Introduction

Pace-of-life syndrome (POLS; Wolf et al. 2007; Réale et al. 2010; Sol et al. 2018) theory has emerged in recent years to explain observed patterns of life history, physiology, and behavior in the context of fitness trade-offs. At its core, POLS theory considers how life history, behavioral, and physiological traits coevolve under species- and population-specific environmental conditions (Dammhahn et al. 2018). Under this theory, any particular pace-of-life (POL) exists along a fast-slow continuum of life history traits that reflect conflicts between maximizing survival and fecundity (Stearns 1983; Sæther 1988; Dammhahn et al. 2018; Sol et al. 2018). On one end of this continuum, fast-living strategies tend to be characterized by short life spans, increased prioritization of current reproductive output, and risk tolerance; on the other, slow-living strategies or POL generally exhibits long lifespans, increased prioritization of future over current reproductive output, and increased risk aversion (Martin et al. 2000; Wolf et al. 2007; Hau et al. 2010; Sol et al. 2018). In this way, life history characteristics, physiological traits, and behavioral tendencies should fall predictably upon a single predictable continuum or syndrome, and this syndrome could explain variation in these traits both among species (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002) and individuals (Réale et al. 2010).

Current and future reproduction have frequently been found to a trade-off in nesting animals, such as turtles (Rivalan et al. 2005) and birds (Nilsson and Svensson 1996), making examinations of POLS in such systems a promising avenue of research. In oviparous species that construct nests, nest placement often determines offspring survival (e.g., Dunk et al. 1997; Wilson 1998; Mitchell et al. 2015). However, behaviors associated with reproduction and nesting can have substantial effects on maternal survival, as nest placement decisions may influence maternal risk (Spencer 2002). The manner in which nesting females balance this trade-off depends in large part on life history traits (Stearns 1992; Ghalambor and Martin 2001; Schwarzkopf and Andrews 2012; Refsnider et al. 2015), where long-lived species with delayed ages of maturity are generally expected to select habitats that maximize adult survival probability (Spencer 2002). Physiological and morphological traits should also influence the manner with which nesting animals respond to trade-offs; for example, ectothermic physiology places individuals in situations where they must balance predation risk and thermoregulatory behavior, which ultimately affects metabolism and growth rates (Kashon and Carlson 2018). Viewed from a POLS perspective, covariance between life history, physiological, and behavioral traits related to nesting and risk-taking should lead to tangible differences in how animals prioritize reproductive activities in any given season (Southwood 1977; Clark and Shutler 1999; Kolbe and Janzen 2002; Gaillard et al. 2010).

As long-lived organisms on the slow end of the fast-slow life history continuum, freshwater turtles represent particularly intriguing study organisms for studying costs and benefits associated with nest site selection. Nesting female turtles make terrestrial movements over several days to weeks to reach nest sites, often with strong natal philopatry and nest site fidelity (Rowe et al. 2005; Steen et al. 2012; Refsnider et al. 2015). As ectotherms, thermoregulation is important for adult survival, and overexposure of nesting females to high temperatures influences the timing and duration of turtle nesting movements (Burger and Montevecchi 1975; Spotila and Standora 1985; Spotila et al. 1997; Jessop et al. 2000). Efficient thermoregulation may also be particularly important for turtles developing eggs (Litzgus and Brooks 2000; Millar et al. 2012), as internal temperatures during gestation can influence embryo survival in reptiles (Telemeco et al. 2017). While POLS theory predicts that most turtle species should prioritize their own survival prospects over current reproductive output (Congdon et al. 1993, 1994), few studies have assessed how well physiological and behavioral traits match predictions of POLS theory during nesting excursions in turtles or other slow pace-of-life taxa.

We examined hypotheses about habitat selection and nesting choices grounded in POLS theory in Blanding’s turtle (Emydoidea blandingii), integrating information on nesting movements, locations, and success with a landscape-scale sampling of thermal conditions, predator activities, and landscape habitat conditions. Blanding’s turtles may move up to 1 km before selecting terrestrial nest sites in open patches of habitat (Steen et al. 2012) and often exhibit strong nest site fidelity (Congdon et al. 1983). Furthermore, these terrestrial nesting excursions are generally among the longest distances moved within a given season and often represent the only terrestrial excursions made by females (Ross and Anderson 1990; Rowe and Moll 1991). While hardened shells may reduce the sensitivity of turtles to predation risk, many well-armored vertebrates still respond to predation risk (if they fall on the slow end of the fast-slow life history continuum) by shifting nest placement in order to reduce adult encounters with predators (e.g., turtles in Spencer 2002) or by decreasing time spent moving through patches with higher risk (e.g., porcupines in Pokallus and Pauli 2015). Blanding’s turtles also exhibit a lower critical thermal maximum than most other aquatic turtle species (Hutchinson et al. 1966), and as such exposure to extreme temperatures in terrestrial habitats may constitute a serious risk to nesting females. Thus, Blanding’s turtles may be forced to balance factors that influence their own perception of risk to their own survival and that of their nest while navigating the landscape to reach nesting areas.

