Introduction

The human brain is a complex network of interacting regions connected by white matter tracts. The characterization of structural and functional features of such a network in healthy subjects and diseased people has the potential to improve our understanding of the pathophysiology and clinical manifestations of many neurological and psychiatric conditions (Filippi et al. 2013). This has led to the use of new tools for complex system analysis to tackle brain diseases (Bullmore and Sporns 2009). Among these, graph theory is a mathematical framework which allows to describe a network as a graph, consisting of a collection of nodes (i.e., brain regions) and edges (i.e., structural and functional connections) (Bullmore and Sporns 2009; Rubinov and Sporns 2010). Using graph theory, distinct modifications of brain network topology have been identified during development and normal ageing, and disrupted functional and structural connectivities have been associated with several neurological and psychiatric conditions, including dementia, amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, and schizophrenia (Filippi et al. 2013). In the latter, connectomic approaches have contributed to prove the theory of this condition as a disconnection syndrome (Filippi et al. 2013).

In multiple sclerosis (MS), the occurrence of disconnection has been substantiated by structural MRI studies of brain network topology (He et al. 2009; Li et al. 2013; Shu et al. 2011), which showed a decreased structural connectivity of regions of the fronto-temporal lobes. At present, functional abnormalities of brain network topology have been investigated only marginally in this condition (Schoonheim et al. 2011).

By applying graph analysis to resting state (RS) fMRI data, we investigated the topological organization of the functional brain connectome in a large cohort of MS patients. Our working hypothesis was that the disruption of brain network functional connectivity at a global and regional level is likely to contribute to the clinical manifestations of the disease, especially cognitive impairment.

Materials and methods

Subjects

We recruited consecutively 246 right-handed MS patients (Polman et al. 2011) (85/161 men/women, mean age = 42.3 years, range = 19–60 years). One hundred and twenty-one patients (44/77 men/women) had relapsing remitting (RR) MS (Lublin and Reingold 1996), 45 (10/35 men/women) benign MS (BMS) [Expanded Disability Status Scale (EDSS) score ≤3.0 after a disease duration of at least 15 years] (Hawkins and McDonnell 1999) and 80 (31/49 men/women) secondary progressive (SP) MS (Lublin and Reingold 1996). To be included, patients had to: (1) be relapse- and steroid free for at least 3 months before the fMRI experiment; (2) have no significant medical illnesses or substance abuse that could interfere with cognitive functioning; and (3) have no other major systemic, psychiatric or neurological diseases.

All patients underwent a complete neurologic examination within 2 days of the MRI study, with rating of the EDSS score. As a measure of cognitive impairment, we used the paced auditory serial addition test (PASAT), which is sensitive to abnormalities of working memory and information processing speed. This test is accepted as the reference test for cognitive assessment as part of the MA functional composite score in MS (Fischer et al. 1999). Patients were considered cognitively impaired (CI) when PASAT score was 2SD below the average score of a comparable control group for age, gender and education (Amato et al. 2006).

Fifty-five right-handed, age- and gender-matched healthy controls (HC) (19/36 men/women, mean age = 41.7 years, range = 20–60 years) with no previous history of neurological, psychiatric, or medical disorders, and a normal neurological exam were also enrolled. Table 1 summarizes the main demographic, clinical and conventional MRI characteristics of the different study groups.

Table 1 Main demographic, clinical and conventional MRI characteristics of healthy subjects and patients with MS

Ethics committee approval

Approval was received from the local ethical standards committee on human experimentation, and written informed consent was obtained from all subjects prior to enrolment.

