Introduction

Brood parasitism in birds often implies a large cost to the parasitized hosts. These costs include egg losses, lower hatching success, lower chick survival and all other costs attributed to misdirected parental care (Petrie and Møller 1991; Øien et al. 1998; Rothstein and Robinson 1998; Davies 2000; Krüger 2007; Spottiswoode et al. 2012). If costs are important, natural selection should favour the evolution of host defences against brood parasites (Rothstein 1974, 1975; Briskie et al. 1992; Sealy et al. 1998; Peer and Sealy 2004).

The recognition and subsequent rejection of parasite eggs is perhaps the best studied host defensive behavior against brood parasites (Rothstein 1982; Sealy 1995; Sealy and Bazin 1995; Peer et al. 2000; Peer and Sealy 2004; Kilner and Langmore 2011). However, not all host species show the ability to recognize and reject parasite eggs (Rothstein 1974; Hauber et al. 2004; Peer and Sealy 2004). When brood parasites lay eggs of varying morphology (colour and/or pattern), hosts usually reject those eggs that are most dissimilar to their own eggs, but they fail to recognize eggs with a similar colouration and/or spotting pattern (Mason 1986; Mermoz and Reboreda 1994; Sackmann and Reboreda 2003). This variability in the response of the host to the polymorphism of the parasite’s egg is related to the host’s cognitive processes in the form of mechanisms of defence against parasites. In these cases, the matching of the pattern, size or colour between the host’s eggs (or a learned template of them) and the parasite’s eggs can be used to decide to reject the foreign egg or not (Rothsthein 1974, 1975; Moksnes 1992; Hauber and Sherman 2001; Hauber et al. 2006; Moskát and Hauber 2007; Moskát et al. 2010). It has been suggested that this assessment may involve an acceptance threshold based on similarity between the characteristics of the eggs of both, parasites and hosts (Reeve 1989; Liebert and Starks 2004; Hauber et al. 2006). This acceptance threshold should be somewhat flexible in order to maximize the benefits to the host of rejecting parasite eggs and, at the same time, minimize the costs of rejecting its own eggs due to incorrect egg recognition (Reeve 1989; Liebert and Starks 2004; Hauber et al. 2006). As the predictability of brood parasitism for a given species or population increases, the acceptance threshold should be more restrictive and, vice versa, as parasitism frequency decreases, the acceptance threshold should relax (Moksnes et al. 1993; Hauber et al. 2006).