We hypothesized that females should exhibit risk aversion and select habitat to minimize stress and perception of risk during nesting excursions. Risk aversion during nesting forays may be represented spatially by avoidance of areas associated with increased predator encounters or suboptimal thermal conditions. However, as armored, slow-moving, ectothermic vertebrates, we predicted that turtles should show stronger patterns of habitat selection associated with regulating body temperature than with reducing predator encounters, as avoidance of areas with higher predator encounter probability may be unnecessary when morphological defenses can buffer against fatal predator encounters. We also hypothesized that perceived risk factors encountered during nesting movements will influence turtles’ ultimate choice of nest site. In keeping with POLS theory, we predict that individuals subjected to increased perception of risk and stress will be more likely to nest in areas that minimize additional exposure of adults to risk and stress. We also hypothesized that the combined effects of risk on the terrestrial landscape and risk avoidance behaviors that increase the safety of adult females will result in choices of nest sites that are suboptimal for nest success.

Materials and methods

Study area

Our study was conducted during the summers (nesting seasons) of 2016 and 2017 at Sandhill Wildlife Area (SWA) in Wood County, Wisconsin, which harbors a large, stable, and well-studied Blanding’s turtle population (Reid and Peery 2014; Reid et al. 2016a, 2016b, 2017). Sandhill Wildlife Area is well-suited for studies of fitness trade-offs because preferred nesting areas (restored oak savannah and other open areas) are separated from preferred aquatic habitats by forest and other unsuitable habitat, suggesting that nesting turtles may be exposed to predation risk and suboptimal thermal conditions during long-distance terrestrial nesting movements. Much of the work on nesting ecology at this site was based around three focal nesting areas towards the western, central, and northeastern portions of the site, although nests have been found along roads and near open areas throughout the area (Fig. S1a); in most cases, turtles nested in open habitats associated with loose substrates. For all work involved, it was not possible to record data blindly, because our study involved focal animals in the field.

Radio telemetry and nest location

We captured 20 turtles in early spring of 2016 using baited hoop net traps and through opportunistic encounters on roads, and 24-month radio transmitters were attached to left rear costal scutes of all captured adult females using marine-grade epoxy. A small iButton data logger (DS1921G; Maxim Integrated) was also affixed to the carapace in a similar manner to measure thermal conditions experienced by the turtle, but on right rear costal scutes instead. Before attachment, we waterproofed iButtons using a dark-colored Plasti Dip (Plasti Dip International), and set each iButton to record temperatures every 30 min. In most cases (n = 16 out of 20 originally captured in 2016), females captured during the 2016 field season were tracked again during the 2017 field season, and all female turtles processed in this way were tracked weekly until late May. Three additional animals were captured in 2017 to supplement radio telemetry efforts. Previous research on Blanding’s turtles in Wisconsin suggested that nesting usually begins early or mid-June (BNR pers. obs.). Therefore, starting in late May to early June, turtles were tracked every day to determine the onset of nesting movements. Once a turtle left the water, it was radio-tracked daily until nesting occurred, which typically takes 5–17 days (Ernst and Lovich 2009).

We attached thread spools to the rear vertebral scutes of females when they were near potential nesting grounds to identify nest locations, as nesting females typically buried thread trails when excavating nest cavities (Beaudry et al. 2010; Reid et al. 2016a). At the time of nest location, we recorded information on nest placement using a handheld GPS (to the nearest ± 5 m) and placed markers approximately 2to 3 m away from each estimated nest location. Nest depredation constitutes the single most important determinant of nest success in Blanding’s turtle (causing > 90% of observed nest failures; Congdon et al. 2000). Thus, nests were monitored daily for the first week (as Blanding’s turtle nest depredation rates are highest in the first several days after nest placement; Ernst and Lovich 2009) and then weekly for approximately 1 month to detect patterns of nest success. Nests were deemed successful if no evidence of depredation (determined based on an excavated cavity with egg shells scattered around it) was found between June and July.

