Introduction

Parental care is described as any strategy or behaviour adopted by breeding individuals to promote the survival of their offspring, encompassing from energy provision for gametes to food provisioning and protection of offspring (Trivers 1972; Clutton-Brock 1991). Despite the great variety of reproductive strategies and patterns of parental care, social monogamy with biparental care is the most common reproductive strategy in birds (Cockburn 2006). In monomorphic and monogamous species, parents commonly have similar investment in reproductive activities like food provisioning, nest sanitation, and brooding (Gill and Haggerty 2012; Sánchez et al. 2018). However, in some of such species, energy investment and the role performed by parents differ considerably, with each sex performing certain breeding tasks exclusively or more frequently than its mate (Gowaty 1996; Stutchbury and Morton 2001; Kokko and Jennions 2008).

Even in more equitable division of parental tasks, there are asymmetric investments between parents due to anisogamy, physiological and behavioural differences between sexes (Lessels 2012). Although parental investment in monogamous monomorphic birds is considered fairly equitable, higher reproductive costs associated with parental care normally rests upon females due to egg laying and a higher frequency of egg and nestling brooding (Gowaty 1996; Carere and Alleva 1998; Bowers et al. 2014). In contrast, males usually engage in territory and nest defence through vigilance, vocalization, and physical aggression, while also providing food to the incubating and/or brooding female (Gowaty 1996; Winkler 2016). Therefore, differences in parental investment and division of reproductive tasks are expected to be found to varying degrees in monogamous monomorphic birds (Carere and Alleva 1998; Espíndola-Hernández et al. 2017; Sánchez et al. 2018).

Costs of parental investment in offspring increase considerably with brood size and age in altricial birds (Lack 1947; Skutch 1949). This investment increase is often nonlinear, as large broods usually receive less per capita food provisioning, leading to a trade-off between brood size and offspring quality (Silver et al. 1985; Stutchbury and Morton 2001; Bowers et al. 2014). The most frequent form of care used to quantify parental effort in the nesting stage is food provisioning to nestlings, as the time and energy required to supply the energetic demand of nestlings escalates with brood size and age (Silver et al. 1985; Bowers et al. 2014). By being more easily quantified, food provisioning has been used to gauge parental care, sexual and parent–offspring conflicts, as well as to understand breeding strategies of altricial birds (Low et al. 2012; Gill and Haggerty 2012; MacLeod and Brouwer 2018). Nest attendance and nestling brooding (nestling thermoregulation) are also important for the comprehension of sex asymmetry in energy investment, as such tasks may occupy a large part of the daily activities of parents (Carere and Alleva 1998; Hill et al. 1999; Evans and Stutchbury 2012). Lastly, nest sanitation can be a costly and frequent activity for parents, as it requires parents visiting the nest, waiting for and/or prodding nestlings to defecate, and then disposing off the nestlings’ excrements (Hurd et al. 1991; Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2017).

In many altricial birds, nestling excrements are enclosed in a mucous covering called faecal sacs (Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2013). Through the removal of faecal sacs, birds avoid the potential negative effects of excrements soiling the nest structure (Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 20142016). The research on nest sanitation has been usually focused on its importance to prevent diseases and parasites from afflicting the parents and offspring through removal of external or unwanted material and faecal sacs, or on the direct benefits of faecal sac ingestion by the parents (i.e. the acquisition of nutrients or water) (Hurd et al. 1991; Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2016, 2017). The more frequent disposal of faecal sacs by a specific sex could serve as an indication of higher breeding effort, indicating a need and/or benefit to recycle water and nutrients from faecal sacs, possibly providing another way to compare parental expenditure between sexes (Hurd et al. 1991; McKay et al. 2009). The few studies that integrate nest sanitation in the framework of sexual differences in parental care report cases of female-dominant, male-dominant, and equitable nest sanitation (Carere and Alleva 1998; Markman et al. 1995; McKay et al. 2009; Bolopo et al. 2015).

The topic of parental care has traditionally attracted attention from the ornithological scientific community, but severe gaps in our knowledge do remain, especially in the southern hemisphere and regarding tropical birds, where few studies exist and extended parental care occurs in comparison with northern hemisphere species (Russell et al. 2004). We studied a Neotropical passerine, the Pale-breasted Thrush (Turdus leucomelas), to evaluate the parental division of tasks during the nesting stage by investigating different components of parental care (i.e. food provisioning, nest attendance and brooding, and nest sanitation). We hypothesized that the Pale-breasted Thrush would present indistinguishable food provisioning rates between sexes irrespective of brood size and age, but nest attendance and brooding would be carried out by females, akin to other species of the genus (e.g. Hill et al. 1999; Sánchez et al. 2018; Batisteli et al. 2020). Regarding nest sanitation, we expected a pattern similar to food provisioning with which it is usually synchronized (Quan et al. 2015).