MRI acquisition

Using a 3.0 T Philips Intera scanner, the following sequences of the brain were acquired from all subjects during a single session: (a) T2*-weighted single-shot echo planar imaging (EPI) sequence for RS fMRI [repetition time (TR) = 3,000 ms, echo time (TE) = 35 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view (FOV) = 240 mm2; matrix = 128 × 128, slice thickness = 4 mm, 200 sets of 30 contiguous axial slices, parallel to the AC-PC plane]. Positioning of RS fMRI scans was carefully performed to include the entire cerebellum/pons region; (b) dual-echo turbo spin echo (TSE) [TR/TE = 3,500/24–120 ms; flip angle = 150°; FOV = 240 mm2; matrix = 256 × 256; echo train length (ETL) = 5; 44 contiguous, 3-mm-thick axial slices]; and (c) 3D T1-weighted fast field echo (TR = 25 ms, TE = 4.6 ms, flip angle = 30°, FOV = 230 mm2, matrix = 256 × 256, slice thickness = 1 mm, 220 contiguous axial slices, in-plane resolution = 0.89 × 0.89 mm2). Total acquisition time of RS fMRI was about 10 min. During scanning, subjects were instructed to remain motionless and not to think anything in particular. All subjects reported that they had not fallen asleep during scanning, according to a questionnaire delivered immediately after the MRI session.

Conventional MRI analysis

T2 hyperintense lesion volume (LV) was quantified by one experienced observer, unaware of patient’s identity, using a local thresholding segmentation technique (Jim 5, Xinapse Systems Ltd., Northants, UK). Normalized brain volume (NBV) was calculated from 3D T1-weighted images (Smith 2002).

RS fMRI data pre-processing

All fMRI analyses were performed by an experienced observer, unaware of subject’s identity, Using Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM8), RS fMRI images were realigned to the mean of each session with a six degree rigid-body transformation to correct for minor head movements. The mean cumulative translations were 0.11 mm (SD 0.22 mm) for HC and 0.13 mm (SD 0.24 mm) for MS patients (p = 0.3). Mean translations were 0.13 mm (SD 0.24) for CP and 0.12 mm (SD 0.24) for CI MS patients (p = 0.8). The mean rotations were <0.1° in all groups (p = 0.3). Data were normalized to the SPM8 default EPI template using a standard affine followed by a non-linear transformation (Ashburner and Friston 1999), and band-pass filtered between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz using the REST software (http://resting-fmri.sourceforge.net/) to partially remove low-frequency drifts and physiological high-frequency noise. Using REST, non-neuronal sources of synchrony between RS fMRI time series and motion-related artifacts were minimized by regressing out the six motion parameters estimated by SPM8 (Lund et al. 2005), and the average signals of the ventricular cerebrospinal fluid and white matter. The removal of the average signal from the cerebrospinal fluid, which is highly correlated with physiological noise, is likely to improve dramatically the reliability of the RS fMRI measurements, especially from cerebellar regions (Diedrichsen et al. 2010). No spatial smoothing was applied, to avoid spurious correlations between neighbouring voxels, as previously suggested (Sanz-Arigita et al. 2010).

Construction of functional brain networks

To construct functional brain networks, we employed an automated anatomical labelling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. 2002) to parcel the brain into 116 cortical and subcortical regions of interest (ROI). Time series were extracted from each ROI by averaging the signal from all voxels within each region. Bivariate correlations between each ROI pair were obtained by calculating the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between ROI time courses. These correlation coefficients represent functional connectivity strengths between brain regions. Correlation matrices obtained from all study subjects were thresholded into binary connectivity matrices at different correlation thresholds (τ, resulting in unweighted graphs with the nodes representing brain regions and edges/links representing functional relationships between brain regions) (Rubinov and Sporns 2010). The number of connections surviving at a given correlation threshold can vary between subjects: in other words, even when fixing τ graphs from different subjects might have a different number of significant links. Therefore, previous studies did not use a fixed τ for all study subjects, but instead defined the so-called network sparsity, essentially meaning that they forced the total number of existing connections to be the same for all study subjects (de Haan et al. 2009; He et al. 2008; Tian et al. 2011; Yao et al. 2010). Such an approach may lead to inaccurate results, since fixing a pre-defined sparsity may result in a modification of network topology (van Wijk et al. 2010). For instance, in networks with low average connectivity, a consistent number of not significant correlation values might be converted into significant connections to achieve the imposed network degree; in contrast, in networks with high average connectivity, a large number of significant correlations might be ignored (van Wijk et al. 2010). Therefore, as performed in previous studies (Meunier et al. 2009; Salvador et al. 2005; van den Heuvel et al. 2009), we chose to construct our graphs by fixing the same τ for all study subjects. Because there is no definitive method for choosing τ we examined several possible network configurations for a range of τ values ranging from 0 to 0.9 (excluding negative correlations), and explored the consistency of results over this range (Sanz-Arigita et al. 2010). Then, we explored network characteristics only over the range of thresholds that yielded not disconnected graphs (0 ≤ τ ≤ 0.20, with increments of 0.01). Table 2 reports the number of subjects showing graph disconnectivity as a function of increasing correlation threshold τ. For MS patients, the corresponding median EDSS score is provided. The range of selected correlation thresholds is close to values which were demonstrated to provide the best trade-off between a low positive false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995) and false non-discovery rate for functional connectivity matrices (Sala et al. 2014).

