Introduction

Due to the costly nature of predation risk, there are considerable advantages for prey to gain information about potential predators before they pose a significant threat (Lima and Dill 1990). As such, the ability of prey to evaluate future predation risk should be highly beneficial (Mathis et al. 2008). Embryonic learning represents a behavioral carry-over effect, whereby embryos exposed to environmental cues have the opportunity to learn about the identity of predators that will pose a threat for them in the next life stages (Marshall et al. 2003; Ferrari and Chivers 2010; Nelson et al. 2013; Polo-Cavia and Gomez-Mestre 2014). In some systems, this latent learning has been shown to be as sophisticated as that of post-embryonic prey (Ferrari and Chivers 2009c; Mitchell and McCormick 2013). For instance, wood frog tadpoles can modulate the intensity and timing of their antipredator responses to a predator threat based on information learned as embryos (Ferrari and Chivers 2009a). Prey that can use early information to display appropriate antipredator responses upon predator encounters later in life should benefit from similar survival benefits than those provided by non-latent learning (Mathis et al. 2008; Ferrari and Chivers 2013).

Embryonic learning requires exposure to novel predator cues coupled with accurate risk assessment of that predator (Ferrari et al. 2010b). For many organisms, chemicals are often the main source of information available to embryonic individuals (Hay 2009; Ferrari et al. 2010b). The cues used by those embryonic species to learn usually encompass the chemical signatures from predators (predator odors) and cues from injured conspecifics (Chivers et al. 1996; Brown and Smith 1998; McCarthy and Fisher 2000; Jacobsen and Stabell 2004). For a wide variety of aquatic species, this latent associative learning can result in immediate, overt antipredator responses when the threat is detected in later life stages (Mitchell and McCormick 2013; Nelson et al. 2013; Atherton and McCormick 2015). For instance, Polo-Cavia and Gomez-Mestre (2014) demonstrated this when larval western spadefoot toads (Spea hammondii) were able to recognize novel red swamp crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) predators after embryonic conditioning only if predator cues were paired with injured conspecific cues. This learning can also serve to inform the prey about novel, but closely related predators via a process of generalization (Ferrari and Chivers 2009b) and can help prey identify temporal patterns of risk to improve allocation of foraging and antipredator efforts (Ferrari et al. 2010a).

In addition to latent behaviors, developmental carry-over effects may reduce predation risk by altering growth, developmental rates and timing of life history transitions in subsequent life stages (Sih and Moore 1993; Warkentin 1995; Relyea 2001; Pechenik 2006; Tarvin et al. 2015). For example, Chivers et al. (1996) showed that exposure to the predators of damselfly larvae led damselfly embryos to delay hatching, leading them to hatch at large sizes, which increased their chances of survival with larval predators. Developmental carry-over effects can, therefore, have significant impacts on the functional development of individuals (Van Buskirk and Schmidt 2000, Warkentin 2007). While both behavioral and developmental carryover effects have been investigated, very few studies have looked at the interplay between the two. We know that, for a given life stage, prey can benefit from both behavioral and morphological defenses, with very few experiments investigating the integration of the two defense types (but see DeWitt 1998). Whether embryos have the ability to integrate that level of complexity so early is unknown. However, early life exposure might represent the optimal way to induce the development of specific traits that would be beneficial in the current set of conditions (Ferrari et al. 2015).