In the study reported here we analysed the recognition and rejection behaviour of the brown-and-yellow marshbird (Pseudoleistes virescens), a species that inhabits marshy areas and humid grasslands in the pampas of Argentina and neighbouring areas of Uruguay and Brazil (Ridgely and Tudor 1989) and which is heavily parasitized by the shiny cowbird, Molothrus bonariensis (Hudson 1874; Gibson 1918; Orians 1980; Mermoz and Reboreda 1999). In our study population, frequency of parasitism has been found to average 65 % over a 15-year period (range 35–91 %; Mermoz et al. unpublished data). In addition, multiple parasitism (i.e. ≥2 cowbird eggs per parasitized nest) averaged 60 % of parasitized nests (range 50–80 %). The costs of cowbird parasitism to the host are related to the egg pecking behaviour of females (Mermoz and Reboreda 1994, 1999), lower hatching success of eggs in parasitized clutches (Mermoz et al. unpublished data) and lower survival of nestlings (Duré Ruiz et al. 2008; Mermoz et al. unpublished data). In the Southern temperate pampas shiny cowbird eggs are polymorphic in terms of spotting pattern, varying from completely immaculate white to heavily spotted (Mason 1986; Mahler et al. 2008). The brown-and-yellow marshbird lays brown-spotted eggs with a white background (Fig. 1). The results of previous studies in our study population have shown that the brown-and-yellow marshbird is able to recognize and reject those cowbird eggs which are more dissimilar to their own eggs in terms of spotting pattern (Mermoz and Reboreda 1994; Mermoz et al. 2013). Accordingly, the rejection rates of immaculate cowbird eggs are about 90 %, while those of spotted cowbird eggs are only 8 % (Mermoz et al. 2013). This variability in the rejection behaviour led to our study of the cues used by brown-and-yellow marshbirds to recognize and subsequently reject parasite eggs. We therefore designed and carried out a field experiment in which we added one mimetic egg (i.e. similar in appearance to the brown-and-yellow marshbird eggs) together with one non-mimetic (a white immaculate or a novel blue egg with spots) to the host’s nest. The addition of a novel blue egg with spots also constitutes an artificial stimulus that allowed us to assess if the egg rejection behaviour of brown-and-yellow marshbirds depends on previous experience with the phenotype of a foreign egg. Prevalent theories on the rejection of parasite eggs assume that host recognition of parasite eggs is based on the discordance of egg characteristics (rejecting the less common egg type in the clutch; Rothstein 1975, Hauber and Sherman 2001) or on the comparison of eggs with an imprinted (Rothstein 1974) or memory-based template of the characteristics of the host’s own eggs (Moksnes 1992; Lotem et al. 1995; Hauber and Sherman 2001). Both mechanisms imply that the acceptance threshold is based on dissimilarity between the host’s own eggs and the parasite eggs rather than on the aspect (phenotype) of the foreign egg. Consequently, we tested if experience with the egg phenotype of the parasite is important in determining the level of egg rejection in the brown-and-yellow marshbird. If the acceptance threshold is based on the dissimilarity between eggs, we would expect the host to have similar rejection rates for both non-mimetic foreign eggs regardless of the phenotype of the egg.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Examples of artificial parasite eggs used in the experiments. These artificial eggs were painted to simulate white immaculate cowbird (Molothrus bonariensis) eggs (Cwl; top row), novel blue eggs with spots (NBS; middle row) and spotted cowbird eggs (CwS; bottom row). b Examples of natural spotted cowbird eggs. c A naturally parasitized brown-and-yellow marshbird (Pseudoleistes virescens) nest with three host eggs (Pv) and two spotted cowbird eggs (Cw). d An experimental nest where we had added an artificial blue egg with spots (NBS) and one spotted cowbird egg (CwS). The eggs were accepted and the nest was later parasitized naturally by a cowbird (Cw)

The simultaneous addition of a spotted and a white immaculate or blue egg with spots also allowed us to analyse if the presence of dissimilar foreign egg morphs affects the acceptance threshold of mimetic (i.e. similar in appearance) parasite eggs. According to the acceptance threshold hypothesis, the acceptance threshold should be more restrictive when hosts detect an increased risk of parasitism (Reeve 1989; Hauber et al. 2006). In this case, in the presence of strongly dissimilar egg morphs, the acceptance threshold of more mimetic eggs is expected to be reduced.

Materials and methods

This study was carried out during 2002–2004 near the town of General Lavalle (36°26′ S, 56°25′ W) in the province of Buenos Aires, Argentina (for a detailed description of the study site see Mermoz et al. 2013).

We first assessed the decision rules used by brown-and-yellow marshbirds to recognize and reject cowbird eggs using artificial eggs made of plaster of Paris and painted with acrylic paint. Brown-and-yellow marshbirds are grasp-ejecters and do not break the egg during ejections. Their behaviour is the same with eggs constructed of plaster of Paris and natural eggs (Mermoz et al. 2013), regjecting artificial and natural eggs at similar rates (Haupt and Mermoz, unpublished data). Artificial eggs were painted to simulate a natural cowbird spotted egg [hereafter referred to as “spotted eggs” (CwS in Fig. 1)], the white morph of the parasite [hereafter referred to as “immaculate eggs” (CwI in Fig. 1)] and a novel blue egg with small brown spots [hereafter referred to as “blue eggs” (NBS in Fig. 1)]. These blue eggs did not resemble any egg morph of the parasite nor thos of the host (Fig. 1d) and constituted a novel stimulus for brown-and-yellow marshbirds.