Methods

Study area

The study was conducted in the campus of the Universidade Estadual Paulista “Júlio de Mesquita Filho” (UNESP) (22°23′45.7″S 47°32′38.3″W) at Rio Claro, Brazil, between 2015 and 2017 from September to December, which covers most of the species’ breeding season (Batisteli et al. 2021). Rio Claro is at the transition between the semideciduous Atlantic Forest and the Cerrado biomes, though heavily impacted by anthropic land uses as sugarcane farmland, pasture, and Eucalyptus plantations. The campus (111.46 ha) consists of a mix of manmade gardens and small forest patches of native vegetation. The regional climate has two well defined seasons: a dry season from April to September (~ 240 mm rainfall) and a wet season from October to March (~ 1100 mm) (Alvares et al. 2013).

Study species

The Pale-breasted Thrush is a visually monomorphic species with a widespread distribution in South America, from the Guyanas, south of Colombia and Venezuela, almost every state in Brazil (except Acre and Rondônia), Paraguay and north Argentina (Sick 1997). It is an omnivore species, feeding primarily on fruits and invertebrates (mainly arthropods), and rarely small vertebrates, found in forest borders, clearings, savannahs, gallery forests, rural and urban areas (Sick 1997; Collar 2005).

The breeding season at the study region spans from September to January; and the breeding cycle of the Pale-breasted Thrush has an average duration of two months from egg laying and incubation (12–13 days), nestling (14–16 days) to post-fledging stages (30–40 days) (Davanco et al. 2013). At the campus, the species prefers areas with sparse to dense trees and shrubs, nesting in buildings and on the vegetation, mainly on forks of large tree trunks. One to four eggs per clutch are laid (usually two to three) in a cup-shaped nest built by the female with roots, vegetation, and mud (Davanco et al. 2013). Up to two successful clutches are laid in a breeding season, with a replacement clutch occurring if one of the previous clutches was unsuccessful (Batisteli et al. 2021).

Field procedures

Adult thrushes were captured with mist nets and marked with a coded metal ring and coloured rings. We collected a small drop of blood by cutting the tip of the nail from each captured individual for sex identification, which was done by an outsourced laboratory (UNIGEN, São Paulo) through Polymerase Chain Reaction (PCR) amplification of DNA sex-specific gene sequences. We observed only breeding pairs for which at least one of the parents was sexed and colour-ringed to enable sex identification from a distance.

Nests were found by active searching and monitored every 2–3 days with the aid of a pole with an attached mirror to record clutch size and hatching days. We used GoPro Hero 2 cameras positioned 0.5–1.0 m from nests, distances that allowed for quality recordings. Parents quickly returned to the nests and resumed breeding tasks after camera installation, which allows us to assume a minimal interference of this procedure on the parents’ behaviour. Recordings began the day following the hatching of all the eggs in nests found with eggs (n = 24), or immediately for nests discovered already with nestlings (n = 9). For clutches found after eggs had already hatched, nestling age was estimated by comparing feather development to photos of other monitored clutches of known age. We carried out filming sessions of 0.9–2.0 h duration (see Results) every two days either during the morning (07:00–12:30 h) or afternoon (14:00–18:00 h) depending on weather conditions. Recordings ceased when nestlings were 14–16 days old to avoid their premature departure from the nest upon our approximation (Appendix 1, Fig. S1).

Video recordings were then viewed to record for each parent the number of feeding visits per hour (hereafter “food provisioning rate”), the number of food items brought per hour (hereafter “food quantity rate”) (Biermann and Sealy 1982; Gill and Haggerty 2012), nest sanitation visits (i.e. visits in which there was faecal sac removal), nest sanitation rate (i.e. number of faecal sacs removed per hour), and the method of faecal sac removal (i.e. either the removal by carrying the faecal sac away from the nest or the ingestion of it at the nest; Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2013). We also estimated the percentage of parental attendance to the nest, defined as the time spent inside or perched on the nest rim (Appendix 1, Fig S2), and the percentage of time spent brooding the nestlings, defined as the parent laying on top of the nestlings, both calculated in relation to the duration of each filming session. Since brood parasitism can modify parental care behaviour and sometimes sexual conflict (Hoover and Reetz 2006; Požgayová et al. 2015), nests parasitized by the Shiny Cowbird (Molothrus bonariensis) were excluded from the study. We gathered data from 10 broods in 2015, 12 in 2016 and 11 in 2017.