Table 2 Number of subjects showing graph disconnectivity as a function of increasing correlation threshold τ

Network analysis

Global and regional network properties were explored using the Brain Connectivity Matlab toolbox (http://www.brain-connectivity-toolbox.net) (Rubinov and Sporns 2010).

Global network analysis Six global network metrics, including two small-world measures [i.e., clustering coefficient (C) and characteristic path length (L)] (Watts and Strogatz 1998), mean network degree, global efficiency (Latora and Marchiori 2001), assortativity, and hierarchy (Bassett et al. 2008) were explored. The clustering coefficient (C) of a node is the number of links existing between its nearest neighbours and represents the fraction of the node’s neighbours that are also neighbours of each other (Watts and Strogatz 1998). In turn, the mean network C represents how strongly a given network is locally interconnected. The characteristic path length (L) is defined as the average shortest path length between all pairs of nodes in the same network (Watts and Strogatz 1998). Since L represents the average number of nodes that have to be crossed to go from one node to any other node, it is taken as a measure of functional integration. The degree (k) of a single node is the number of links connected to that node. The mean network degree is the average degree of all network nodes, and is a measure of network density (Rubinov and Sporns 2010). Global efficiency is the average inverse shortest path length (Latora and Marchiori 2001) and is a measure of the overall information transfer efficiency across the whole network. Assortativity, also known as degree correlation (Newman 2002), is a measure of correlation between nodal degree and mean degree of its nearest neighbours, obtained by averaging the correlation coefficients of the degrees of every connected node pair (Newman 2002). Positive assortativity values indicate that nodes are likely to be connected to other nodes with the same degree and, therefore, that high-degree nodes or hubs of the network are likely connected to each other. Finally, the hierarchical structure of our networks was quantified by the β coefficient, which is a parameter of the power-law relationship between C and k of the nodes in the network (Ravasz and Barabasi 2003): C = k −α. We estimated α by fitting a linear regression line to the plot of log(C) versus log(k) for the network.

Networks having small-worldness properties were defined as those significantly more clustered than random networks, but having approximately the same characteristic path length. To assess small-worldness, we computed the normalized clustering coefficient γ = C real/C rand, and the normalized characteristic path length λ = L real/L rand (Watts and Strogatz 1998), where C real and L real were clustering coefficients and characteristic path lengths of the average brain networks of each study group, and C rand and L rand represented the corresponding indices calculated on 100 matched random networks preserving the same numbers of nodes, edges, and degree distribution as the observed average network (Maslov and Sneppen 2002). By definition, a small-world network has γ > 1 and λ ≈ 1 (Watts and Strogatz 1998). These two measurements can be summarized into a simple quantitative metric, small-worldness, σ = γ/λ > 1 (Humphries et al. 2006).

Hubs and regional network analysis Hubs were identified on the basis of centrality in the network. We used two metrics of nodal centrality: nodal degree and betweenness centrality. Degree, the number of links connected to a node, is one of the most common measures of centrality. Betweenness centrality is the fraction of all shortest paths in the network that pass through a given node (Rubinov and Sporns 2010); therefore, bridging nodes, that connect disparate parts of a network, result in a high betweenness centrality. Degree and betweenness centrality were calculated for every threshold (τ) that yielded not disconnected graphs (i.e., between 0 and 0.20). Then, the two nodal parameters X nod (degree and betweenness centrality) were obtained by integrating over all considered thresholds (Tian et al. 2011). A brain region was defined as a hub when X nod of any of its two nodal metrics was at least 1 SD higher than the average of the corresponding parameter over the entire network (Tian et al. 2011).