Quantifying sub-lethal predation risk and thermal extremes

Females at SWA sometimes exhibit injuries consistent with predator attacks and have a higher overall incidence of injury than males at the same site (Table S1, Fig. S2), although yearly survival rates of males and females are similar (Reid et al. 2016a). While males are slightly larger than females in this species (Ernst and Lovich 2009), the differences in body size between males and females are likely too slight to lead to differences in survival. This suggests that nesting exposes females to additional risks of attack from predators and that these risks can incur costs to females during their nesting forays. Predation risk to adult turtles was measured with 33 wildlife cameras stratified across the landscape using a grid-based sampling design. The entire study area was subdivided into 500 m × 500 m cells, with cell size set to maximize spatial coverage throughout the study site given limited camera availability. If the centroid of a grid cell contained terrestrial habitat (delineated using the WISCLAND dataset; http://dnr.wi.gov/maps/gis/datalandcover.html), we placed a wildlife camera trap (Reconyx HC500 and PC800) baited with fatty acid scent (FAS) predator-survey discs within 20 m of the grid center. Camera traps remained in the field from late May to mid-July of each study year, after which storage cards were removed and photographs were examined for the presence of potential mammalian predators. We identified each predator observed to species; photographs of the same species taken within 5 min of each other were considered temporally autocorrelated and were grouped as a single detection. We quantified two metrics for each camera location: predator species richness and relative predator activity (Table 1). Given that there is considerable uncertainty about how turtles may respond to mammalian predators, we predicted that areas with increased predator species richness may appear riskier to nesting turtles due to a wider variety of cues from more species. In contrast, areas with increased predator activity would appear riskier due to an increased frequency with which cues are refreshed. Given the relatively small number of independent detections across both years (n = 79), we summarized predation risk metrics across both years. An inverse-distance weighting (IDW; Shepard 1968) approach was then used to interpolate both adult predation risk metrics between camera trapping stations. We chose to interpolate across the entire study area regardless of land cover type, as wetland and upland habitat are highly intermingled at this study site.

Table 1 Covariates used for each analysis. All land cover covariates are based on WISCLAND

We measured thermal characteristics across the landscape at 28 temperature monitoring stations using the same 500 m grid used to measure predation risk. As with iButtons deployed on turtles, iButtons planned for environmental temperature measurement were coated in PlastiDip before deployment. We then generated a single random location within each grid cell and sampled that location if it contained terrestrial habitat; in this way, we monitored temperatures in terrestrial habitats likely encountered by turtles moving to nest sites and standardized our thermal monitoring system to include only terrestrial habitats simultaneously assessed for predation risk surveys. At each temperature monitoring station, we suspended a waterproofed iButton from a string approximately 9 cm above the ground to approximate shell height of an adult Blanding’s turtle (Ernst and Lovich 2009). We sampled a mix of both shaded and open areas in this way to capture variability in vegetative cover experienced by turtles.

All iButtons monitored temperatures every 30 min from late May to early July. Before processing thermal data for each year, we first examined reported minimum and maximum temperatures at local National Weather Service (NWS) monitoring stations and excluded any data logger measurements outside of the range of temperatures reported by this NWS station; generally, these observations were unrealistically warm (> 50 °C), suggesting that these measurements either represent periods of extreme solar exposure or data logger malfunction, and < 5% of available data were excluded in this way. We then trimmed thermal records for each turtle and iButton station to the same temporal duration, generally encompassing late May to late June. We extracted environmental temperature data for each time with available telemetry data and used this environmental data to create time-specific interpolated thermal layers to allow instantaneous comparison of shell temperatures with environmental temperatures (Table 1). We then summarized, for each iButton station, the proportion of each day above the 90th percentile and below the 10th percentile of observed temperatures within each study season. These daily estimates of extreme temperature exposure were then averaged throughout each study season for each iButton station. These values were then interpolated across iButton stations using the same procedure outlined for predation risk metrics to produce two metrics of temperature exposure: high temperature exposure and low temperature exposure (Table 1).