Statistical analysis

We used Generalized Linear Mixed-Effects Models (GLMMs) with Poisson error distribution to investigate whether there were sex differences in parental care behaviour during the nestling stage, having as response variables food provisioning, food quantity, and nest sanitation rates. In these models, we included parent sex (male or female), brood size (1, 2 or 3), period of day (morning/afternoon), and the interaction between sex × brood size as fixed factors, and brood age as a covariate. We included in all these models the duration of each filming session as an offset variable, i.e. an independent variable with coefficient fixed at 1. As two nests were reused in the same breeding season and other five nests were used in multiple years (i.e. one nest used three times and five nests used twice), nest identification and a brood code made up of brood identification and year were used as random factors.

We ran Tukey pairwise post-hoc tests to evaluate male and female differences when the interaction term was significant, and to identify the differences in parental care behaviours according to brood size when it was a significant factor. For nest attendance, we built Linear Mixed-Effects Model (LMM) using the percentage of time spent on the nest (arc-sine and square-root transformed to achieve residual normality) as a continuous response variable, while sex, brood size and period of day as fixed factors and brood age as a covariate. In this case, the interaction between sex and brood age was included because brooding differs between sexes in this genus, with the females being the major or sole sex responsible for brooding (Hill et al. 1999; Batisteli et al. 2020), which tends to cease as nestlings age. Herein, nest identification and brood code were used as random factors. We used a backward stepwise procedure to select the significant factors of the models. Additionally, we utilised the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) to confirm the results and best model, and the most relevant factors from our previous procedure. Models were ranked from the lowest AIC score. Only models with ΔAIC < 2 for each response variable were presented, except for cases in which there would be a single model, when the second-best model was presented as well. We visually checked whether the residuals of each model complied with assumptions of normality. All analyses were done in R version 3.6.2 (R Core Team 2019) using the packages ‘car’ (Fox et al. 2012), ‘emmeans’ (Lenth et al. 2020), ‘lme4’ (Bates et al. 2015), ‘lmerTest’ (Kuznetsova et al. 2017), AICcmodavg (Mazerolle 2019). Values are reported as mean ± standard deviation.

Results

We recorded 33 nesting attempts (with initial brood sizes of 1, 2 and 3 nestlings in 9, 13, and 11 nests, respectively) by 26 unique breeding pair combinations. Brood reduction from 3 to 2 and from 2 to 1 nestling occurred in two and three breeding events, respectively. Filming sessions averaged 1.5 ± 0.2 h, ranging from 0.9–2.0 h, totalling 153.5 h. Each nest was filmed on average 3.2 ± 1.4 times (range 1–6 times).

Food provisioning and food quantity

The overall average food provisioning rate was 1.61 ± 1.13 feeding trips/h. The best model for food provisioning rate included the single terms sex, brood size and brood age, whereas the interaction between sex and brood size and the period of day were deemed unimportant and removed (Table 1). Both sexes fed the nestlings, with a significant difference between them in food provisioning (Table 1, males = 1.76 ± 1.12 feeding trips/h/nestling, females = 1.45 ± 1.12 feeding trips/h/nestling). Female provisioning rates were 2.23 ± 1.51, 2.45 ± 1.43, 2.70 ± 1.24 feeding trips/h for broods with one, two and three nestlings, respectively, whereas the corresponding male food provisioning rates were 2.42 ± 1.42, 3.35 ± 2.21, 4.28 ± 1.96 feeding trips/h (Fig. 1A). Overall food provisioning rate increased with brood age (Table 1, Fig. 2A). Post-hoc tests revealed a difference in food provisioning between broods with one and three nestlings (Tukey: z = −3.156, p < 0.005), but no difference between broods with one to two, and two to three nestlings (both p > 0.120).

