Introduction

Movement and resource selection patterns of individuals determine population-level distributions across heterogeneous landscapes (Lima and Zollner 1996). Individuals make choices perceived to maximize net quality of available food, cover, and other resources contributing to survival and reproductive success (i.e., fitness; Cooper and Millspaugh 1999). Patterns in choice reflect resource requirements and the relative value of available habitats. Resource selection is a spatial process describing animal movements through a variable landscape (Thurfjell et al. 2014; Avgar et al. 2016). Individuals tend to stay in or move toward environments perceived to maximize resource quality and minimize predation risk (Dinkins et al. 2012; De Cesare et al. 2014; Atuo and O’Connell 2018).

Population distributions and individual home ranges incorporate patterns of daily movement and resource valuation (Schooley 1994; Mayor et al. 2009; Dzialak et al. 2012). Selection also varies by individual and can be influenced by intrinsic factors, such as life stage, breeding status, and body condition (Paasivaara and Pöysä 2008; Brooke et al. 2015; Kane et al. 2017). For young animals, habitat choices and movement patterns may be influenced by age and development (Wiltermuth et al. 2015; Jenkins et al. 2017). Patterns of resource selection and movement can cascade to influence survival of young (Rotella and Ratti 1992; Mainguy et al. 2006; Goddard and Dawson 1999). The contribution of variation in individual behavior to population-level processes, such as resource selection and survival, has not been thoroughly addressed in applied ecological research (Merrick and Koprowski 2017).

Northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus; hereafter bobwhite) is an early successional, disturbance-dependent species experiencing range-wide declines on rapidly changing landscapes in North America (Hernández et al. 2013). Productivity and recruitment contribute significantly to bobwhite population growth (McConnell et al. 2018). Nest-site characteristics for bobwhite have been well studied; vegetation must provide adequate cover to conceal incubating adults and nests from predators and protect them from heavy rainfall or temperature extremes (Klimstra and Roseberry 1975; Collins et al. 2009; Carroll et al. 2015). During the brood phase, vegetation must additionally facilitate movement and foraging activities of young. Many factors, such as dispersal from nesting habitat, nutritional requirements, predation pressure, weather, and mobility, can explain brood resource selection and movement decisions (Sonerud 1985; Rotella and Ratti 1992; Tanner et al. 2017).

We quantified resource selection and movement patterns of bobwhite broods from hatch through their development period. We evaluated brood choices across a mixed landscape of grasslands managed for wildlife, agricultural croplands, cool-season pastures, and hayfields. We estimated population-level selection patterns within a Bayesian hierarchical integrated step-selection analysis (iSSA; Online Resource 1; Avgar et al. 2016). We evaluated both movement and resource utility by incorporating daily step length patterns into our evaluation of resource selection (Fortin et al. 2005; Forester et al. 2009; Thurfjell et al. 2014; Avgar et al. 2016). We also evaluated changes in selection patterns during the development period and the influence of variation in this behavior on the fate of young broods < 35 days old. Within our full resource selection models, we tested age- and fate-specific hypotheses. We had a priori expectations of differential brood responses to the following vegetation types:

  1. (a)

    Agriculture. Young broods would avoid agricultural croplands due to their high protein diet requirements and a lack of adequate invertebrate prey in cropland monocultures (Palmer et al. 2001). Accordingly, we expected successful young broods would avoid agriculture, while failed broods may show stronger selection for agriculture (Taylor and Burger 2000).

  2. (b)

    Idle native grasslands. Young broods would avoid native grasslands that were not burned or grazed within the previous two years due to juveniles’ small body size and limited mobility through thick, undisturbed vegetation (Doxon and Carroll 2010). We expected successful young broods to avoid idle native grasslands, while failed broods may show stronger selection for those grasslands.

  3. (c)

    Shrub cover. Broods would select for shrub cover throughout their development period as shrubs provide thermal protection and overhead cover from predators (Perkins et al. 2014; Carroll et al. 2015; Unger et al. 2015; Brooke et al. 2017). Successful young broods would show stronger selection for protective shrub cover than young broods that failed.

  4. (d)

    Tree composition. Broods would avoid tree cover and areas close to trees, as they provide perches for aerial predators and habitat for mesocarnivores, such as Red-tailed hawks (Buteo jamaicensis) and raccoons (Procyon lotor; Dinkins et al. 2012; Dzialak et al. 2012). Young flightless broods would select areas farther from trees compared to older broods who could flush from predators. Successful young broods would select areas with less tree cover and areas farther from trees compared failed broods (Lohr et al. 2011).