Carry-over effects may be an important mechanism for invasive species experiencing novel stressors. It is currently unknown if carry-over can influence invasion success or if invaders are capable of embryonic learning or adopting developmental carry-over effects to preemptively evaluate and respond to novel predators (Sih et al. 2010; Garcia et al. 2012; Pintor and Byers 2015). Here, we tested if the American bullfrog (Lithobates catesbeianus), a highly successful invasive species, was capable of carrying over experience with predators across life history stages. Bullfrogs exhibit complex life histories and behaviors and thus are an excellent candidate species to test for carry-over effects (Hayes and Jennings 1986). This large anuran has successfully established invasion ranges across four continents (Lever 2003) and is a serious conservation threat to native communities (Lowe et al. 2000; Pearl et al. 2004). Previous research found that bullfrogs in the northwestern US invasion range perceive largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides, as a novel threat (Garcia et al. 2012). Invasive bullfrogs collected from habitats with no resident largemouth bass populations were unable to respond to largemouth bass chemical cues with appropriate anti-predator behaviors. This is interesting as both bullfrog and largemouth bass native and invasive ranges notably overlap in the US (Garcia et al. 2012). The lack of an innate response to a historic predator suggests that bullfrogs may rely more on flexible antipredator strategies rather than a fixed behavioral response. As such, learning about resident fish predators during the embryonic stage may have significant advantages for bullfrogs in newly invaded habitats.

In this study, we tested the ability of American bullfrogs to display latent learning and developmental carry-over effects based on embryonic experience with various environmental cues using a factorial design. We conditioned embryos to one of three conditioning treatments: (1) exposure to a novel predator odor alone (largemouth bass), (2) exposure to injured conspecific cues paired with predator odor or (3) exposure to a water control. As larvae, we exposed them to one of four testing cues: (1) predator odor alone, (2) injured bullfrog cues alone, (3) a combination of injured bullfrog cues and predator odor or (4) a water control. We recorded their behavioral response to the cues (refuge use) along with morphological measurements to quantify differences in growth rate. Based on previous work on embryonic learning (Mathis et al. 2008), we predicted that larval bullfrogs conditioned as embryos to predator cues paired with injured conspecific cues would be capable of responding to the predators with increased refuge use, while those embryos not receiving the injured conspecific cues treatment would not. We predicted developmental carry-over effects would be a function of injured conspecific cue exposure alone or in combination with predator cues, and would be independent from any latent behaviors.

Materials and methods

Animal collection

We collected five freshly laid bullfrog egg masses (>24 h after deposition) from a seasonal pond at the William L. Finley National Wildlife Refuge near Corvallis, Oregon. The pond contained no resident fish populations. All embryos were transported to Oregon State University and held in a controlled-environment chamber at 15 °C and on a natural photoperiod. Using a split-clutch design, egg masses were divided and mixed, with 75 total eggs from three or more egg masses added to 5-L glass aquaria (N = 9). Aquaria were aerated and filled with 3 L of filtered, dechlorinated tap water. Conditions were standardized for all aquaria for the entirety of their embryonic period.

Cue preparation

Aquaria were randomly assigned to one of three embryonic conditioning treatments: control, largemouth bass chemical odor (fish cues), and injured conspecific cues paired with largemouth bass chemical odor (tadpole + fish cues). Fish were collected and held for 7 days prior to the initiation of the experiment and fed a combination of live crickets and worms. To generate the fish cues, we temporarily held one adult largemouth bass (30 cm total length) in a 19-L bucket of filtered, dechlorinated tap water for 45 min. To generate tadpole cues, one large bullfrog tadpole was emulsified, daily, in 100 ml of control water before each conditioning (10.7 cm ± 0.8 cm SD large tadpole total length). All cues were made immediately before being administered each day.

Embryonic conditioning

Embryos were exposed to conditioning treatments for two consecutive days (27–28 May 2014) with cues administered at 1300 hours each day. In all aquaria, 1 L of tank water was removed and replaced with treatment water. “Control” tanks received 1 L of filtered, dechlorinated tap water; “fish cue” tanks received 1 L fish cue water, and “tadpole + fish cues” tanks received 5 mL of the tadpole emulsion and 995 mL of fish cue water. Cue exposure stopped (29-May-2014) when hatching was imminent, indicated by embryonic tail straightening (developmental Gosner stage 18; Gosner 1960). Three embryos from each tank were collected daily during conditioning and at hatching, to assess development and growth rates over the conditioning period.

