Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia (VaD) account for the majority of dementia cases among elderly people [1]. Diagnosis of these disorders is essential to be able to provide appropriate pharmacological treatment and make a correct prognosis. Making a diagnosis is a multidimensional process based on medical history, neuroimaging, and behavioural observations, but it relies primarily on a neuropsychological assessment. This is especially so in the early stages of disease. Nevertheless, the differential diagnosis between the two pathologies is a highly challenging task. Although AD and VaD have traditionally been considered to result from different etiologies, there is a growing body of evidence suggesting that the two diseases share common pathogenic mechanisms. For instance, epidemiological data show that the canonical vascular risk factors are associated with both VaD and AD [2]. Furthermore, neuropathological data reveal an overlap between cerebrovascular disease and markers of AD in approximately 40 % of all post-mortem examinations [3, 4]. Finally, there may be similarities in the clinical presentation, as both AD and VaD unveil cognitive deterioration, functional impairment, and behavioural symptoms [5].

Although a large number of studies have examined possible differences in cognitive performance between AD and VaD, the data in the literature are conflicting. The most consistent findings suggest that AD is characterised by a greater impairment in episodic memory, whereas patients with VaD display greater deficits in executive/attentional abilities [6, 7]. Other studies have instead found marked executive functioning and working memory impairments in patients with mild-moderate AD and VaD, with no differences being observed between the two diseases [8, 9]. The main reason for these discrepant results likely relies on differences in study populations, arising from different enrolment criteria, patients’ demographic characteristics and severity of the illness. A further source of uncertainty relies in the inherent variability associated with the definition of VaD. VaD typically refers to cognitive and behavioural disorders associated with different cerebrovascular diseases, i.e., small vessel disease, multi-infarct disease, or strategic vascular lesions. The last two pathologies express a variety of cognitive dysfunctions, which depends on the site and extent of the damage, whereas in small vessel VaD, the most prominent type of cognitive impairment is a dysexecutive syndrome [10]. However, the executive dysfunctions can also be present in AD patients, under a variety of different patterns, depending on their vascular involvement [1113]. It is, therefore, an unsettled issue whether degenerative dementia without evidence of small vessel pathology (DD) and small vessel VaD present distinct clinical features.

The main aim of this study was to analyze the neuropsychological pattern of DD and small vessel VaD subjects in the early stages of disease by assessing two groups of patients, whose demographic characteristics and disease severity were matched. The secondary aim was to investigate differences in the progression of cognitive impairment between the two diseases as well as to assess the role of vascular risk factors. We hypothesized that in the early phase of cognitive impairment, there are differences between small vessel VaD and DD in both the neuropsychological pattern and cognitive progression.

Materials and methods

Subjects

We identified that, from a series of 278 consecutive outpatients referred to the Neuropsychological Service of the Dementia Center of Sant’Andrea Hospital (Rome, Italy), 83 patients with probable VaD (29.86 %) diagnosed according to the NINDS-AIREN criteria [14]. The diagnostic procedures included a neurological and neuropsychological evaluation, neuroimaging study (CT or MRI), routine blood analysis, thyroid function, homocysteine, vitamin B12, folate, and electroencephalogram. Single-Photon-Emission-Computerized Tomography was performed in some patients. The remaining 195 patients were diagnosed with the following diseases: 131 patients with probable DD (47.12 %), 27 with mixed dementia (9.71 %), 25 with probable FTD (8.99 %), and 12 with dementia and extrapyramidal diseases (4.32 %) (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flowchart of subjects included in the study. Data are mean ± SD

Eight of the 83 patients with probable VaD displayed strategic or multi-infarct lesions at the neuroimaging evaluation and were thus excluded from the data analysis. Seventy-five patients with probable VaD were consequently included in the study, in which they were matched for age and education level with 75 patients affected by DD. If a VaD patient matched more than one DD patient, the closest age was adopted as the decisive factor. DSM-IV TR criteria were used for the clinical diagnosis of dementia [15]. None of the subjects had other neurological or psychiatric disorders, hearing or vision impairments, severe metabolic disturbances, or a history of alcohol or drugs abuse or of head injury.

Procedure

All the subjects underwent a standard neuropsychological evaluation used as a screening tool in our center. Global cognitive functioning was assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) [16], whereas other areas of cognition were investigated by means of the following tests: selective attention (Visual Search—Attentional matrices) [17], episodic long-term memory (Story Recall test) [18], non-verbal logical reasoning and problem-solving ability (Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices) [19, 20], word generation by phonological and semantic cues (Phonological and Semantic Verbal Fluency test) [18, 21], auditory comprehension of complex sentences (Token test) [17, 22], and spatial abilities and constructional praxis (Copying Drawings) [17, 23]. The raw scores of each test were adjusted for age and education according to the distribution of Italian normative data.