Sample size calculation

The main aim of this study is the comparison of network measures (i.e., degree, clustering coefficient, path length, efficiency, assortativity and hierarchy) between healthy controls (N = 55) and MS patients (N = 246). Assuming a type I error (alpha) of 0.05, our study is powered at 90 % to detect a statistical significant difference for each network measure, equal between groups to a Cohen-standardized effect size of 0.5.

Statistical analysis

Brain T2 lesion volumes (LV) were log-transformed before statistical analysis due to their skewed distribution. Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to verify Gaussianity of distribution of demographic, clinical, conventional MRI and network metrics. Variables following a Gaussian distribution (i.e., age, disease duration, log T2 LV, NBV and all global network measures) were compared between groups using two-sample t tests or ANOVA models, as appropriate. Categorical variables (i.e., EDSS, the presence/absence of cognitive impairment and regional network measures) were compared between groups using the Mann–Whitney and the Kruskal–Wallis tests, as appropriate. For all these measures, the following between-group comparisons were decided a priori: (1) HC vs MS patients (as a whole), (2) HC vs CP and CI patients, and (3) HCs vs RRMS, RRMS vs SPMS, RRMS vs BMS, and SPMS vs BMS (decided on the basis of the clinical evolution of the disease).

ANOVA models, adjusted for age and gender, were used to compare global network properties between groups. The analysis was repeated by correcting p values for multiple comparisons over the different correlation thresholds τ with the FDR approach (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995). To investigate the effect of gender, network metrics were also compared between male/female HCs and male/female MS patients using 2 × 2 ANOVA models adjusted for age.

A Mann–Whitney test was used to compare integrated nodal parameters (X nod) of degree and betweenness centrality from all 116 automated anatomical labelling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al. 2002) regions between study groups. Correction for multiple comparisons was performed by applying a modified version of FDR, called positive FDR (pFDR) (Storey 2003). While the traditional FDR approach involves a sequential p value rejection method based on the observed data (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995), by fixing the error rate and estimating the corresponding region of rejection, the pFDR method applies the opposite strategy, i.e., a rejection region is fixed and the corresponding error rate is estimated (Storey 2003). This approach, statistically more powerful than FDR (Storey 2003), allows to control for multiple comparisons and ranks brain regions according to their importance in explaining between-group differences. Ranked significance is measured by the q value, the pFDR analogue of the p value (Storey 2003). We considered statistically significant only brain regions associated with an error rate smaller than 0.1 % (i.e., q ≤ 0.001).

The relationships of global and regional network properties with conventional MRI measures were tested using linear regression models with age and gender as confounding covariates.

Results

Clinical and conventional MRI measures

Thirty-four percent of MS patients were CI. Compared to cognitively preserved (CP), CI MS patients were significantly older, had longer disease duration, higher EDSS and T2 LV, and lower NBV. The proportion of CI patients was higher in SPMS than RRMS (50 vs 23 %, p < 0.001), while it did not differ between BMS and RRMS (37 vs 23 %, p = 0.34). Disease duration was longer in BMS vs SPMS (p = 0.004) and RRMS (p < 0.001) patients, and SPMS vs RRMS (p < 0.001) patients. T2 LV was significantly higher in SPMS and BMS vs RRMS patients (p < 0.001 and 0.001). NBV was significantly lower in SPMS vs RRMS (p < 0.001) and BMS (p = 0.03) and in BMS vs RRMS patients (p = 0.009).