  5. (e)

    Daily movement. Daily distance travelled would be greater with age and successful broods would move shorter distances on average compared to failed broods (Lohr et al. 2011).

Methods

Study area

We tracked movement and resource use of bobwhite broods on five state conservation areas and neighboring private lands in southwest Missouri from 01 June to as late as 13 December 2016–2018 (Fig. 1a). Wah’Kon-Tah Prairie (1226 ha), Stony Point Prairie Conservation Area (388 ha), and Wade and June Shelton Memorial Conservation Area (129 ha) were extensive native grassland sites managed with fire, grazing, and mowing (Fig. 1b). Shawnee Trail (1471 ha) and Robert E. Talbot Memorial Conservation Areas (1764 ha) were intensively managed sites that incorporated narrow rows of strip crops and woody field borders among larger grassland units, providing food and cover for wildlife in smaller management units (2–24 ha; Fig. 1c). Native grasslands on conservation areas included remnant and reconstructed prairies composed of warm season grasses (e.g. Andropogon gerardii, Schizachyrium scoparium, and Sorghastrum nutans) and diverse, abundant forbs (e.g. Liatris pycnostachya, Echinacea pallida, and Solidago spp.). Mixed grasslands on conservation areas and private lands were composed of a mix of native warm season grasses, forbs (Ambrosia spp.), and non-native grasses (Festuca arundinacea, Dactylis glomerata, Phleum pratense). Private lands surrounding these conservation areas were primarily cool-season grazing pastures and hayfields (Festuca arundinacea, Dactylis glomerata, Phleum pratense), closed canopy woodland, with agricultural fields of corn (Zea mays), soybean (Glycine max), and winter wheat (Triticum spp.). Grassland management on conservation areas included prescribed burning, conservation grazing, disking, and woody and invasive species control (herbicide and mowing).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study site locations on five conservation areas in southwest Missouri (left). Extensive native prairies (center) are larger continuous tracts of native grassland (dark grey) interspersed with shrubs and trees (black) and bordered by non-native grasslands (light grey). Intensively managed conservation areas (right) incorporate agricultural strips (white), woodland units and woody fencerows (black) within a matrix of restored native prairies (dark grey) and non-native mixed or cool-season grasslands (light grey)

Brood tracking and captures

We monitored resource selection and movement patterns of bobwhite broods daily using radio-telemetry. Most bobwhite broods were tracked from hatch. Some broods were opportunistically tracked post hatch when radio-tagged adults who did not participate in nest incubation were discovered attending broods. For broods ≤ 3 weeks old, habitat choices were determined by tracking radio-tagged attending adults. For broods > 3 weeks old, we used locations of radio-tagged juveniles to evaluate brood resource selection patterns if attending adults were depredated or abandoned their independent young. We found young brood mates tended to move together as a unit through the summer and into fall even without an attending adult present.

We captured broods 2–4 weeks old using the corral technique and a thermal imaging camera before first light (Smith et al. 2003). Our corrals were 1 m tall with a PVC pipe frame and panel walls made from extra-strength window screen. We also used garden netting as a cover for the corral to prevent individuals from flushing and escaping on capture. We sutured radio-tags on one to six juveniles per brood weighing > 20 g with 0.6 to 0.8 g transmitters (American Wildlife Enterprises, Monticello, FL, USA), which had 45–60 days of expected battery life (Terhune et al. 2020). Transmitters never exceeded 4% of a juvenile’s body mass. We attached transmitters low on the back, below the scapula along the thoracic vertebrae, and clipped antennae to 7 cm to avoid entanglement in grasses (Terhune et al. 2020).

Broods were tracked on average 6 days per week and observations with at least two consecutive daily locations were included in our resource selection analysis as a used step. We rotated the order of broods tracked and collected at least one roost location before first light each week. Broods were tracked to within 10 m of their location each day unless they were inaccessible, such as on restricted private lands, in which case locations were triangulated. Brood locations were projected from observer UTM coordinates and the distance and azimuth to the brood. We collected data on cover type, management, and woody vegetation at each location. All guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed and animal procedures were approved under University of Missouri Animal Care and Use Committee protocol #8766.