Larval rearing

Conditioning treatment groups and environmental conditions were maintained after hatching; all individuals were transferred to 9 30-L HDPE tubs containing 20 L of filtered, dechlorinated tap water. Larvae were fed algal pellets and a rabbit chow/fish flake mix (3:1) ad libitum with partial water changes performed every 5 days. Temperatures were raised to 18 °C on 30-June-2014 to reflect seasonal conditions.

Larval exposure trials

Individuals were tested for predator recognition 90 days after hatching. Following the 3 × 4 design, every treatment combination was replicated 8 times, for a total of 96 replicates. Embryonic conditioning treatments included exposure to a novel predator odor alone (largemouth bass), exposure to injured conspecific cues paired with predator odor or exposure to a water control. Larval exposure treatments included predator odor alone, injured bullfrog cues alone, a combination of injured bullfrog cues and predator odor or a water control. Experimental units (19 × 32 cm clear plastic tubs) contained one refuge consisting of an 8 × 6 cm piece of corrugated black plastic. Placement of the refuge rotated clockwise around the four corners of the unit across all 96 replicates. All units contained 1 L of control water (filtered and dechlorinated tap water) and 1 L of the randomly assigned larval exposure treatment water. Individual tadpoles were randomly selected from each embryonic conditioning treatment group, added to units, and given a 30-min acclimation period before behavioral observations began. Behavioral spot-checks and recording of refuge use were conducted every 20 min for a total of 12 observations per unit beginning at 1100 hours Each instance of refuge use across all 12 observations was recorded for all individuals. Observers were blind to assigned treatment combinations, and all experimental units were located behind observation blinds to limit observer disturbance.

Upon termination of the experiment, larvae were measured (total and snout-vent length), weighed, and developmentally staged (Gosner 1960). All individuals were humanely euthanized using MS-222 and preserved in 70% ethanol. All applicable institutional and national guidelines for the care and use of animals were followed.

Statistical analyses

We used a principal component analysis (PCA) to create a composite body size variable from total larval body length, larval snout-vent length, and larval weight. The PCA used a covariance matrix and the scores resulted from the non-rotated solution. The first axis (PC1) explained 91.5% of the variance and loaded mainly on total larval body length and secondarily on snout-vent length. The second axis (PC2) explained 8% of the variance and loaded mainly on snout-vent length. We ran two one-way ANOVAs to test the effect of embryonic conditioning treatment on PC1 and PC2 to inform subsequent analyses on potential interactions between morphological and behavioral responses. We also ran one-way ANOVAs on hatching size (embryonic body length) and developmental stage at hatching to assess direct effect of conditioning treatment.

Variation in individual refuge use was assessed using a generalized linear model with a binomial logit link to accommodate the structure of the response variable. The model was built as a time series of observations with embryonic conditioning and larval exposure as predictor variables and larval length (PC1) as a covariate. All statistical analyses were conducted using R version 3.2.3 (R Core Team 2015) and packages, car and ggplot2 (Wickham 2009; Fox and Weisberg 2011). The datasets supporting these results have been uploaded as part of the supplementary material.

Results

Our one-way ANOVA quantified the effects of embryonic conditioning on larval body size. We found a significant relationship between PC1 (total larval length) and embryonic conditioning (F 2,92 = 4.3, p = 0.016); there was no significant relationship between PC2 (snout-vent length) and embryonic conditioning (F 2,92 = 0.4, p = 0.7). Embryos conditioned to predator odor and bullfrog injury cues grew into longer bodied larvae (8–10% longer) relative to individuals exposed to predator odor only or controls (tad + fish:  = 24.2 mm, SD = 3.8 mm; fish:  = 21.9 mm, SD = 3.4 mm; control:  = 22.4 mm; SD = 2.8 mm). There was no significant difference in larval body length across the predator odor only or control treatments (Fig. 1). All larvae measured at 90 days since hatching were at developmental Gosner stage 25 (Gosner 1960). There was no significant difference in embryo body length or developmental stage during cue administration or at hatching across the three embryonic conditioning treatments (embryo length: F 2,49 = 2.6, p = 0.08; embryo developmental stage F 2,49 = 1.8, p value = 0.17).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Boxplot of 90 days post-hatch larval length (PC1, accounting for 91.5% of the variation in larval total length and snout-vent length) for American bullfrog tadpoles (n = 96) that received water (control), largemouth bass and injured tadpole cues (tadpole + fish) or largemouth bass odor only (fish). Letters indicate significant difference at α = 0.05