Delineating used and available habitat

Before testing hypotheses, we first processed both movement and nesting data to facilitate habitat selection analyses. For movement habitat selection analyses, we first calculated the average distance between consecutive telemetry locations for all turtles with > 5 locations in a year for both 2015 and 2016. We then used the largest of these two distances (350 m in 2016) to generate a buffer zone around each turtle location, and randomly sampled five points around each telemetry location (see Fig. S3). We consider these randomly generated points to represent habitat available to each turtle, according to a use vs. availability design (Design III in Manly et al. 2002), and assigned 1 to each used location and 0 to each available location. After data filtering, we retained 187 telemetry locations for Blanding’s turtles across 14 unique animals, which were compared with 1023 random locations across the 2 years of study.

For nest site selection analyses, we drew on a dataset of 141 nests located between 2011 and 2017. Techniques used for locating these nests are described in Reid et al. (2016a), but briefly involved searching for nesting individuals in the evening between May and July, and using thread spools to track turtles to nesting locations. Before assessing nest site selection, we first delineated the boundaries of each nesting patch using a kernel density approach with individual nests as observations. We then randomly selected 50 locations within each nesting patch to represent within-patch nesting habitat availability and assigned 1 to used locations and 0 to available locations (see Fig. S3). For each used and available location, we calculated the distance to forests, wetlands, open water, and roads (Table 1).

Analysis

To test hypothesis 1 (that female movement behavior pre-nesting was more strongly influenced by temperature than by predation risk), we evaluated multiple competing models of habitat selection using an information theoretic approach. Prior to building models, we calculated correlation coefficients between thermal and predation risk covariates, and covariates with r > |0.6| were not included simultaneously in the same model statements to reduce multicollinearity. Covariates were not correlated between categories (thermal or predation risk), but the following pairs of covariates were highly correlated: predator species richness and relative predator activity and high and low temperature exposure probabilities. We then used the function “clogit” in the package survival in R (Therneau 2015) and conditional logistic regression to evaluate a suite of a priori model statements that describe differences in temperature and predator metrics (Table 1) between used and available locations (Table S2). Conditional logistic regression allows used locations to be paired with nearby available locations and is considered more suitable for paired use-availability designs (Compton et al. 2002; Duchesne et al. 2010). Due to issues with mixed-effect model convergence, we chose to compute robust standard errors instead, which have been used to control for individual-level effects in paired regression models (Fortin et al. 2005; Forester et al. 2009; Zeller et al. 2017). We used Akaike’s Information Criterion, corrected for small sample size (AICc; Sugiura 1978; Hurvich and Tsai 1991), to evaluate models best supported by this dataset. For this, as well as analyses described below, we considered any model within 2 ΔAICc of the top model to be competitive with the top model and interpreted 95% confidence intervals for covariate estimates (Burnham and Anderson 2002). We calculated McFadden’s pseudo-R2 for each model as a metric of model fit (McFadden 1973).

To test hypothesis 2 (that female risk aversion should lead to nest placement decisions that are related to reducing the perception of risk and stress), we compared predation risk and temperatures experienced by nesting turtles before nesting between successful and unsuccessful nests and related these same movement covariates to patterns of nest placement. We first calculated predation risk and thermal covariates for each turtle with known nests (n = 26 nesting excursions across 17 individual turtles), regardless of the number of locations available for each turtle within each year. Since some turtles with known nest locations did not have available turtle-specific thermal data due to data logger loss and malfunctions, we used only indirectly-measured covariates (predator species richness and activity and high and low temperature exposure) for this analysis and summarized averaged levels of each covariate for each pre-nesting telemetry path. We then modeled the influence of a priori combinations of predator and temperature covariates on nest fate using logistic regression (Table S3). As in analyses for hypothesis 1, no models included more than one each of predation risk and exposure-based thermal covariates, and each model statement was run multiple times for each combination of predator and temperature covariates. We repeated this same model selection approach with each nest selection covariate as the response variable using linear regression; in this case, we naturally log-transformed each nest selection covariate before modeling the effects of adult experience on nest selection to improve model fit.