Cover type covariates

We included covariates representing the structural and compositional diversity of vegetation on conservation areas and their surrounding private lands. Classification of herbaceous cover types, management practices, and woody vegetation structure was informed by bobwhite ecological literature and applied management objectives. Herbaceous cover types included agricultural row crop (Ag), idle agricultural fields (Agid), native grass (N), mixed grass (M), and cool-season grass (C) and were characterized by percent cover within a 50 m radius for each used and available location (Online Resource Table S1). We further characterized grassland cover types by management practices using a 2-year burning (pb) and grazing (gr) history or a within-year mowing history (mw); we classified grasslands as idle (id) if no management had occurred over a two-year period (Online Resource Table S1). Burns occurred September–March prior to each breeding season, while grazing occurred April–September for 90- or 120-day periods during the brood season. For example, the category Npbgr classified native grasslands that were burned and grazed at least once either in the same year or two separate years within a two-year management history. Ngr classified native grasslands that were grazed at least once in the previous two years, but not burned. These included patches within patch-burn grazing systems that had not been burned in two years and units within conservation grazing-only systems. We hypothesized grasslands managed with fire, grazing or both may have greater utility than idle grasslands for bobwhite broods, as these disturbances increase bare ground, plant diversity and arthropod abundance (Taylor et al. 1999; Engle et al. 2008). Croplands were available to broods within intensively managed sites and on private lands surrounding all conservation areas except Wah’Kon-Tah. Intensively managed sites maintained small units of crops and food plots for wildlife use. Much of the private lands surrounding conservation areas in southwest Missouri, USA have been converted to pasture, hayfields, and cultivated crop. These cover types were largely classified from field observations of plant community composition and maps from area wildlife managers. Where field crew access was limited, some private lands were classified using 2018 CropScape and 2016 National Agriculture Imagery Program datasets (USDA 2016, 2018). We characterized woody vegetation composition and configuration as either percent shrub (Sh) or tree cover (Tr) within 50 m and by distance to nearest tree (TrD; m) from each used and available location. Shrub and tree cover were mapped using Light Detection and Ranging data. We created a digital surface model and a digital elevation model at 3.6 m resolution and calculated their difference to estimate vegetation height in ArcGIS. We classified elevations of 0–0.7 m as ground cover, 0.7–3.5 m as shrub cover, and 3.5–40 m as tree cover. We validated woody cover classification by comparing results with ground-truthed GPS points and aerial imagery (USDA 2016) across the study extent.

Used-available sampling design

We simultaneously evaluated effects of bobwhite brood age on habitat selection and daily movement decisions in a Bayesian, hierarchical conditional logistic regression that included both a resource utilization kernel and a movement kernel (Avgar et al. 2016). We incorporated spatial processes into a biologically meaningful sample of availability by drawing control points from an empirical distribution of observed movements (Fortin et al. 2005; Rhodes et al. 2005; Forester et al. 2009; Thurfjell et al. 2014). We sampled ten random, available step lengths from a log normal distribution based on observed daily movement distributions of each brood (Online Resource 2; Eq. S1, Eq. S2). We also sampled ten random, available step headings from a uniform distribution of all possible headings (0–2π radians). Vegetation types at end points of each used step were then compared to ten projected end points of each available step.

We included all daily brood step observations for our population-level, age-specific, step-selection analysis. To compare selection patterns between broods that failed and broods that succeeded, we only used step observations for broods ≤ 35 days old and we excluded broods whose fate could not be determined at 35 days old.

Hierarchical Bayesian selection model

We estimated the probability (P) brood (n) in age or fate class (k) would use location (j) with habitat features (m) at time (t) given a set (J) of available steps and locations(i) where i ≠ j using a conditional logistic regression (\(P\left( {x_{njktm} } \right)\); Eq. 1). Resource selection was attributed to multiple cover type and management predictors m observed at the end point of each daily used step H(\(x_{njtm}\)) and each daily available step H(\(x^{\prime}_{nitm}\)). Resource selection was quantified as a vector of cover type coefficients describing selection for each brood and each age or fate class (\(\beta_{nkm}\); Eq. 1). Movement patterns were estimated from observations of daily distance travelled (\(L_{njt}\)) and quantified in the movement coefficient vector \(\alpha_{nk}\) for each age and fate class. Variation in selection and movement among age classes and between broods that survived and broods that failed at 35 days were evaluated via an interaction with a class dummy variable, \(S_{nkt}\) (Eq. 1; Cooper and Millspaugh 1999; Bloom et al. 2013). The linear function representing the utility (\(U\)) of location \(j\) at time \(t\) to brood \(n\) for class \(k\), (\(U_{njtkm}\)), provided by a set of m habitat resources is \(U_{njktm} = \beta_{nkm} \cdot x_{njtm} \cdot S_{nkt}\).