We found a significant interaction between embryonic conditioning and larval exposure on mean refuge use (Wald test, X 2 = 23.4, df = 6, p < 0.001; Table 1). A behavioral response was found only when predation risk intensity varied across the embryonic and larval exposure treatments (Fig. 2). For example, individuals conditioned to high predation risk (predator odor with bullfrog injury cues) as embryos showed increased likelihood of refuge use as larvae when exposed to predator odor alone (log odds: 2.245; 95% CI: 0.947–4.109). Further, predator odor conditioned individuals showed increased likelihood of refuge use when exposed to the combination of predator odor with bullfrog injury cues (log odds: 1.612; 95% CI: 0.715–2.655; Fig. 3). Individuals conditioned to either control water or injured bullfrog cues alone did not increase refuge use regardless of exposure cue.

Table 1 Results of a Wald test type-III analysis of variance on effects of embryonic conditioning and larval exposure on refuge use in American bullfrogs (Lithobates catesbeianus)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Log odds of refuge use probability of American bullfrog tadpoles (Lithobates catesbeianus) as result of embryonic conditioning and experimental exposure with 95% confidence intervals (n = 96; a value >0 indicates higher probability of refuge use of treatment compared to control exposure; a value <0 indicates lower probability of refuge use of treatment compared to control exposure). Exposure treatments to one of three variations from the control: (1) predator odor (fish—largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides), (2) injured tadpole cues (tadpole), and (3) a combination of injured tadpole cues and predator odor (tadpole + fish). These individuals received one of three conditioning treatments as embryos: (1) predator cue (fish), (2) a combination of injured bullfrog cues and predator cues (tadpole + fish) and (3) a water control

Fig. 3
figure 3

The observed and predicted refuge use of American bullfrog tadpoles (Lithobates catesbeianus) over time for combinations of embryonic conditioning and experimental exposure treatment (n = 96). Points represent observed data and the gray curve and shaded area represent the model estimated refuge use and associated 95% confidence interval. Exposure treatments consist of the following: (1) predator odor (fish—largemouth bass, Micropterus salmoides), (2) injured tadpole cues (tadpole), (3) a combination of injured tadpole cues and predator odor (tadpole + fish), and (4) control water. These individuals received one of three conditioning treatments as embryos: (1) predator cue (fish), (2) a combination of injured bullfrog cues and predator cues (tadpole + fish) and (3) a water control

Discussion

The degree to which carry-over effects influenced American bullfrog development and behavior was dependent on the intensity and predictability of perceived risk across the embryonic and larval environments. Developmental carry-over effects occurred in individuals conditioned as embryos to high risk scenarios, specifically the combination of predator odor and injured bullfrog cues. These effects occurred regardless of larval exposure regimes (Fig. 4). Behavioral carry-over, however, was found only when embryonic conditioning cues differed from larval exposure cues, with larvae behaving with increased refuge use only when perceiving variability in risk across the embryo/larval transition. In consistently low predation risk scenarios (predator odor alone or controls), we found no evidence for developmental or behavioral defenses. This indicates a complex trade-off between a fixed developmental strategy and a more flexible behavioral strategy in risky environments.