Global network analysis

Small-worldness was verified in HCs and MS patients (as a whole and in the different study subgroups) (Table 3). The majority of graph theoretical metrics were significantly abnormal in MS patients compared to HCs (Fig. 1; Table 4), especially at low correlation thresholds. Mean network degree, global efficiency, and hierarchy were lower (p values ranging from 0.01 to 0.05), and path length and assortativity higher (p values ranging from <0.001 to 0.05) in MS patients (Fig. 1; Table 4). No effect of gender was detected.

Table 3 Small-worldness metrics γ, λ and σ in healthy controls and MS patients over the range of correlation thresholds examined
Fig. 1
figure 1

Plots of global network properties in healthy controls (black line) and patients with MS (black dashed line) over the selected range of correlation thresholds τ: a mean network degree; b clustering coefficient; c characteristic path length; d global efficiency; e assortativity; and f hierarchy. See text for further details. Shade areas indicate the range of thresholds where between-group differences were statistically significant

Table 4 Average values and standard deviations (SD) of global network parameters in healthy controls (HC) and patients with MS as a whole and in the different study subgroups

Regional network analysis

Hub regions

Table 5 and Fig. 2 report hubs in HCs and MS patients. The bilateral cerebellum (crus I), left cerebellum (crus II), right anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), right orbitofrontal cortex, bilateral inferior and middle temporal gyrus, and bilateral middle cingulate cortex were hubs in both groups. The left precuneus, superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and ACC were hubs in HCs, only. The left superior temporal pole and cerebellar lobule IV–V were hubs in MS patients, only. Basal ganglia hubs were mostly located in the left hemisphere in HCs, and in the right hemisphere in MS patients.

Table 5 Brain hubs in healthy controls and patients with MS, as a whole and according to the presence/absence of cognitive impairment
Fig. 2
figure 2

Brain hubs (a left hemisphere, b right hemisphere) of the functional networks of healthy controls (HCs) and patients with MS as a whole and according to the presence/absence of cognitive impairment. Hubs were identified as brain regions having either integrated nodal degree or betweenness centrality one standard deviation greater than the network average. Hubs present in HC only are reported in red, hubs present in MS patients only are reported in blue, and hubs present in all groups are reported in black. CP cognitively preserved, CI cognitively impaired, ACC anterior cingulate cortex, Caud caudate nucleus, Cereb cerebellum, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, Ling lingual gyrus, MCC middle cingulate cortex, MTG middle temporal gyrus, OFC orbitofrontal cortex, Pall pallidus, Precun precuneus, Put putamen, SFG superior frontal gyrus, Sup TP superior temporal pole, Thal thalamus

Regional nodal characteristics

Compared with controls, MS patients showed a significantly lower nodal degree in the bilateral caudate nucleus and right cerebellum (crus I) (Table 6). Betweenness centrality did not differ between MS patients and HCs.

Table 6 Results of the comparison of nodal degree between healthy controls and patients with MS, as a whole and according to the presence/absence of cognitive impairment

Network analysis and cognitive impairment

Global network analysis

Compared to HCs and CP patients, CI MS patients had lower mean network degree, global efficiency and hierarchy (p values ranging from 0.002 to 0.02) and higher path length (p values ranging from 0.002 to 0.01) at several correlation thresholds (Fig. 3; Table 4). No differences were found between CP MS patients and HCs.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Plots of the global network properties significantly different between healthy controls (black line), cognitively preserved (CP) patients with MS (continuous grey line) and cognitively impaired (CI) MS patients (dashed grey lines) over the selected range of correlation thresholds τ: a mean network degree; b characteristic path length; c global efficiency; and d hierarchy. Shade areas indicate the range of thresholds where between-group differences were statistically significant

Regional network analysis

Table 5 and Fig. 2 report hubs in CP and CI MS patients. Hubs in CP MS patients were similar to those described for the whole group of MS patients except for the presence of additional hubs in the lobule VIII of the right cerebellum, the left orbitofrontal cortex and left caudate nucleus. A preservation of the thalamic and ACC hubs in the left hemisphere was also detected. Differently from HCs and CP patients, CI MS patients did not have hubs in the left frontal cortex. They also showed no thalamic hubs (Table 5; Fig. 2).