$$ P\left( {x_{njktm} } \right){ }\frac{{\exp \left[ {{ }\beta_{nkm} \cdot H\left( {x_{njtm} } \right) \cdot S_{nkt} + \alpha_{nk} \cdot \ln \left( {L_{njt} } \right)^{2} \cdot S_{nkt} } \right]}}{{\mathop \sum \nolimits_{i = 0}^{J} \exp \left[ {\beta_{nkm} \cdot H\left( {x^{\prime}_{nitm} } \right) \cdot S_{nkt} + \alpha_{nk} \cdot \ln \left( {L^{\prime}_{nit} } \right)^{2} \cdot S_{nkt} { }} \right]}} . $$
(1)

We implemented our iSSA in a hierarchical Bayesian framework to account for individual- and population-level variations through random sampling (Gillies et al. 2006). Brood-level random regression coefficient vectors for resource selection, \(\beta_{nkm}\) and movement, \(\alpha_{nk}\), represented samples of population-level selection patterns, \(\beta_{km}^{*}\) and \(\alpha_{k}^{*}\) (Gillies et al. 2006; Thomas et al. 2006). This model structure included vague prior distributions for brood-level parameters (Eq. 2), and hyperparameters (Eq. 3; Thomas et al. 2006). Hyperparameter vectors, \(\beta_{km}^{*}\) and \(\alpha_{k}^{*}\), described mean population-level selection behavior and their precision, \(\sigma_{\beta km}^{*}\) and \(\sigma_{\alpha k}^{*}\) (Thomas et al. 2006). Random regression coefficients in the conditional logistic likelihood function address problems of non-independence among consecutive observations of individuals, differences in availability and preferences among individuals, and unbalanced data across individuals sampled (Gilles et al. 2006; Duchesne et al. 2010). This hierarchical structure allows inference on population-level patterns, by accounting for bias associated with unbalanced, correlated observations of individual animals, dependence of local availability on choice, and animal movement patterns (Online Resource 3; Gillies et al. 2006; Thomas et al. 2006; Duchesne et al. 2010).

$$ \beta_{nkm} \sim {\text{dnorm}}\left( {\beta \cdot H_{km}^{*} , \sigma \cdot H_{km}^{*} } \right), $$
(2)
$$ \alpha_{nk} \sim {\text{dnorm}}\left( {\alpha \cdot M_{k}^{*} , \sigma \cdot M_{k}^{*} } \right), $$
$$ \beta \cdot H_{km}^{*} \sim {\text{dnorm}}\left( {0, 0.0001} \right), $$
(3)
$$ \alpha \cdot M_{k}^{*} \sim {\text{dnorm}}\left( {0, 0.0001} \right), $$
$$ \sigma \cdot H_{km}^{*} \sim {\text{dgamma}}\left( {0.01, 0.01} \right), $$
$$ \sigma \cdot M_{\alpha k}^{*} \sim {\text{dgamma}}\left( {0.01, 0.01} \right). $$

Candidate model development and evaluation

We evaluated a set of cover type and management parameters representative of vegetation available on public and private lands. This set included agricultural row crop (Ag; corn, soybean, sunflower (Helianthus annuus), winter wheat), idle agriculture (Agid), native grasslands that were idle (Nid), grazed (Ngr), burned (Npb), or mowed/hayed (Nmw), and mixed grasslands that were idle (Mid), grazed (Mgr), burned (Mpb), or mowed (Mmw). We examined multicollinearity among habitat covariates within models and avoided combinations of covariates that resulted in a variable inflation factor (VIF) > 2.5. We excluded cool-season grasslands to avoid multicollinearity (Online Resource 4).

We evaluated age- and fate-specific resource selection behaviors for agriculture, idle native grasslands, shrub (Sh) and tree cover (Tr), distance to trees (TrD), and daily step length distributions (Step; Table S1). This set of available cover types was evaluated as the most parsimonious variables addressing (1) the value of agricultural units available to broods on intensively managed sites and surrounding private lands, (2) the importance of disturbance on native grassland utility, (3) the influence of woody composition on brood choice, and (4) changes in habitat availability with growth and development. To evaluate age-specific variation in resource selection, brood stages were classified based on the ages at which young can fly short distances (≤ 14 days) and females may abandon a brood to attempt a second nest (35 days). A three-stage model included broods that were flightless ≤ 14 days old, dependent 14–35 days old, and independent > 35 days old. A two-stage model compared selection behavior of young broods ≤ 35 days old and old broods > 35 days old. A single-stage model included no age class information. To evaluate the influence of choice on survival of young, we subset our data to include only observations of broods whose fate at 35 days was known. We then compared selection behavior of successful and failed broods ≤ 35 days old.