Fig. 4
figure 4

A conceptual model illustrating the relationship between carry-over effects and embryonic/larval predation risk gradients. American bullfrogs (Lithobates catesbeianus) exposed to Largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides) cues display developmental (longer larval lengths) and behavioral (refuge use) carry-over responses, which varied based on the timing, intensity and predictability of the predator cue exposure. High predation risk is representative of the predator odor and bullfrog injury cue treatment (tadpole + fish) and the mid-level predation risk treatment is the predator odor only (fish) treatment. Control and bullfrog injury cue only treatments elicited no behavioral or developmental responses

Predation risk intensity

Bullfrog embryos exposed to chemical cue combinations indicative of a high predation risk environment exhibited a developmental carry-over response. Individuals conditioned to predator odor and bullfrog injury cues as embryos hatched into larva that grew 10% longer compared to individuals conditioned to lower risk environments (predator odor only or control treatments). This was irrespective of larval exposure, resulting in a fixed life history strategy based on environmental conditions experienced only during the embryonic stage (Figs. 1, 4). Remarkably, this body size difference was quantified in individuals that experienced a relatively truncated conditioning period (bullfrog embryos were exposed to conditioning treatments for 2 days prior to hatching). Size differences can be directly attributed to a carry-over effect as no treatment differences in body size or length at hatching were detected.

Predation risk predictability

Uncertainty in predation risk can lead to the selection for flexible rather than fixed antipredator defenses (Roff 1992; Tollrian and Harvell 1999). Only when individuals were exposed to risk during both the embryonic and larval stages did we find evidence for a learned response. In contrast to most embryonic learning studies, this response occurred when predation risk intensity varied across the embryonic and larval exposure treatments. Individuals that were conditioned as embryos to a high risk environment (the combination of predator odor and injured bullfrog cues) exhibited increased refuge use only when exposed as larvae to a lower risk scenario (predator odor only). Inversely, individuals conditioned to predator odor only behaviorally responded as larvae when exposed to high risk environments (the combination of predator odor and injured bullfrog cues; Figs. 2, 4). Predation scenarios with low predictability, or variable risk profiles across life history stages, resulted in a more flexible antipredator strategy.

Fitness consequences

Each of these predator avoidance strategies, developmental and behavioral, have associated trade-offs in terms of energetics, survival, growth, and time until metamorphosis (Roff 1992). Predator-induced plasticity in morphological traits has been documented in multiple aquatic taxa (DeWitt 1998; Tollrian and Harvell 1999; Hettyey et al. 2015), particularly anurans (Smith and Van Buskirk 1995; Van Buskirk 2000; Relyea 2004; McIntyre et al. 2004). The effectiveness of changes to tail morphology can be highly predator specific (Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000; Relyea 2001; Van Buskirk 2002; Wilson et al. 2005) and further quantification of the functional implications of longer body lengths regarding swim speed and escape performance is needed. However, functional tradeoffs likely exist, with induced defenses associated with greater energetic expenditures, reduced development rates and longer time until metamorphosis (Van Buskirk 2000).

Behavioral carry-over effects that influence antipredator behaviors can also be costly for amphibian larvae (Skelly and Werner 1990; Epp and Gabor 2008a; van Allen et al. 2010). Refuge use, in particular, reduces an individual’s time spent foraging, and thus decreases growth and development rates (Wilbur and Collins 1973; Werner 1986; Relyea and Werner 1999). However, hiding can be an extremely effective strategy for minimizing predation risk, providing threat-sensitive protection in unpredictable predation environments. These trade-offs between increased survival in the presence of predators and decreased growth and development rates would result in strong selection for accurate risk assessment across all life history stages (Richardson 2001).

Particular treatment combinations resulted in larvae capable of producing both developmental and behavioral antipredator responses (Fig. 4). Scenarios in which individuals were conditioned to high risk cues as embryos and exposed to low risk cues during the larval stage resulted in individuals exhibiting both longer larval body lengths and greater time spent in refuge. While no statistical interactions between larval length and refuge use response were detected, we predict that individuals would suffer reduced growth and development rates over time as function of increased time spent in refuge and increased costs associated with induced morphological defenses (Benard and Fordyce 2003). Further study is needed to quantify fitness consequences associated with adopting both antipredator strategies simultaneously.