Compared to HCs, both CP and CI MS patients showed significantly lower nodal degree in the bilateral caudate nucleus. CI MS patients also showed a decreased nodal degree in the right cerebellum (crus I and lobule VIII), left ACC, left SFG, left precuneus and left thalamus (Table 6). No differences were found between CP and CI MS patients. Betweenness centrality did not differ between groups.

Network analysis and MS clinical phenotype

Global network analysis

At low correlation thresholds, mean network degree, global efficiency, path length, and assortativity were significantly different between MS phenotypes (p values ranging from <0.001 to 0.05; Table 4). At post hoc analysis, no significant differences were found between MS phenotypes.

Regional network analysis

Figure 4 and Table 7 report hubs in the different MS clinical phenotypes. Hubs in HCs are also reported for clarity. The left superior temporal pole and right basal ganglia were hubs in all MS clinical phenotypes. In addition, RRMS patients had a hub distribution similar to that of HCs, apart from the loss of hubs in the left basal ganglia. BMS patients had an increased number of hubs in the bilateral cerebellum and right lingual gyrus, as well as a loss of the hub in the left ACC. SPMS patients showed a reduced number of cortical hubs, especially in the left hemisphere, which were shifted to posterior brain regions.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Brain hubs (a left hemisphere, b right hemisphere) of the functional networks of healthy controls (HCs) and patients with different MS clinical phenotypes. Hubs were identified as brain regions having either integrated nodal degree or betweenness centrality one standard deviation greater than the network average. Hubs present in HC only are reported in red, hubs present in MS patients only are reported in blue, and hubs present in all groups are reported in black. RRMS relapsing remitting MS, BMS benign MS, SPMS secondary progressive MS, ACC anterior cingulate cortex, Caud caudate nucleus, Cer cerebellum, ITG inferior temporal gyrus, Ling lingual gyrus, MCC middle cingulate cortex, MTG middle temporal gyrus, OFC orbitofrontal cortex, Pall pallidus, PHG parahippocampal gyrus, Precun precuneus, Put putamen, SFG superior frontal gyrus, Sup TP superior temporal pole, Thal thalamus

Table 7 Brain hubs in healthy controls and patients with MS and different phenotypes

Compared to HCs, RRMS patients experienced a significantly lower nodal degree of the bilateral caudate nucleus (q ≤ 0.001). A trend towards a lower nodal degree of the left caudate was also detected when comparing SPMS vs RRMS (q = 0.004) or BMS (q = 0.03) patients, and BMS vs RRMS patients (q = 0.04).

Network analysis and structural damage

In patients, global and regional network properties were not correlated with T2 lesion volume and normalized brain volume.

Discussion

Cortico-cortical and cortico-subcortical disconnections secondary to the involvement of white matter tracts have been consistently demonstrated by structural MRI studies of patients with MS. This architectural disruption is clinically relevant, since it was found to contribute to cognitive impairment in these patients (Dineen et al. 2009; Mesaros et al. 2012). We hypothesized that a disconnection between functionally relevant brain areas might also contribute to explain cognitive deficits of MS patients, and better characterize disease heterogeneity, in term of clinical phenotypes. To prove our hypothesis, we used a graph theoretical analysis of RS fMRI to assess global and regional topological brain functional organization in a large cohort of MS patients. The main advantage of RS fMRI analysis is that this task-free approach allows inferences related to differences between healthy and diseased subjects, which are not influenced by task performance, thus permitting the assessment of patients with severe clinical disability and/or cognitive deficits (Rocca et al. 2010c).