We fit resource selection models in a Bayesian framework in Program R version 3.6.1 (R Core Development Team 2017) using JAGS (Plummer 2003) via the JagsUI package (Kellner 2019). All models evaluating age- and fate-dependent patterns in resource selection were run with 3 chains of 30,000 iterations, a burn-in of 10,000 iterations, and a thinning rate of 4, which resulted in 15,000 total posterior samples for each parameter. We inspected trace plots for mixing across Markov chains, checked that R-hat values were < 1.1 (Brooks and Gelman 1998). We evaluated model performance by ranking models based on Widely Applicable Information Criterion and interpreting the top model (WAIC, Watanabe 2010). The proportions of posterior samples that do not span zero (f value) indicate the strength of habitat selection (positive) or avoidance (negative). The magnitude and uncertainty of age- and fate-dependent patterns in resource selection were quantified as the proportion of posterior distributions non-overlapping between age or fate classes.

Results

Population-level brood resource selection and movement

We monitored resource selection patterns of 101 bobwhite broods for a total of 2790 step choice sets from 2016 to 2018. These data contained 627 steps from 80 broods ≤ 14 days old, 1092 steps from 91 broods 15 to 35 days old, and 1071 steps from 45 broods 36 to 114 days old. Brood step length was not correlated with variation in hours between daily tracking locations; therefore, we did not include time within resource selection models (Online Resource Figure S1). Ranges of habitat and step length data for used and available locations are summarized in Online Resource Table S1.

All resource selection models performed better than the null model (Online Resource Table S2). The model containing 3 stage-specific estimates for resource selection and step length distribution performed better than the 2-stage and the null model that did not consider effects of age class (∆WAIC = 35.8, 105.2, respectively; Online Resource Table S2). Therefore, we focused interpretation on the top-ranked model that included parameters for resource selection and step length distribution for three stages: flightless broods ≤ 14 days old, dependent broods 15 to 35 days old, and independent broods > 35 days old.

Broods showed the strongest selection among native grasslands for areas that were burned and grazed within the past two years (Npbgr = 0.41, 95% CRI 0.24, 0.57, f = 1.00), followed by areas burned only (Npb = 0.31, 95% CRI 0.05, 0.54 f = 0.99), and mowed (Nmw = 0.21, 95% CRI 0.05, 0.35, f = 0.99; Fig. 2, Fig. 3a, b, Online Resource Table S3). Broods showed weak selection for native grasslands that were grazed only (Ngr = 0.12, 95% CRI = − 0.11, 0.30, f = 0.88; Fig. 2, Online Resource Table S3). Broods > 35 days old selected for idle native grasslands (Nid[2] = 0.36, 95% CRI = 0.05, 0.63, f = 0.99), whereas broods ≤ 14 days old did not select for idle native grasslands (Nid[0] =  − 0.16, 95% CRI = − 0.73, 0.22, f = 0.74; Fig. 2, Fig. 3c, Online Resource Table S3). Broods > 35 days showed stronger selection for idle native grasslands than broods ≤ 14 days old (proportion non-overlapping posteriors 0.843; Table S4). Among mixed grasslands, bobwhite selected for areas mowed in that year (Mmw = 0.29, 95% CRI = 0.15, 0.42, f = 1.00). All age classes were selected for agricultural row crop and idle agricultural fields (Ag[0] = 0.39, 95% CRI = 0.12, 0.66; Ag[1] = 0.36, 95% CRI = 0.12, 0.58; Ag[2] = 0.52, 95% CRI = 0.33, 0.69, f = 1.00; Fig. 2, Fig. 3d, Online Resource Table S3, Table S4). Among woody habitat components, broods were selected for shrub cover and avoided locations further from trees (Figs. 2, 3e, f, Online Resource Table S3). Selection for shrub cover and avoidance of areas farther from trees was stronger for older broods than younger broods (0.968 and 0.871 non-overlapping posteriors, respectively; Figs. 2, 3e, f, Online Resource Table S4). Broods ≤ 14 days old took shorter steps (mean = 129 m) and travelled < 200 m in a day more frequently than older age classes, while broods > 35 days old took longer steps (mean = 167) and travelled > 200 m more frequently than younger age classes (1.00 non-overlapping posteriors; Fig. 4, Online Resource Table S3, Table S4). The 95th percentile of our step length distribution curves for young flightless broods ≤ 14 days old, dependent broods 15–35 days old, and independent broods > 35 days were 309 m, 334 m, and 404 m, respectively.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Posterior distributions of population-level habitat selection parameters estimating utility of agricultural fields (Ag), native grasslands (N), and mixed grasslands (M) that were idle (id), grazed (gr), burned (pb), grazed and burned (pbgr), or mowed (mw), as well as brood selection for shrub (Sh) and tree cover (Tr), and selection for distance from tree (TrD) for northern bobwhite broods in southwest Missouri 2016–2018. The solid line at y = 0 represents the threshold between support for selection (+) or avoidance (−) of each habitat parameters. Stage-specific effects S[−] are presented with black mean point estimates for broods ≤ 14 days old [0] (black circle), 14 to 35 days old [1] (black triangle), and > 35 days old [2] (black square) and 95% CRI (bar). Habitat selection parameters that are stage-independent display mean point estimates in white (white square) and 95% CRI (bar)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Predicted relative probability of use of habitats by northern bobwhite broods in southwest Missouri 2016–2018 as a function of: a percent cover within 50 m of native grassland that was burned and grazed, or b only burned and not grazed within the past two years; and stage-dependent predicted relative probability of use as a function of c percent cover within 50 m of idle native grassland, d agricultural crop, e shrub cover, and f predicted relative probability of use as a function of distance to nearest tree for young flightless broods ≤ 14 days old (S[0]; dotted line), dependent broods 14 to 35 days old (S[1]; dashed line), and independent broods > 35 days old (S[2]; solid line)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Distributions of daily distanced travelled by northern bobwhite broods in southwest Missouri 2016–2018 derived from the mean, 2.5, and 97.5% credible intervals of posterior samples of the step length parameter. Stage-specific step length distributions for broods ≤ 14 days old (S[0], dotted line, light grey), 14–35 days old (S[1], dashed line, medium grey), and > 35 days old (S[2], solid line, dark grey). Mean predicted daily distance travelled for each stage are plotted with point estimates for broods ≤ 14 days old (black circle), 15–35 days old (black triangle), and > 35 days old (black square)