We searched for nests during early spring (October–November) and chose early laying nests for the experiment (with one or two host eggs; clutch size of brown-and-yellow marshbirds is 4–5 eggs; Mermoz and Reboreda 1998). Nests were assigned to one of two treatments. In Treatment 1 (T1) we added simultaneously one spotted (CwS) and one blue (NBS) egg to the host nest; in Treatment 2 (T2) we added simultaneously one spotted (CwS) and one immaculate (CwI) egg to the host nest. The simultaneous occurrence of two cowbird eggs in one nest is not unusual for the hosts, as the nest-searching areas of female cowbirds overlap (Scardamaglia and Reboreda 2014) and host nests could be parasitized in the same day by two or more females (Mermoz and Reboreda 1999; Gloag et al. 2014). For example, in a study in the chalk-browed mockingbird (Mimus saturninus), the mean time interval between two parasitic events was found to be 5 min, with 31 % of the parasitic events occurring within 60 s of each other (Gloag et al. 2013; R. Gloag, personal communication).

We performed a total of 39 experiments (29 with the spotted/blue egg pair and 10 with the spotted/immaculate egg pair). All experiments were performed early in the morning (0600–0800 hours) and, to minimize disturbance to the hosts, we waited until the adults had left the vicinity of the nest to place the eggs in the nests. We monitored the focal nest (Altmann 1974) continuously for 45 min after the experimental parasitism using binoculars from a distance of 40–60 m. If the experimental eggs were not ejected during this observation period, we checked the nest 2–4 h later and then every 24 h up to a maximum of 5 days.

Data analyses

We compared the frequency of ejection of spotted cowbird eggs on each treatment with those we obtained in a previous study during 1992–1996 when an artificial spotted cowbird egg was put singly into brown-and-yellow marshbird nests (see Mermoz et al. 2013).

To facilitate the comparison of our previous results with those from the present study, we recalculated the ejection frequencies obtained in Mermoz et al. (2013) by including all experimental nests that lasted at least 2 days (see below). Our recalculated rate (21 %) differed from that reported by Mermoz et al. (2013) (8 %), as we included nests which were pecked by cowbirds or depredated. We compared the frequency of egg ejection in experimental nests between treatments using Fisher’s exact test.

We used a survival analysis using non-parametric Cox proportional hazard models to compare the time lapsed until each artificial egg (i.e. spotted, immaculate and blue eggs) was rejected (Jahn-Eimermacher et al. 2011). Eggs that remained in the nest after 5 days, or were depredated but remained in the nest at least 2 days, were considered censored data.

All p values related to the results are two-tailed, and differences were considered to be significant at p < 0.05.

Results

All eggs had high rejection rates. Within the most dissimilar egg types, blue eggs (NBS) were ejected from 22 of 28 nests (78.6 %; one experimental nest was deserted), whereas immaculate eggs (CwI) were ejected in equal proportion to the blue (NBS) ones (77.7 %; 7/9 nests; Fisher’s exact test p = 0.64; Fig. 2; one experimental nest was depredated during the first day after the experimental introduction). The ejection rates of spotted eggs (CwS) did not differ when they were introduced with the blue eggs (CwS/NBS) and when they were introduced with the familiar immaculate (CwS/CwI) eggs (Fisher’s exact test p = 0.45; Fig. 2). The mimetic spotted egg was ejected from 22 of 37 experimental nests (59 %; Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Proportion of artificially added eggs rejected by brown-and-yellow marshbirds according to egg colouration. NBS Novel blue eggs with spots, CwI immaculate white cowbird eggs, Singly CwS spotted cowbird eggs added singly, CwS/NBS spotted cowbird eggs rejected when added simultaneously with the novel blue ones with spots, CwS/CwI spotted cowbird eggs rejected when added simultaneously with cowbird immaculate ones. Numbers above bars Number of experiments