Table 1 Results of the Generalized Linear Mixed-Effects Models (GLMMs) with Poisson error distribution and the Linear Mixed-Effects Model (LMM) testing the effect of sex, brood size (fixed factors), brood age (covariate), period of day (morning/afternoon) and the interactions of sex with brood size and with brood age on aspects of nestling care (food provisioning rate, food quantity rate, nest sanitation rate and nest attendance) in the Pale-breasted Thrush (Turdus leucomelas)
Fig. 1
figure 1

Food provisioning rate A food quantity rate B and nest sanitation rates C of males and females Pale-breasted Thrushes (Turdus leucomelas) according to brood size. In boxplots horizontal lines represent medians, whiskers represent minimum and maximum values, while dots represent outliers. Asterisk indicates significant difference at α = 0.05

Fig. 2
figure 2

Food provisioning rate A, food quantity rate B, and percentage of the observation time spent on the nest C across brood ages by males and females of the Pale-breasted Thrush (Turdus leucomelas) in an urban population in southeast of Brazil

The overall average food quantity rate was 2.08 ± 1.66 food items/h. The best model for food quantity rate included all the factors except period of day (Table S1). Food quantity rate, in contrast to food provisioning, was not affected by brood size, but brood age, sex, and the interaction sex × brood size were all significant factors (Table 1). Food quantity rate increased with brood age (Fig. 2B) and differed between sexes (Table 1, males = 2.33 ± 1.64 food items/h/nestling, females = 1.80 ± 1.64 food items/h/nestling). Post-hoc tests revealed that males showed higher food quantity rates than females in broods with three nestlings (Tukey: z = −1.336, p < 0.001), but not in broods with one or two nestlings (both p > 0.786) (Fig. 1B). Post-hoc tests also revealed that food quantity rate provided by males did not differ between broods with one to two nestlings (z = 2.625, p = 0.091), and two to three nestlings (z = −2.658, p = 0.084), but there was difference between broods of one to three nestlings (z = 4.756, p =  < 0.001). The quantity of food delivered by females did not change with brood size (p > 0.786 for all pairwise comparisons) (Fig. 1B).

Nest sanitation

In 91.5% of the nest sanitation visits, a single faecal sac was removed. Therefore, we did not analyse the quantity of faecal sacs removed per nest sanitation visit. In 377 events of nest sanitation, 418 faecal sacs were removed by either ingesting or carrying faecal sacs away in the beak. Both sexes participated in nest sanitation, and faecal sacs were ingested 153 times by females (90.53% of their total faecal sac removal) and 214 times by males (86.29%). Females and males carried away the remaining 16 and 34 faecal sacs, respectively. A female fed 1 faecal sac to a nestling. There was no faecal sac unremoved from the nest, and most of them were removed as soon as nestlings defecated, which often occurred synchronized with food provisioning.

The best model for nest sanitation rate included brood size, sex, and the interaction between these two factors, whereas brood age and period of day were dropped off (Table 1). There was a significant difference between sexes in sanitation rates (Table 1, males = 0.71 ± 0.59 sanitation event/h/nestling, females = 0.59 ± 0.73). Nest sanitation rates were not affected by brood size, whereas the p-value of the interaction sex × brood size was significant but close to the critical level (Table 1). However, pairwise comparisons by the post-hoc test revealed no differences between sexes in nest sanitation rates in broods with one, two or even three nestlings (p > 0.184 for all pairwise comparisons).

Nest attendance

Visits to the nest lasted on average 7.05 ± 8.45 min for females and 0.82 ± 1.7 min for males. Females spent on average 34 ± 27% of the observation time on the nest, and 24 ± 26% brooding nestlings. Males spent 4 ± 4% of the observation time on the nest and did not exhibit any brooding behaviour toward nestlings.

The best model for nest attendance included sex, brood age, the interaction between these two terms, but not brood size and period of day (Table 1). Nest attendance was not influenced by the number of nestlings in the brood, but it decreased with nestling age (Fig. 2C). The interaction between sex and brood age was significant, indicating that females decreased their attendance to nests as nestling grew, while nest attendance by males remained constant and low (Fig. 2C).