Despite maintaining small-world attributes in MS patients, a disruption of global functional organization was detected, as shown by the reduction of mean network degree, global efficiency and hierarchy, and the increase of path length and assortativity. These abnormalities of global network properties allowed us to distinguish CI MS patients from HCs and CP patients, but not the main MS clinical phenotypes. Small-world networks combine high levels of local clustering among nodes and short paths that globally link all the nodes of the network (Bullmore and Sporns 2009). The simplest global network property is the mean network degree, which is a measure of density or the total “wiring cost” of the network. Its reduction in MS patients suggests that their functional brain networks have more nodes with a few connections (low degree) and fewer nodes with many connections (high degree) compared to those of controls (Rubinov and Sporns 2010). To note, network clustering coefficient, which is a measure of functional segregation (i.e., the ability of specialized processes to occur within highly interconnected groups of brain regions), was not affected in MS patients. Conversely, the characteristic path length and global efficiency, which are measures of functional integration (i.e., ability to rapidly combine specialized information from distributed brain regions) (Rubinov and Sporns 2010), were altered. Therefore, our results suggest a preservation of the efficiency of local information transfer and processing and an impairment of global integration, likely to reflect a reduced competence in information exchange between distant brain areas. This is also supported by the higher assortativity and lower hierarchy we found in patients’ networks. In assortative networks, nodes with many connections tend to be connected to other nodes with many connections, and nodes with low connections are linked to other low connection nodes (Newman 2002). Hierarchy is the tendency of hubs to connect to nodes that are not otherwise connected to each other (Bassett et al. 2008). Therefore, increased assortativity and reduced hierarchy indicate an impaired wiring efficiency at a system level. Conceivably, this is likely to contribute to explain cognitive impairment in MS patients (since cognition depends on coordinated interactions among many brain regions). Abnormalities of these two metrics also point to a tendency to form subnetworks of highly connected core regions (Bassett et al. 2008; Newman 2002), which might be protected from being injured due to redundant connections (Newman 2002).

Abnormalities of network properties similar to those we have found in MS patients have been described in patients with schizophrenia and have contributed to support the hypothesis of schizophrenia as a disconnection syndrome (Bassett et al. 2008). Conversely, the only study that has assessed, so far, a few of these network metrics in a small group of MS patients (Schoonheim et al. 2011) found no abnormalities of clustering coefficient and a reduced path length in male patients only. Differences in number of subjects enrolled (only 15 subjects in Schoonheim et al. 2011), and analysis methods (Pearson’s correlation vs synchronization likelihood; computation of network average degree vs use of correlation threshold) are likely to account for differences between ours and previous results.

The analysis of network organization at a regional level allowed us to identify the nodes with a different role in MS patients vs HC, not only in term of a qualitative difference in hub distribution, but also of a reduced functional connectivity of strategic brain regions. Independent of cognitive status and disease phenotype, the hallmarks of hubs distribution in MS patients were: (1) a loss of hubs in the SFG, precuneus and ACC in the left hemisphere; (2) a different lateralization of basal ganglia hubs; and (3) a formation of hubs, which were not seen in HCs, in the left temporal pole and cerebellar lobule IV–V. A decreased nodal degree was found in the bilateral caudate nucleus and right cerebellum.

The precuneus, SFG and ACC are part of the default mode network (DMN). Disruption of DMN functional connectivity has been associated with cognitive deficits in elderly individuals (Damoiseaux et al. 2008) and MS patients (Rocca et al. 2010c). The precuneus and the surrounding posterior cingulate cortex are among the brain regions with the highest metabolic rates at rest (Cavanna and Trimble 2006), and have been shown to compose the structural core of the human brain (Hagmann et al. 2008). This functional/anatomical correspondence has led to the hypothesis that core regions of the parietal lobe may be the basis for shaping large-scale brain dynamics (Hagmann et al. 2008). Anatomically, the principal extraparietal cortico-cortical connections of the precuneus are with regions of the frontal (prefrontal cortex and ACC) and temporal (associative cortex in the superior temporal sulcus) lobes, and its subcortical connections are with the thalamus, caudate nucleus and putamen (Cavanna and Trimble 2006). Through connections with the pons, the precuneus can also gain access to multiple cerebellar circuits. Hence, our findings suggest a disruption of functional interactions of cortical and subcortical structures of this network, which is thought to be involved in elaborating integrated and associative information.