The severity of cognitive impairment was stratified according to the MMSE score as follows: subjects with an MMSE score ≥21 were defined as mild, whereas subjects with an MMSE score ≤20 were defined as moderate.

Fifteen subjects with DD and ten subjects with probable VaD underwent a cognitive re-evaluation based on the same neuropsychological battery after 1 year (Fig. 1). The low number of re-tested subjects was due to many reasons, such as behavioural problems, appearance of new comorbidities, relatives/caregivers not available, and severity of cognitive deficit. In re-test patients, the presence/absence of six vascular risk factors and of an internal carotid stenosis were evaluated as follows: (1) arterial hypertension: history of blood pressure values higher than 160/95 mmHg or antihypertensive medication intake; (2) hypercholesterolaemia: serum cholesterol level over 220 mg/dl or statin intake; (3) hypertriglyceridaemia: serum triglyceride level over 140 mg/dl; (4) diabetes mellitus: plasma glucose level over 110 mg/dl or anti-diabetic drug intake; (5) heart disease: previous myocardial infarction or electrocardiographic evidence of atrial fibrillation; (6) smoking: more than five cigarettes per day for at least 5 years; non-smoking: less than five cigarettes per day or stopped smoking at least 10 years before; and (7) internal carotid stenosis: stenosis exceeding 30 % of at least one internal carotid on Doppler ultrasound examination.

Statistical analysis

All the analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, Version 13.0). Any differences between DD and VaD patients in the demographic characteristics, MMSE score, and neuropsychological performance were analyzed using t test for independent samples. Neuropsychological scores, excluding the MMSE score, were also treated as categorical variables rather than continuous variables and dichotomized for each test between impaired performance and normal performance according to the cutoff for Italian normative data; in this last case, the performance of DD and VaD patients was compared using χ 2 test.

Neuropsychological test and re-test scores were compared using the paired t test. Spearman’s non-parametric rank correlation was used to assess any correlation between the neuropsychological values in the re-test examination and the presence/absence of vascular risk factors. Finally, any differences in the presence/absence of vascular risk factors in the two subject groups were evaluated by Chi-square and Fischer’s exact tests. Statistical significance was set at P < 0.05.

Results

The demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of the whole sample are summarized in Table 1. The mean MMSE score was similar in the two groups (mean value ± SD: 24.4 ± 4.1 in DD patients and 24.3 ± 4.9 in VaD patients). Mild cognitive impairment (MMSE ≥ 21) was present in 62 subjects in each of the groups, whereas the remaining 13 subjects in each group displayed a moderate cognitive impairment (MMSE ≤ 20).

Table 1 Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological scores in DD and VaD subjects

No significant difference was found between DD and VaD subjects in any of the neuropsychological tests except Story Recall (mean value ± SD: 5.5 ± 3.9 vs 8.4 ± 5.1, respectively; P = 0.0001) (Table 1). Furthermore, the percentage of patients, whose performance was impaired in the Story Recall test, was higher in DD subjects (65 %) than in VaD subjects (28 %) (P = 0.001; Chi-square test).

In keeping with the severity of cognitive impairment, DD subjects with mild cognitive impairment performed worse in the Story Recall test than VaD subjects (mean value ± SD: 6.08 ± 4.48 vs 9.08 ± 4.57; P < 0.001); by contrast, no differences emerged between DD and VaD subjects with moderate cognitive impairment.

Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics and the MMSE score at baseline of both the subjects who underwent and of those who did not undergo the 12-month neuropsychological re-evaluation. The only difference that emerged was in the MMSE score, which was higher in subjects who were re-evaluated in both the groups. In DD patients, the mean re-test value was significantly worse than the mean baseline value in the following tests: MMSE (P = 0.037), Corsi (P = 0.041), Story Recall test (P = 0.032), Phonological Verbal Fluency test (P = 0.02), and Copying Drawings (P = 0.043); the only significant worsening in the VaD subjects was instead detected in the Visual Search test (P = 0.036) (Table 3).

Table 2 Demographic characteristics and MMSE score at baseline in DD and VaD subjects with or without 12-month re-evaluation
Table 3 Comparison of test and re-test neuropsychological scores in DD and VaD subjects

The presence of vascular risk factors was higher in VaD than in DD subjects, though the difference did not attain statistical significance (Table 4). In DD subjects, the number of vascular risk factors was correlated with the drop in the MMSE score (r = 0.711, P = 0.003; Spearman’s correlation), though not with the drop in each neuropsychological test at the 12-month follow-up. Finally, no correlation was detected between the number of vascular risk factors and either overall or specific cognitive decline in VaD subjects.