To test hypothesis 3 (that combined effects of risk on the terrestrial landscape and risk avoidance behaviors that increase safety of adult females will result in choices of nest sites that are suboptimal for nest success), we used the full nest success dataset (encompassing 2011–2017) to identify factors on the landscape that maximize nest success, and used a use vs. availability analysis to determine whether turtles select for factors that reduce nest mortality or adult exposure to predators when selecting nest sites. Because we aimed to link patterns of habitat selection to nest success, we also compared these same covariates between successful and unsuccessful nests (with 1 assigned to successful habitat and 0 to unsuccessful nests). For analyses of land cover effects on nest selection and nest success, correlation coefficients between covariates were calculated before model construction, and any covariates with r > |0.6| were not included in the same model statements. We then modeled the effects of a priori combinations of landscape covariates (Table S4) using unpaired logistic regression. As for hypothesis 1, we used AICc to rank models. We also explored how predator covariates influence nest selection and nest success using a similar approach to determine if turtles are actively selecting areas that either (a) reduce nest depredation, which would suggest prioritization of current reproductive output, or (b) reduce adult exposure to predators, which would suggest prioritization of future reproductive output. In order to consider if turtles select nests to reduce nest depredation, we created a separate interpolation layer that represents nest predator activity, focusing on the primary documented nest predator at this site and for this species, the raccoon (Ernst and Lovich 2009). In order to consider if turtles select nests to reduce adult exposure to predators, we used the same predator species richness and activity surfaces as for analyses of pre-nesting habitat selection, as all predator species included in this surface represent animals that turtles may perceive as a threat. Then, we modeled the effects of predation risk to nests and adults on nest selection and nest success using unpaired logistic regression and used AICc to rank models. As in analyses of hypothesis 1, we calculated McFadden’s pseudo-R2 for each nest selection and nest success model. For all analyses involving p-values, we set α = 0.05, and means are presented ± 1 SD unless otherwise noted. All data analyses were completed in R, version 3.4.3.

Results

Interpolations of predation and thermal covariates

A total of 79 independent predator detections were made between May and June across both years of study (55 in 2016 and 24 in 2017), representing seven total species: badger (Taxidea taxus), black bear (Ursus americanus), bobcat (Lynx rufus), coyote (Canis latrans), fisher (Martes pennanti), raccoon (Procyon lotor), and striped skunk (Mephitis mephitis). The most records were made for coyotes (40/79), followed by raccoons (25/79), bear (5/79), and bobcat (4/79), while the remaining three species were only detected on one or two occasions (5/79 total). Interpolated estimates of predator species richness suggested the presence of more predator species around the largest nesting patches in the western, central, and northeastern portions of the site (Fig. S1). Conversely, interpolated estimates of predator activity suggested a single hotspot of activity in several of the northeastern nesting patches, with varying levels of predator activity throughout the study area. After data filtering, a total of 36,542 turtle shell temperature measurements (21,581 in 2017 and 14,961 in 2016) and 59,481 environmental temperatures (34,045 in 2017 and 25,436 in 2016) collected from May to June 2016 and 2017 were available for interpolation and data analysis. Across all temperature measurements in both years, the average shell temperature was 24.53 ± 3.93 °C and average environmental temperature was 22.87 ± 5.58 °C. Estimates of high temperature exposure varied from 0% to 6.41%, and estimates of low temperature exposure varied from 0% to 6.63% (Fig. S1).

Predator encounter-thermal microclimate trade-offs

The top pre-nesting habitat selection model suggested that turtles were generally warmer than random locations (β = 2.248, 95% CI = 1.492 to 3.003, Fig. 1a), but avoided areas exposed to extremely high temperatures during pre-nesting movements (β = − 0.497, 95% CI − 0.838 to − 0.156, Fig. 1b). Although the interaction term between these two variables was negative, its confidence intervals overlapped with zero (β = − 0.479, 95% CI = − 1.044 to 0.087). Three models had ΔAICc < 2.0, and each model included an interaction between T and H (Table S5). Although the remaining two models with ΔAICc < 2.0 included variables related to predation risk, confidence intervals for each predation risk covariate estimate overlapped with zero.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Pre-nesting habitat selection for time-specific temperatures, based on the top habitat selection model. Light red shading indicates 95% CIs. b Plot of the effects of high temperature exposure on pre-nesting habitat selection based on the top habitat selection model. Light red shading indicates 95% CIs, and gray points indicate observed data used in model fitting

Relationships between movement and nesting behavior

The distance nests were placed from forests was positively associated with average predator species richness experienced by turtles before nesting (β = 1.37, 95% CI = 0.49 to 2.25; Fig. 2; Table S6). No models that related movement covariates to distance of nests to wetlands, open water, or roads outperformed null models (see Table S7). No covariates related to movement habitat selection appeared to have an impact on nest success, as the null model was either the top model or within 2 AICc of the top model in each comparison (see Table S8 for a full summary of these results).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Plot of the relationship between average predator species richness (Rav) pre-nesting and natural-log-transformed nest distance to forest edges (DF), based on the top model relating pre-nesting habitat selection to nest selection. Light red shading indicates 95% CIs, and gray points indicate observed data used in model fitting