Resource selection and movement patterns of successful and failed broods

We evaluated resource selection and movement of 80 broods ≤ 35 days old to compare patterns between broods that did and did not survive to 35 days old. These data contained 273 steps from 29 failed broods, F[0], and 1187 steps from 51 successful broods, F[1]. We removed 21 broods from this analysis because brood fate was unknown due to attending adult mortality, dropped radio-tags, or because we were unable to track broods on restricted private lands.

Our top model suggested brood resource selection differed between broods that failed and broods that succeeded in surviving to 35 days because it performed better than our null model without fate classes (∆WAIC = 2.3; Online Resource Table S2). We did not detect a difference in selection for agricultural cover, idle native grassland cover, tree cover, or daily step lengths between broods that failed and broods that survived to 35 days; however, there were differences in selection for shrub cover and distance to trees (Fig. 5, Online Resource Table S3, Table S5). Broods that failed weakly avoided shrub cover (Sh[0] = − 0.21, 95% CRI = − 0.59, 0.129, f = 0.88) and were less likely to choose habitats with greater percent cover compared to successful broods (0.921% posterior support; Online Resource Table S5), while broods that succeeded selected for shrub cover (Sh[1] = 0.19, 95% CRI = 0.039, 0.32, f = 0.99) and were more likely to use habitat with greater percent cover within a 50 m radius (80.7% posterior support; Fig. 6, Online Resource Table S3, Table S5). Both failed and successful broods avoided areas further from trees than randomly available (Fig. 5, Online Resource Table S3). However, broods that failed were more likely to select areas closer to trees than broods that succeeded (76.4% posterior support; Figs. 5, 6, Online Resource Table S3, Table S5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Split violin plots showing distributions of habitat parameters evaluating selection patterns of northern bobwhite broods in southwest Missouri that failed and broods that survived to 35 days old. Cover type effects for agriculture (Ag), idle native grasslands (Nid), shrub (Sh) and tree (Tr) cover, and distance to nearest tree (TrD) are presented as split violins for failed (left, grey) and successful (right, white) broods [0|1]. Means for failed broods (grey triangle) and successful broods (white triangle) and 95% CRI (bar) are shown in violin plots

Fig. 6
figure 6

Fate-dependent predicted relative probability of use for northern bobwhite broods in southwest Missouri 2016–2018 as a function of a percent shrub cover within 50 m and b distance to nearest tree for successful broods that survived to 35 days old (F[1]; solid line) and broods that failed before 35 days old (F[0]; dot-dash line)

Discussion

Bobwhite broods showed the strongest selection for native grasslands that were both burned and grazed at least once within the previous two years. We also found resource selection patterns changed with age. Young broods avoided idle native grasslands, while older broods selected for those cover types. Selection for shrubs increased with age and older broods selected areas closer to trees compared to young broods. We found variation in these choices had consequences on early-life survival. Selection for woody cover differed between young broods that survived to 35 days and young broods that failed. Broods that survived were more likely to choose areas with greater percent shrub cover and areas farther from trees than broods that failed. We evaluated movement as a behavioral component of resource selection and found daily distance travelled increased with age, but movement patterns did not differ between broods that succeeded and broods that failed to 35 days.