Table 4 Vascular risk factors in 15 DD subjects and 10 VaD subjects with 12-month neuropsychological follow-up

Discussion

Our results show that in the early phase of cognitive decline, Story Recall scores were significantly worse in dementia patients without evidence of small vessel pathology than in those with small vessel VaD. The Story Recall test provides one of the most reliable neuropsychological assessments for episodic long-term verbal memory function and is widely used in clinical settings. Some studies have consistently shown that medial temporal lobe and cortical-subcortical frontal structures interact in the long-term memory [24, 25]. A storage (or retention) failure is caused by damage to medial temporal structures, whereas a retrieval failure is caused by a frontal-subcortical dysfunction [26]. In processing the episodic long-term memory, when storage is efficient, the inherent organization of prose material can facilitate the encoding and retrieval of the cognitive operation. By contrast, when storage is impaired, performance in the Story recall test is invariably compromised [27]. This is indeed what we observed in our DD patients. This finding can be explained by neuropathological changes that prevalently occur in medial temporal regions in the early stages of AD. A storage failure in episodic long-term memory in the early AD is consequently not surprising [28, 29]. In our study, all the subjects with VaD had small vessel cerebrovascular disease, with changes in white matter being responsible for the subcortical brain damage. Although this damage is as severe as to cause a general cognitive impairment and to yield a clinical diagnosis of dementia, it does not affect the medial temporal lobe either so early or to such a selective degree. This explains the preserved memory storage that we observed in our VaD subjects. In keeping with the findings of the previous studies, we may hypothesize that, in the early stages of the disease, small vessel vascular dementia subjects compensate for any failure in retrieval when tested by means of the Story Recall test more effectively than DD patients [30].

Another interesting observation emerges from the analysis of the 12-month neuropshycological follow-up. DD subjects displayed a faster cognitive decline than VaD subjects in most of the tasks, which suggests that all the cognitive domains are rapidly involved. By contrast, VaD subjects perform more stably when compared with their baseline values in all the tests except Visual Search, which consists of an attentive task that measures a person’s ability to select a target within a visual scanning context. According to the most recent neuropsychological theories, attention is a complex system that presides over a number of distinct neuronal circuits. Three largely disparate attentional control systems have been identified: alerting, orienting, and executive control. Although these systems interact in many practical contexts, they are to a certain degree functionally and anatomically independent [31]. According to Posner’s theory, Visual Search can be conceptualized as an orienting task, and more specifically as the process of selecting information from sensory input [32], which is activated by a cortico-subcortical network involving the pulvinar, superior colliculus, superior parietal lobe, and frontal eye fields [33]. These neural structures contribute to the representation of spatial awareness as a component of the dorsal pathway that connects visual cortex areas to the frontal lobe [34]. In our VaD subjects, the rapid decline in the attention domain may be explained by the involvement of cerebral white matter. As all our VaD patients had small vessel disease associated with marked subcortical ischemic vascular involvement of cerebral white matter, we may hypothesize that the rapid decline in attention was caused by the interruption in the fronto-subcortical networks. This hypothesis is supported by the recent MRI studies in which an association was found between the impairment in the frontal-parietal-subcortical network and the decline in executive functions [35, 36].

In keeping with the findings of the previous studies, our data suggest that vascular risk factors may exacerbate the cognitive decline in subjects affected by dementia [37, 38]. We found a positive correlation between the number of vascular risk factors and the reduced MMSE score in DD subjects. Vascular risk factors, such as hypertension, diabetes, smoking, and hypercholesterolemia, are known to target the neurovascular unit, upon which they exert a deleterious effect. This effect causes neurovascular dysfunction and increases the brain’s susceptibility to injury by altering the regulation of the cerebral blood supply, by disrupting the blood brain barrier function, and by reducing the trophic support and the potential repair of the injured brain [39]. We cannot explain the lack of correlation between the vascular risk factors and cognitive decline in our VaD patients. One reason may be the small sample size used in our study. However, Pavlovic et al. recently reported, in keeping with the results of other studies, that the cognitive decline in patients with small vessel disease is related to the severity of white matter hyperintensity, and not to vascular risk factors [40]. Therefore, in contrast to AD, we cannot exclude that neurovascular function in small vessel VaD is so highly damaged that vascular risk factors do not exert any effect on cognitive decline.

Although our study has certain limitations, such as the small sample size and the short follow-up, the results that we obtained are supported by a comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests and by closely matched groups of patients. These results suggest that a neuropsychological evaluation helps to differentiate degenerative dementia without evidence of small vessel pathology from small vessel VaD in the early stages of these diseases when cognitive impairments are not yet severe. In these stages, episodic verbal memory function is impaired to a greater extent in dementia patients without evidence of small vessel pathology than in those with small vessel VaD. Furthermore, our results suggest that cognitive decline progresses at different speeds in these two diseases: progression is faster in DD than in small vessel VaD and is only influenced by the presence of vascular risk factors in DD. Further research on the comparative neuropsychological evaluation of dementia patients without evidence of small vessel pathology and VaD patients affected by small vessel diseases is required to shed more light on the neuropsychological pattern in the early phases of these two diseases. Future studies should be based on a large sample size, careful characterization of patients, and MRI evaluation.