Spotted cowbird eggs added to the nests with immaculate (CwS/CwI) or blue (CwS/NBS) eggs suffered a higher rejection rate than those spotted cowbird eggs which were added singly (Singly CwS) into the nests (22/37 vs. 4/19 nests, respectively; Fisher’s exact test p = 0.009; Fig. 2). Although these experiments were performed over different time periods, there is no evidence that the rejection rates of cowbird eggs by brown-and-yellow marshbirds vary among years—the rejection rates of naturally laid cowbird spotted eggs were similar between studies. During 1992–1996, brown-and-yellow marshbirds ejected at least one cowbird spotted egg in 15 over 97 naturally parasitized nests, whereas during 2002–2004 they rejected five cowbird spotted eggs over 29 nests (Fisher’s exact test p = 0.42). Furthermore, 86.2 % of white immaculate cowbird eggs (25/29) were rejected during the 1992–1996 breeding periods (including the artificially added eggs), whereas 77.7 % of the added white immaculate cowbird eggs were rejected in present experiments (7/9; Fishers’ exact test p = 0.44).

Latency to rejection was also lower for the more dissimilar blue (NBS) and immaculate eggs (CwI) than for the mimetic spotted (CwS) eggs (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 9.18, p = 0.01; Fig. 3a). Mean rejection times were 1.71 ± 1.57 and 1.71 ± 1.65 days [mean ± standard deviation (SD)] for immaculate (CwI) and blue (NBS) eggs, respectively, whereas it was 2.64 ± 1.70 days for spotted eggs [2.71 ± 1.64 days when added with the blue egg (CwS/NBS) and 2.33 ± 2 days when added with the immaculate egg (CwS/CwI)]. Latency to rejection of spotted eggs was reduced when they were introduced with dissimilar eggs (\(\chi_{2}^{2}\) = 8.86, p = 0.01; Fig. 3b), compared to when they were introduced singly. The mean rejection time of spotted cowbird eggs was 2.71 ± 1.64 and 2.33 ± 2 days (mean ± SD) when added with the blue (CwS/NBS) and the immaculate eggs (CwS/CwI), respectively, whereas it was of 4.01 ± 1.46 days when they were added singly (Singly CwS; Fig. 3b).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Latency to rejection of artificially added eggs by brown-and-yellow marshbirds according to egg colour morph. a Comparison of rejection time of spotted cowbird (CwS), novel blue egg with spots (NBS) and immaculate cowbird (CwI) eggs. The CwS eggs were added simultaneously either with a NBS or a CwI egg. b Comparison of rejection times for spotted cowbird eggs when they were added singly (Singly CwS), simultaneously with one blue egg with spots (CwS/NBS, and simultaneously with one immaculate cowbird egg (CwS/CwI)

Discussion

We found that brown-and-yellow marshbirds rejected non-mimetic blue and white immaculate eggs at higher rejection rates, and more quickly, than they did cowbird spotted eggs. Although we had no knowledge of whether the individual hosts assessed in our study had any previous experience with immaculate cowbird eggs, all were naive to the novel blue egg. In addition, rejection behaviour towards the novel and the more familiar egg phenotype did not differ, providing evidence that experience with the parasite egg is not directly involved in the decision rule. The acceptance threshold in this species may be set only on a relative dissimilarity gradient in egg appearance, maximizing the benefits of rejecting parasite eggs relative to the costs of rejecting own eggs (Hauber et al. 2006). The notion that the acceptance threshold is based on dissimilarity rather than on the specific phenotype of the parasite eggs is implicit in all studies performed to date which have assessed the recognition cues of hosts by using artificially painted eggs to simulate non-mimetic parasite eggs (e.g., Móskat et al. 2008; Samaš et al. 2011; Bán et al. 2013; Hauber et al. 2014). Therefore, parasite egg rejection in most hosts may be based on a single and non-specific decision rule that requires only knowledge of the aspect of the host’s own eggs (either learned or imprinted) and which is based on the difference between own eggs and foreign eggs or objects (sensu Moskát et al. 2003).