Population-level habitat selection

Coupling grazing and fire on tallgrass prairies and grasslands promote heterogeneity in vegetation structure and composition (Fuhlendorf and Engle 2004; Veen et al. 2008). In Missouri, this practice of patch-burn grazing is implemented with 1/3 of a unit burned (Oct–Mar) and then grazed (Apr–Aug) for 90–120 days. In subsequent years, another third is burned until a given rest year or until the cycle is repeated. This promotes a shifting mosaic that facilitates brood use since at least one-third to one-half of an area has been disturbed in the last 2 years. Broods likely selected for burned and grazed native grasslands because those diverse, disturbed prairie plant communities provided adequate cover, open bare ground, and high invertebrate abundance for improved juvenile foraging, growth, mobility, and survival (Hurst 1972; Taylor et al. 1999; Engle et al. 2008; Doxon and Carroll 2010; Kamps et al. 2017).

We also found broods selected for native grasslands that were burned, but not grazed in the previous two years. Small, patchy prescribed fires remove accumulated litter to create open foraging habitat in close proximity to thicker roosting habitat (Taylor et al. 1999; Kamps et al. 2017). This can reduce brood home range size, which is indicative of improved habitat quality (De Vos and Mueller 1993). We also found weaker selection for native grasslands that were grazed, but not burned in the previous two years. Light to moderate grazing that leaves adequate vegetation height for cover can benefit broods, but fire is an important component of grassland disturbance regimes (Sutter and Ritchison 2005; Harper et al. 2015).

Contrary to our predictions, we found broods selected for both native and mixed grasslands that were mowed or hayed within a breeding season. We expected broods would avoid cut grasses because these practices remove cover, have greater litter if not hayed, and may not improve plant diversity or habitat structure for bobwhite broods (McCoy et al. 2001; Taylor et al. 1999; Gruchy and Harper 2014). Mowing/haying may negatively influence survival through direct mortality of broods or indirectly through increased rates of depredation of young in vegetation that reduces cover (Bollinger et al. 1990). Timing of the management practice may explain differences between predicted and observed effects in our study. Mowing and haying of native grasses typically happened in late summer after July 1 and after peak nesting season, meaning units were not cut for much of the brood season (Harper et al. 2007; Birckhead et al. 2014). Mowing also only changes vegetation structure temporarily. Warm-season grass canopy can grow to heights that provide adequate cover for broods within a few weeks of harvest (Birckhead et al. 2014).

Age-dependent mobility and habitat selection

Non-environmental factors can influence resource selection patterns (Jones 2001). For precocial young that leaves the nest soon after hatching, age strongly influences development and mobility. Individual physical condition can affect resource selection by influencing perceived habitat quality and how an individual moves across a landscape (Merrick and Koprowski 2017). The first 35 days of life are a critical period for success of bobwhite broods and during this time broods may exhibit resource requirements unique to later stages of development (Lusk et al. 2005; Hannon and Martin 2006). We found that bobwhite broods exhibited age-specific differences in movement and habitat selection.

Age influenced daily distances travelled by broods. We found the radius of habitat available to broods older than 35 days increased by just over 30% within a 114-day development period. Taylor and Guthery (1994) reported daily home range size and minimum daily distances traveled nearly doubled from pre-fledging to post-fledging brood periods (0.7 ha and 277 m to 1.4 ha and 589 m, respectively). Post-fledging broods may travel greater distances and may be better able to explore within their daily home ranges than pre-fledging broods. Age can also influence the relative quality of different habitats as resource needs and risk perception change with development. Small flightless young survive to stages where they can thermoregulate, flush from predators, modify foraging behavior and diet, and potentially better use new habitats. Age and mobility thereby interact with resource selection to influence selective pressures, such as body condition, predation, and exposure (Merrick and Koprowski 2017).