Discussion

We found that both males and females of the Pale-breasted Thrush engaged in food provisioning, nest sanitation and nest attendance, but differed in their investment in each of the tasks. Interestingly, for food quantity and nest sanitation rates, we found a significant effect of the interaction between sex and brood size, suggesting that the number of nestlings influences care investment differently by each parent. In this sense, while females keep constant their food quantity rate with increasing brood size, males modulate their effort by increasing the number of food items delivered to the young. Furthermore, brood age was positively related to both food provisioning and food quantity rates, likely due to the gradual changes of nestling energy requirements over their rapid growth and development period. Nest sanitation was performed by both sexes, with males having higher rates of faecal sac removal. Finally, only females brooded nestlings and thus remained on the nest for longer periods than males. This extensive nest attendance by females decreased rapidly through the nesting stage, as nestling development leads to an increased thermoregulation capacity (Andreasson et al. 2016), consequently allowing for a reduction of time spent brooding and attending nests. Males had consistently short nest attendance periods throughout the nesting period, usually only visiting to feed nestlings and remove faecal sacs. Nonetheless, while body masses do not differ between sexes in the non-breeding period in our study population, females tend to have higher body mass than males during the breeding season (Floreste et al. 2021). Therefore, it is possible that the care of nestlings in addition to the other reproductive tasks performed by males (e.g. territory defence, nest sanitation, fledgling care) take a more severe toll on their body condition than to females.

Food provisioning and food quantity

Food provisioning and food quantity, two complementary aspects of nestling provisioning effort, were influenced by different factors. Similar provisioning rates were expected for males and females of the Pale-breasted Thrush as observed in other thrush species (Sánchez et al. 2018), but males showed higher food provisioning and quantity rates, both increasing with nestling age. These intersexual differences cannot be attributed to a specialization in parental roles as both delivered invertebrates and fruits to the nestlings in similar frequencies (Pizo, unpubl. data). A larger brood size (e.g. three nestlings) elicited a higher food provisioning rate (feeding trips/h) irrespective of the parent sex, however, the effect of brood size on food quantity (food items/h) was restricted to males. Brood size and age are factors known to affect positively food provisioning rates in birds (Silver et al. 1985; Gill and Haggerty 2012; Bowers et al. 2014), as both age and the number of nestlings increase energy requirement and thus food demand. Therefore, larger broods require greater parental investment to sustain growth and development, and stable per capita food provisioning across brood sizes benefit the survival chances and quality of the offspring (Stutchbury and Morton 2001; Bowers et al. 2014).

In our study, although total food quantity increased with brood size, the largest broods still received lower per capita provisioning in comparison with single-nestling broods (Fig. 1A, B). Males of the Pale-breasted Thrush fulfil the role of keeping the per capita food quantity rate from declining drastically in the largest broods. In contrast, the number of food items delivered per hour by females are nearly constant across brood sizes, meaning that its per capita food quantity rate decreases in larger broods. This difference is only present in broods with three nestlings, in which males had significantly higher food quantity rates. A similar case of only males adjusting food provisioning rates as a function of brood size was found in the Common Swift (Apus apus), in which larger broods resulted in increased male effort, while smaller and older broods lead to reduced care by males in favour of seeking extra-pair copulations (Carere and Alleva 1998). Indeed, the certainty of paternity has been suggested as an important factor influencing the commitment of male passerines to parental duties (Arnold and Owens 2002), but unfortunately we do not have information on the frequency of extra-pair copulations in our study population to evaluate this possibility.

As larger clutches represent a higher energetic cost for females than smaller clutches (Visser and Lessells 2001), the female tendency to maintain food quantity rates constant across brood sizes could be explained with fitness constraints associated with laying a larger clutch. However, if that was the case, the number of food items per hour delivered by females would have been expected to be higher in smaller clutches, which did not happen. A likely alternative explanation for the constant provisioning effort by females is that they performed at their maximum level (Drent and Daan 1980), but this seems not feasible in our case. Otherwise, we should assume that females are at their maximum effort even in clutches of a single nestling, which provides weak support for this idea considering that the modal clutch size in this species is three (Batisteli et al. 2021).

While males do increase their food quantity for large broods, alone they are not able to keep the food quantity rates as high as in smaller broods, meaning that the combined per capita food quantity will inevitably be reduced for large broods, which may impact nestling quality (i.e. body mass and/or nestling development) and survival in the post-fledging stage (Styrsky et al. 2005; Gill and Haggerty 2012; Bowers et al. 2014; Sofaer et al. 2018; Evans et al. 2020). Thus, a trade-off could be at play, as the higher survival chance of a small clutch might be a breeding strategy as viable as a larger clutch with lower survival chance (Smith et al. 1989). Still, there is some contention on the claim that clutch size directly influences fledgling survival (Bowers et al. 2014; Remeš and Matysiovoká 2016). Given that clutches of three eggs are common in our study population (66% of 129 clutches; M. A. Pizo unpubl.), we assume that despite the possibility of lower post-fledging survival of a larger brood, producing a higher number of fledglings remains advantageous for the Pale-breasted thrush.