Such an impairment of regional network properties was found to contribute to cognitive impairment and phenotypic variability of MS patients. Hub distribution was relatively intact in CP patients, who also were found to harbour additional hubs in the cerebellum, orbitofrontal cortex and caudate nucleus. Conversely, CI patients did not have hubs in the thalamus and left frontal lobes. The lack of frontal lobe hubs in CI MS patients agrees with the results of several previous active fMRI studies, which have consistently demonstrated an increased recruitment of regions located in the frontal lobes. These studies also suggested that an increased functional connectivity between frontal regions may represent a compensatory mechanism contributing to a preserved cognitive performance (Bonnet et al. 2010; Rocca et al. 2010b; Staffen et al. 2002). Thalamic involvement has been associated with many clinical manifestations of MS, including fatigue, and cognitive and motor deficits (Minagar et al. 2013). However, while several studies have investigated the contribution of thalamic structural damage to MS clinical manifestations (Minagar et al. 2013), this is one of the first studies showing that functional loss of the thalamic connecter is associated with MS cognitive impairment. In addition, it suggests that the redistribution of functional hubs can be functionally ineffective or associated with more severe clinical manifestations (probably reflecting a maladaptive mechanism). This is the case, for instance, for the hubs located in the cerebellum and lingual gyrus in CI patients. An impaired functional connectivity between the cerebellum and the frontal lobes has been shown to contribute to failure of cognitive compensation in MS patients (Bonnet et al. 2010); on the contrary, an increased recruitment of posterior brain regions and the cerebellum has been detected in CI patients with primary progressive MS (Rocca et al. 2010b).

While the analysis of global network properties was not informative to explain clinical phenotypic variability, abnormalities of hub distribution contributed to better characterize the main MS clinical phenotypes. Indeed, this analysis revealed that RRMS patients retained the majority of hubs, while SPMS experienced the formation of several additional hubs in the posterior regions of the brain and cerebellum, and a hub loss in the left frontal lobe and along the cingulum. This suggests that loss of “critical” hubs and functional incompetence of alternative hubs may contribute to a progressive disease course. Finally, patients with BMS had a pattern of hub distribution somewhat between those of RRMS and SPMS patients, with a preservation of the hub in the left middle cingulum, and several additional hubs in the cerebellum, bilaterally. The capability to maintain over time the functional integrity and specialization of specific brain regions might be among the factors responsible for the favourable clinical course of BMS, and agrees with the results of previous active motor and cognitive fMRI studies (Rocca et al. 2009, 2010a, 2012). Notably, regional modifications of network organization correlated to phenotypic variability had only a partial overlap with those associated with cognitive impairment, indicating the importance of functional abnormalities at a regional level in explaining the various clinical manifestations of MS.

This study is not without limitations. First, the cross-sectional design did not allow us to assess the temporal dynamics of the detected functional abnormalities and how they might eventually impact the development of cognitive deficits or the evolution to a different disease clinical phenotype. Second, cognitive impairment was defined based on the performance at PASAT and not on a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation. Nevertheless, PASAT has been shown to be sensitive to abnormalities of working memory and information processing speed which are among the most frequently affected cognitive domains in MS. Third, the topological organization of brain networks has been shown to be affected by the parcellation strategies applied (de Reus and van den Heuvel 2013), the measure used to assess functional coupling between RS fMRI time series (Zalesky et al. 2012), and the metric used to assess node centrality (Zuo et al. 2012). As a consequence, we cannot exclude that the use of another atlas, a different parcellation method, another coupling metric (e.g., partial correlation (Salvador et al. 2005), synchronization likelihood (Schoonheim et al. 2011) or wavelet analysis (Bassett et al. 2008)), or another centrality measure (e.g., eigenvector centrality) (Lohmann et al. 2010) would have resulted in different characterization of network properties. Fourth, to investigate the influence of structural disease-related damage on functional network parameters we used only T2 lesion volumes and brain atrophy. Future studies using advanced structural MRI techniques, such as diffusion tensor or magnetization transfer MRI, might be more helpful to investigate associations between microstructural damage and functional network abnormalities and their interplay in the different stages of the disease. Even if it is a challenging task, due to the presence of systems interconnected by polysynaptic circuitry, further graph analysis studies are warranted to determine the relationship between structural and functional connectivity abnormalities in these patients.