Nest selection and nest success

The most supported nest selection model suggested that turtles nest closer to forests (β = − 0.0037, 95% CI = − 0.0049 to − 0.0020; Fig. 3a) and farther from open water (β = 0.0017, 95% CI = 0.0010 to 0.0024; Fig. 3a) than expected. Although distance to wetlands and distance to roads were included in this top model, each had confidence intervals that overlapped with zero (wetlands: β = 0.0000, 95% CI = −0.0011 to 0.0013; roads: β = −0.0030, 95% CI = −0.0082 to 0.0019, Fig. 3a). This model had AICc weight = 1 and ΔAICc = 19.14 relative to the second best land cover-based nest selection model (Table S9). Selected nests were also generally in areas with less overall predator activity (β = − 0.08112, 95% CI = − 0.1563 to − 0.0242, Fig. 3a), and no support was found for the raccoon model (ΔAICc = 7.62, β = − 0.1789, 95% CI = − 0.5140 to 0.1280; Table S10).

Fig. 3
figure 3

a Odds ratios for effects of covariates on nest selection. b Odds ratios for effects of covariates on nest success. Bars indicate 95% CIs

The top land cover-based nest success model suggested that nests were more successful when laid further from wetlands (β = 0.004886, 95% CI = 0.002190 to 0.007793; Fig. 3b; Table S9). This model also indicated increasing nest success probability closer to open water; however, as 95% confidence intervals for this effect overlapped with zero (β = − 0.001043, 95% CI = − 0.002369 to 0.000231) and a model with only distance to wetlands was within 0.47 ΔAICc of the top model, we interpret this open water effect as an uninformative parameter, and interpret only the second best model with distance to wetlands as the only predictor (Arnold 2010). In this model, as in the top model, nests were more likely to survive further from wetlands (β = 0.004162, 95% CI = 0.001612 to 0.006993). These areas were also associated with more overall predator activity (β = 0.3730, 95% CI = 0.1582 to 0.6095; Fig. 3b), with no clear effects of raccoon activity on nest success (ΔAICc = 13.16, β = 0.2291, 95% CI = −0.5448 to 1.014; Table S10). Although an outlier was detected in the predator activity model, removal of this outlier did not substantially impact the predator activity covariate estimate (β = 0.3728, 95% CI = 0.1553 to 0.6094).

Discussion

According to POLS theory, an animal’s life history traits and position along a fast-slow life history continuum should lead to predictable covariance in behavioral and physiological traits (Dammhahn et al. 2018). In this study, we hypothesized three core ways in which the Blanding’s turtle’s relatively slow life history should influence behavior during nesting: (i) risk avoidance, particularly of areas that may expose the animal to increased perception of risk (due to predators) or stress (due to suboptimal thermal microclimates) during nesting excursions, and with greater avoidance of areas that expose animals to physiological stress rather than predator encounters due to morphological defenses; (ii) behavioral trade-offs between current reproduction and risk avoidance, leading to nest placement decisions that minimize adult perception of risk and stress with negative or minimal effects on nest success; and (iii) nest site preferences that do not appear to maximize current nest success.

Risk avoidance during movements

In support of our first hypothesis, animals exhibited risk avoidance when moving overland to nesting areas, but selected more strongly for habitats with optimal thermal microclimates rather than those that reduced exposure to predators. Thermally-motivated habitat selection could represent a suite of behavioral strategies to maximize both fitness components—adult survival and nest success—simultaneously. Gravid animals exhibit increased frequency and intensity of thermoregulatory behaviors (Shine 1980; Congdon 1989; Lefevre and Brooks 1995; Millar et al. 2012), and thermoregulatory behaviors appear to impact embryo survival in many species (e.g., Shine 1980; Gutzke and Packard 1987; Gutzke et al. 1987; Telemeco et al. 2017). Thus, preferences for higher temperatures overall may represent adaptations to maximize embryo survival. However, adult overexposure influences the duration of nesting excursions (Burger and Montevecchi 1975; Spotila and Standora 1985; Spotila et al. 1997; Jessop et al. 2000), and efficient thermoregulation impacts adult survival in many reptiles (Adolph and Porter 1993; Nutting and Graham 1993), suggesting that adult survival is partially dependent on accurate thermoregulation during nesting excursions. Since we did not disturb nests to determine cases of developmental mortality associated with thermal conditions experienced by nests or adult females, we were unable to determine whether patterns of thermally-motivated habitat selection in this study were more for the benefit of the mother or offspring. However, given that female thermoregulation has documented positive impacts on both embryo survival and adult survival, we interpret these risk-averse behaviors as potentially associated with both fitness components.