All age classes of bobwhite broods in our study were selected for row crop and idle agricultural fields. We expected young broods to avoid row crops due to poor insect prey abundance or availability (Taylor and Burger 2000). Foraging rates and growth of juvenile bobwhite are low within soybean, corn, and wheat fields compared to field borders and native grasslands, suggesting cropland provides suboptimal foraging habitat (Puckett et al. 1995; Palmer et al. 2001; Doxon and Carroll 2010; Lohr et al. 2011). However, grassland vegetation strips within strip crop units or along field borders may provide adjacent foraging habitat and pesticide restrictions on public lands resulting in weedy fields may provide sufficient insect prey (Doxon and Carroll 2007). Selection for agriculture may also be related to risk avoidance (Dzialak et al. 2012). In crop fields, broods can move easily along the ground, sight approaching ground predators, and have adequate canopy height for cover from aerial predators. While agricultural areas may provide marginal foraging habitat, predator avoidance and improved juvenile survival may be a fitness benefit explaining selection patterns (Palmer et al. 2001; Sinnott 2020).

Bobwhite broods are less likely to occupy fields with dense grass and litter cover (Taylor et al.1999; Osborne et al. 2012). We hypothesized thick grass and litter cover would be more limiting to small, flightless juveniles compared to larger, more mobile broods. We found young broods did not select for idle native grasslands, while older broods did select for those grasslands. Size is an important factor mediating age-dependent habitat utility. With growth, older bobwhite broods were better able to move, forage, and utilize thick idle native grasslands (Doxon and Carroll 2010).

Woody vegetation composition and structure are important components of brood habitat for this shrub-obligate species (Martin et al. 2009; Unger et al. 2015). We found young broods did not select for shrub cover, but young broods that survived to 35 days old and broods > 35 days old did select for shrub cover. Shrubs provide protection from warm daytime temperatures and escape cover from predators (Carroll et al. 2015). We also found both younger broods and older broods selected areas closer to trees than randomly available, but, broods > 35 days old selected areas closer to trees compared to broods ≤ 14 days old. Young broods may avoid areas closer to trees than older broods because they cannot yet flush from predators; predator avoidance strategies may depend on age-specific behavioral responses to threats (Dinkins et al. 2012; De Cesare et al. 2014; Perkins et al. 2014).

Fitness consequences of resource selection behavior

While preferred habitats are often assumed to provide fitness benefits, ecological or perceptual traps may result in selection patterns of individuals or populations that do not optimize important demographic rates (Patten and Kelly 2010; Dzialak et al. 2011; De Cesare et al. 2014; Merrick and Koprowski 2017). Few studies link resource selection behaviors with demographic responses by examining survival consequences of variation in choices among individuals (Jones 2001; Bloom et al. 2013; De Cesare et al. 2014; Merrick and Koprowski 2017). Recent studies have found vegetation composition, management, and habitat suitability can influence survival of bobwhite young (Kamps et al. 2017; Tanner et al. 2019; Sinnott 2020). However, these studies have not addressed demographic consequences of individual-level variation in habitat selection behavior. We found daily step lengths of bobwhite broods that failed were similar to movement choices made by broods that survived to 35 days. Other studies have reported larger movements early in the brood period and greater dispersal distances in fragmented landscapes reduce brood survival (Rotella and Rati 1992; Hannon and Martin 2006). Alternatively, larger brood movements from nest sites may result in better habitat for greater brood survival (Paasivaara and Pöysä 2008). While daily movements were not significantly different among successful and failed broods in our study, more work is needed linking habitat-specific movement and dispersal patterns to bobwhite brood survival.

Selection for agricultural row crop, idle native grassland, and tree cover was similar for broods that succeeded and broods that failed to 35 days. However, selection for woody cover differed. Successful broods chose habitats with more shrub cover, while failed broods weakly avoided those areas. Attending adults choosing areas with more shrub cover are providing broods taller vegetation for greater visual obstruction from aerial predators (Hannon and Martin 2006; Collins et al. 2009; Perkins et al. 2014). Taller vegetation can also create fine-scale thermal refuges during summer daytime peak temperatures; failure to choose more moderate microclimates under shrubby vegetation may lead to thermal stress for young (Carroll et al. 2015). While all young broods selected areas closer to trees than randomly available, successful broods selected areas farther from trees than failed broods, suggesting bobwhite mortality risk is greater for those using mature wooded habitat (Lohr et al. 2011).

Conclusion

Three questions related to patterns of resource selection and movement decisions can help us more robustly understand animal distributions: (1) which habitats are animals choosing? (2) what influences choice? (3) what are the fitness consequences of variation in choice? Patterns of resource selection and movement contain important information about habitat valuation and risk aversion, and the extent to which these are driven by age, season and landscape composition to explain fitness outcomes. We found shrub cover and native grasslands managed with grazing and fire were especially valuable for success of young broods. We encourage researchers to examine hypothesized drivers of successful and failed strategies in animals for deeper learning about how fitness cascades to demographic outcomes.