are likely to become over the third decade (Astin et al. 2011). Indeed, scholars may seek to utilize this cluster analysis approach with more robust measures to determine if there are sizeable groups of persons who report high levels of spirituality, but low levels of religiousness or vice versa. Additionally, scholars may seek to use more definitive statistical approaches (e.g., a Latent Profile Analysis or Latent Class Analysis) to build upon this exploratory cluster analysis. However, given that the clusters were so similar, a Latent Profile Analysis or Latent Class Analysis would have reduced these data to a one-group solution and any nuances across groups would not be captured.

In the current study, we utilized concurrent data, which prevented an exploration of within-person changes across the emerging-adult years. So, while we were not positing a developmental trajectory between R/S and adjustment, it remains important still to examine if such profiles would have emerged previously in the lifespan or later into emerging adulthood and thereafter. Lastly, the sample consisted of emerging adults who are just beginning to embark upon deeper introspection concerning R/S in contrast to those in their later twenties. Given that religious and spiritual development is a life-long journey, and tremendous change occurs within the twenties (Yonker et al. 2012), it is likely that findings would vary across those in their twenties depending upon where the persons are in their R/S. Thus, future research that examines individuals across their twenties is essential to understand these relations more fully.

Implications

The findings of the current study are useful not only to emerging adults to understand the complex ways in which their R/S are related to their psychological adjustment, but they should inform the work that therapists, youth ministers, and campus ministry staff do to support emerging adults’ meaning-making. Simply trying to promote greater R/S for young people will not necessarily increase their adjustment, as evidenced by our findings. Rather, these practitioners need to meet emerging adults where they are at psychologically to promote their meaning-making and answer (or appropriately refer) them to be able to seek full answers to their theological questions. Staying in the conversation with young people is key, regardless of whether that leads them toward or away from commitment in their faith of origin.

Conclusion

The present study identifies unique profiles of emerging adults’ psychosocial adjustment, religiousness, and spirituality. Overall, the findings from this study support the notion that three profiles exist, such that moderate religious and spiritual persons display the lowest levels of adjustment. Conversely, the highly R/S and low R/S profiles display the most positive indicators of psychological adjustment. Consequently, future research that explores such mechanisms of self-regulation, coping, and certainty of beliefs, as well as social support afforded religious and spiritual involvement may be important to explaining emerging adults’ psychological adjustment.