Although patterns of habitat selection before nesting suggested stress aversion in keeping with POLS theory, we failed to document avoidance of areas with higher predicted predator species richness or activity before nesting. On its face, this appears to run counter to POLS theory, since animals with a slow pace-of-life may generally be expected to avoid areas with more predators; however, we emphasize that morphology must be considered in tandem with life history traits to predict behavioral outcomes when exposed to predators (Royauté et al. 2018). Armored vertebrates are generally less likely to exhibit flight responses when they encounter predators, often leaning heavily on their morphological defenses to avoid harm (Stankowich and Blumstein 2005). Although some evidence suggests that armored vertebrates may exhibit avoidance of predation risk during foraging behaviors (Pokallus and Pauli 2015), the relatively abbreviated duration of nesting excursions in this study (1 to 2 weeks), slow metabolic rates of most turtles (Gibbons 1987; Avise 1992), and frequent aquatic foraging in this species (Ernst and Lovich 2009) likely mean that foraging requirements should have little effect on the behavior of nesting turtles in upland habitat. Although females did not appear to avoid areas with more predator species or activity in our study, some of the larger animals documented on our cameras (bears, bobcats, and coyotes, for example) likely could kill and consume an adult Blanding’s turtle, and predators such as raccoons have been documented to occasionally consume or attack female Blanding’s turtles (Standing et al. 1999). Thus, we caution that this lack of documented avoidance of areas with more predators before nesting may not reflect the potential for mortality or injury due to predators and acknowledge that failure to avoid areas associated with increased predation risk may instead indicate that encounter rates between predators and nesting turtles may be relatively low and unimportant for survival. Keeping in mind the relative rarity of observations of adults being consumed by predators in this species, we do interpret the lack of predator avoidance pre-nesting documented in this study as consistent with morphological defenses of turtles and suggest that morphology may produce patterns of risk-taking that may not be consistent with hypotheses based solely on life history traits.

Nest placement and current reproductive success

Nest sites that were further from wetlands had a higher probability of nest success, a finding consistent with accounts for other turtle species (Kolbe and Janzen 2002; Spencer 2002; Reid et al. 2016a). Successful nest sites were, in turn, associated with more predator activity across all species, with no clear differences in activity of the primary nest predator at this site, the raccoon, between successful and unsuccessful nests. This result does appear contradictory at first, as areas with reduced nest depredation and increased nest success may be expected to have less predator activity overall; however, much of this difference in predator activity between successful and unsuccessful nests was driven by the presence of coyotes, which do not appear to consume Blanding’s turtle nests (Ernst and Lovich 2009). This suggests that increased coyote activity around nesting areas is not due to the nests themselves, rather, habitat features that Blanding’s turtles find desirable for nesting may be simultaneously but incidentally more profitable for coyote foraging. Although coyotes have not been found to consume adult Blanding’s turtles during nesting events, attacks by a variety of other species, such as foxes and raccoons, have been documented for adult turtles, and it is likely that coyotes would be no exception (Ernst and Lovich 2009). As in other studies of turtle nest success (Kolbe and Janzen 2002; Spencer 2002), these results indicate that areas associated with successful nests are riskier for nesting adults and indicate that turtles cannot select habitats that both maximize nest success and minimize adult risk at the same time (Spencer 2002).

We failed to find evidence that nest placement maximizes current reproductive success. Turtles exhibited a preference for nest sites close to forests, with no significant preference for nest sites close to or far away from wetlands, and the suite of covariates that predicted nest success appeared to differ entirely from the suite of covariates that predicted nest placement. This was surprising, as several other studies have found that nest preferences are positively correlated with nest success (Fontaine and Martin 2006; Chalfoun and Martin 2007), nest temperature (Kolbe and Janzen 2002), and offspring phenotype (Shine and Harlow 1996; Brown and Shine 2004), and this decoupling of nest site selection and nest success suggests that factors more related to future than current reproduction may be important in driving nest placement. Indeed, although nest success was higher in areas exposed to more predators, turtles chose to nest in areas less exposed to predators. This may indicate that nest placement is more influenced by risk avoidance in adults than maximizing nest success. Nesting behaviors are often costly for many vertebrates and increase adult mortality in some birds (Martin 1993; Merilä and Wiggins 1995; Ghalambor and Martin 2001) and turtles (Spencer 2002; Refsnider et al. 2015). While armored vertebrates are often less susceptible to non-consumptive effects of predation risk (Brown and Kotler 2004), previous studies of armored species indicate that predation risk can shape movement and habitat selection behaviors (Spencer 2002; Pokallus and Pauli 2015).