The major result of our study is related to the shift in the acceptance threshold of mimetic parasite eggs when they were presented together with non-mimetic eggs. It has been proposed that this recognition threshold could vary depending on the predictability of brood parasitism (Reeve 1989; Hauber et al. 2006). According to this view, as the probability of brood parasitism increases, the recognition threshold should be more restrictive, and parasite egg rejection should increase. Our results support this prediction. When spotted cowbird eggs were added to the brown-and-yellow marshbirds nests singly, the percentage of egg ejection was 21 %. However, this percentage increased to approximately 60 % when they were added together with a dissimilar blue or white immaculate egg. Also, the latency to rejection of similar spotted cowbird eggs decreased in the presence of dissimilar eggs in the nest. Together, our results lead us to propose that brown-and-yellow marshbirds have a flexible capability to discriminate and reject parasite eggs. We did not specifically test the cognitive mechanisms underlying this rejection behaviour, but the shift of the acceptance threshold implies that the detection of a non-mimetic egg could trigger a more detailed inspection of nest content. As we parasitized nests when the host had one to two own eggs, birds saw their own and the parasite eggs simultaneously and, therefore, both a template-based recognition and an online self-referencing process (sensu Hauber and Sherman 2001) could be acting in a complementary manner. However, specific experiments should be conducted to test this hypothesis.

This study provides the first evidence of a shiny cowbird host having a recognition system for parasite eggs that is characterized by a relatively flexible acceptance threshold. However, there is experimental evidence indicating that not all of the hosts of the shiny cowbird have a flexible acceptance threshold. In an experiment similar to the one reported here, chalk-browed mockingbirds that were artificially sequentially parasitized with a novel egg without spots and with a spotted cowbird one rejected 45 % of eggs of the novel morph, but they never rejected the spotted cowbird eggs (de la Colina et al. 2012). The chalk-browed mockingbird apparently did not adaptively vary its acceptance threshold as breeding pairs did not modify their acceptance of spotted cowbird eggs, even if they had been previously exposed to a novel egg.

The efficiency of the adaptive flexible system of egg recognition in the brown-and-yellow marshbird as a defence mechanism is low. This species suffers a high parasitism rate (65 %; Mermoz et al., unpublished data) and only rejects a low proportion of parasite eggs (11 %), most of which are white immaculate eggs (Mermoz et al. 2013). It remains paradoxical why this species does not develop a more efficient recognition system. The brown-and-yellow marshbird is a grasp-ejecter that does not damage its own eggs during rejection (Mermoz et al. 2013). However, recognition errors with cowbird eggs having the spotted morph would be potentially high due to their similarity with host eggs (Fig. 1). A “dilution benefit” has also been proposed for chalk-browed mockingbirds which are parasitized multiple times. Egg-pecking by shiny cowbird females is one of the main costs of brood parasitism to large hosts (Reboreda et al. 2003) and, therefore, the presence of cowbird eggs in the nest would reduce the chance of host egg losses due to cowbird pecking (Gloag et al. 2012). Other possible explanations involve the existence of an important gene flow between host populations supporting different brood parasitism rates that might avoid the fixation of a more efficient recognition system (Takasu 1998; Barabás et al. 2004; Røskaft et al. 2006; Stokke et al. 2008). In the shiny cowbird 260 hosts have been recorded (Lowther 2013), and the metapopulation dynamics of those hosts could provide a marked advantage for this brood parasite. Together, the high similarity with most parasite eggs, the potential costs of rejection, the potential benefits of parasite egg acceptance and the existence of an important gene flow between host populations that differ in parasitism rates might preclude the evolution of perfect egg recognition in brown-and-yellow marshbirds.