A decrease in antipredator behavior in response to increased frequency and severity of risk, albeit counter-intuitive at first, is in fact an adaptive response. The Risk Allocation Hypothesis (Lima and Bednekoff 1999; Ferrari et al. 2009) posits that prey cannot continuously increase the intensity of their antipredator response to increased risk, since they will eventually reach an unacceptable level of decreased foraging gain. Rather, the model posits that prey may be able to predict the pattern of risk and instead, allocate high antipredator response during periods of time when risk is perceived as high, and high foraging effort during periods when risk is perceived as lower. The model produces counter-intuitive results, as one individual’s high risk situation might be another one’s low risk situation. In our experiment, the high embryonic/high larval risk tadpoles displayed lower apparent level of refuge use. These results fit within the risk allocation framework.

Invading populations experience novel species interactions, abiotic conditions and physiological demands as they expand their range. These stressors can result in failed introductions if individuals are incapable of mitigating these dangers (Williamson et al. 1986; Rodriguez-Cabal et al. 2013). Our results indicate that invasive American bullfrogs are exhibiting nuanced abilities to detect predation risk based on cue exposure intensity and predictability across life history stages. Carrying over the developmental and behavioral effects of early predator exposure appears to improve bullfrog antipredator response and could increase invasion success. In habitats with consistent predation by largemouth bass, we predict that bullfrog larvae will develop longer bodies and tails relative to populations with variation in risk exposure, and perhaps be capable of escaping predators with improved swim speed and escape performance (Van Buskirk and McCollum 2000). In environments with temporal variation in predation events leading to inconsistent cue exposure and unpredictable risk regimes, we expect to see bullfrog larvae adopting behavioral antipredator responses, such as increased refuge use.

Our study suggests that embryonic learning can provision bullfrogs with specific antipredator behaviors. Larval refuge use response was predicated on that individual being conditioned to some degree of risk, be it predator odor alone or in combination with bullfrog injury cues. The lack of observed plasticity in larval bullfrog behavior is unusual as many amphibian larvae have the ability to respond to novel waterborne predator cues (Garcia et al. 2004; Epp and Gabor 2008b; Ferland-Raymond et al. 2010). However, novel species interactions involving invasive populations add complex elements of evolutionary history (Garcia et al. 2012; Pujol-Buxó et al. 2013; Hettyey et al. 2016). The relative benefits of embryonic learning over general behavioral plasticity for an invasive species are unknown and require future study.

Cue habituation may explain some of our behavioral responses. Habituation to predator stimuli is often highly selective (Blumstein 2016; Hemmi and Merkle 2009), and thus we would expect to see a reduction in refuge use after being conditioned to largemouth bass cue. While we did see minimal refuge use in treatments that exposed individuals to a consistent cue regime, individuals increased refuge use when exposed to largemouth bass cue combined with bullfrog injury cues. Similarly, conditioning to both predator odor and bullfrog injury cues resulted in increased refuge when larvae were exposed to predator cues alone. As such, we posit that embryos may have been desensitized to their conditioning cues, thus resulting in the lack of response to the same cue exposure in the larval stage. Altered cue regimes (addition or removal of tadpole injury cues) may have provided a level of novelty necessary to trigger the observed antipredator response (Winandy and Denoël 2013).

Invasive species are an excellent group in which to study carry-over effects and we would benefit from a stronger understanding of how embryonic learning and developmental carry-over impact species invasions. Invaders capable of accurately assessing risk within and across appropriate life stages can result in enhanced survivorship, increased establishment rates, and invasion success. Further, comparison of native and invasive populations of our most prolific invaders in their ability to learn and respond to novel predators would increase our awareness of population divergence in antipredator strategies. We posit that future invasion studies, and reviews of past research, must consider the influence of stress conditioning in early life history stages. Instead of asking if the chicken or the egg came first, we should be asking if, in fact, the egg knows more than we think it does.

Author contribution statement

TG and MF conceived and designed the study. TG, JU and EB performed data collection. TG and EB developed the statistical analysis. TG, JU, EB and MF contributed to the writing of the manuscript.