The amount of investment provided by males of the Pale-breasted Thrush was unexpected, as there are few cases of higher male food provisioning in passerines with biparental care (Carere and Alleva 1998; Hill et al. 1999; Reed et al. 2007). Additionally, males were responsible for increasing the parental effort in delivering food to broods with three nestlings. For this to happen, a higher brood size needs to be a worthwhile investment considering the potential inverse relationship between the energy invested in current reproduction and future breeding prospects (Parejo and Danchin 2006; Fokkema et al. 2016). In a more equal sexual selection scenario, both sexes will seek to maintain high quality partners over a breeding season or consecutive years. It might be that males perceive the ability to lay clutches of three eggs as a trait that indicates a high-quality female (Gill and Stutchbury 2005; Mahr et al. 2012; Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2020), as the parental investment of an individual has been shown to influence the investment of its mate (Gori 1988; Bebbington and Hatchwell 2016). Possibly, males might also be contributing to avoid decay in female condition, which could enhance the chance of a second nesting attempt (Ritchison et al. 2019). If breeding pairs last through multiple nesting attempts and years, the advantages of great investment in the current partnership increase, as securing high quality and experienced partners is advantageous for reproductive success (Peralta-Sánchez et al. 2020). This is precisely what seems to happen in our study population (A. Batisteli unpubl. data).

Nest sanitation

Both males and females removed faecal sacs, but as with food provisioning rate, nest sanitation rates differed between sexes, being higher for males. The per capita faecal sac removal remained constant in larger broods despite the per capita reduction of food provisioning, which might have led to a decrease in faecal sac removal in larger broods. The synchronization of provisioning and nest sanitation events led us to expect higher nest sanitation rates by males in larger broods, which was not the case. This might simply be explained by the short time that males spent in the nest to feed the nestlings, leaving the nest prior to nestling defecation. As females spent much more time in the nest, they might have removed the faecal sacs that were left unremoved by the males. Furthermore, removal of faecal sacs did not increase with brood age, which suggests that the size of faecal sacs rather than the number of faecal sacs produced increases with nestling age (Weatherhead 1984), akin to what we suggested previously for food provisioning rates. This suspicion is supported by video evidence when comparing the size of faecal sacs with the beak of the parents (Appendix 1, Fig. S3).

As previously mentioned, our results on the method of disposal of faecal sacs might imply that the ingestion of faecal sacs contributes to supply the energetic need of the parents. Hypotheses explaining the putative benefits of faecal sac ingestion are: (1) Parental-Nutrition Hypothesis, which predicts that recycling nutrients from faecal sacs are beneficial, increasing with energetic stress and brood size (McGowan 1995; McKay et al. 2009); the (2) Economic Disposal Hypothesis (Hurd et al. 1991) that proposes that the time saved by ingestion of faecal sacs may be invested in other activities, and the (3) Nest Predation Hypothesis that predicts that ingestion is beneficial by reducing parental movement in and out of the nest, therefore reducing the risk of nest predation and enabling higher nest attendance. In this hypothesis, frequency of faecal sac ingestion increases with larger brood sizes (Ibáñez-Álamo et al. 2013). The first two hypotheses predict higher frequency of ingestion from the most stressed and energy constrained sex, usually the female. In our case, however, males ingested a higher proportion of faecal sacs, which may imply a higher energetic demand, which is supported by the previously mentioned study reporting a reduction in male body condition during the breeding season in our study population (Floreste et al. 2021). Interestingly, the preferential method of faecal sac removal was ingestion throughout the whole nestling stage, contrary to the gradual decrease in faecal sac ingestion with the progress of the nesting stage usually reported in the literature (Darveau et al. 1993). Apart from the energetic demands of parents occasionally supplied by faecal sac ingestion, a lack of mechanical constraint due to the large size of parents in relation to the faecal sacs produced, even at later stages of nestling development, is what makes possible the continued ingestion of faecal sacs throughout the nestling period. Otherwise, faecal sacs would be likely removed rather than ingested (Darveau et al. 1993).

To conclude, several of our initial expectations for the division of reproductive tasks between the sexes in the Pale-breasted Thrush were not met, as the parental investment in our study species differs for most tasks. We have found that only males increased their effort by providing more food items for nestlings according to brood size, while a higher demand from offspring development is also supplied by both sexes with an increase in food provisioning and food quantity. Males also performed faecal sac removal more often than females. Our results then suggest a complex sexual conflict underlying the division of parental tasks in this monomorphic and socially monogamous bird.