Placement of nests close to forests reduced the average number of predator species encountered before nesting. While this result appears somewhat at odds with the lack of avoidance of predators during nesting movements, we interpret this result as evidence that turtles respond to risk differently before and during nesting, and that nest placement decisions can have ramifications for the overall amount of risk experienced by turtles. When moving to nesting areas, female turtles can retreat into shells to avoid predation, and thus would be unlikely to exhibit avoidance of areas with more predators before nesting; furthermore, anecdotal observations of turtles during nesting excursions indicated that animals generally hid under leaf litter and thick understory cover in forested areas when not actively moving, suggesting that forests provide opportunities for cover during nesting excursions. In contrast, animals that are actively nesting may be less capable of retreating into shells or finding cover, are in areas with unobscured lines of sight, and may be acutely concerned about exposure to predators during nesting. Spencer (2002) found that Emydura macquarii adjusted the distance of its nests from water in the presence of predators, as predators upon adults in their system were generally more common near water. Outside of reptiles, birds do appear to adjust nesting behavior in response to perceived predation risk (Eggers et al. 2006; Fontaine and Martin 2006), suggesting that female well-being is a factor that is considered when animals select nest sites, particularly for species with a relatively slow pace-of-life (Spencer 2002).

POLS theory and nesting behavior in ectotherms

Given relatively mixed support for POLS theory in existing literature (Dammhahn et al. 2018), why did we document support for POLS theory in this current study? First, key assumptions of POLS theory—trade-offs between current and future reproduction and connections between physiology/behavior and life history decisions—are well-maintained in nesting systems, particularly for turtles and other long-lived vertebrates. Nesting excursions often tangibly impact adult predation risk, and multiple studies have suggested that animals may make nest placement decisions that reduce adult mortality while increasing nest mortality, suggesting that current and future reproduction often trade-off (Ghalambor and Martin 2001; Spencer 2002). Furthermore, physiological stress is often higher in nesting animals, particularly when brood size is large (Bókony et al. 2009), and behavioral decisions related to basking do appear to differ between nesting and non-nesting animals (Millar et al. 2012). Second, we incorporated gradients of risk and temperature in our study, which allowed us to ground our system in the underlying environmental gradients presumably producing behavioral patterns (Dammhahn et al. 2018). Third, the fitness currency in this present study—nest success—is directly tied to reproductive investment and risk-taking, making it appropriate for explorations grounded in POLS theory. Fourth, we chose to study turtles; as reptiles, they have among the slowest life histories of any vertebrates, and life history may particularly constrain behavior and physiology at the extremes of the fast-slow continuum (Gibbons 1987; Nelson et al. 2002).

Nesting turtles in this study exhibited habitat selection patterns that are consistent with predictions of POLS theory, namely, that long-lived organisms with life history traits associated with a slower pace-of-life should exhibit behavioral and physiological traits that maximize future reproductive output at the expense of current reproductive success (Ghalambor and Martin 2001; Spencer 2002; Dammhahn et al. 2018). However, it should be noted that we only address POLS theory predictions that stem from the relative position of turtles along a fast-slow life history trait continuum. Recent studies have suggested that, even in long-lived species such as turtles, personality and inter-individual differences may explain differences in behavior (Ibáñez et al. 2018; Kashon and Carlson 2018), and a great deal of recent work on POLS theory has asserted that covariance between life history, physiology, and behavior should be maintained both between species and between individuals within the same taxonomic group or species (Dammhahn et al. 2018; Royauté et al. 2018). Additional studies should explore whether life history differences among species within the same taxonomic group, such as species within the freshwater turtle family Emydidae, lead to predictable differences in risk-aversion during nesting excursions or other behaviors.

Furthermore, an important consideration in studies of POLS theory is that environmental characteristics should drive covariance in life history, behavioral, and physiological traits, as these environmental characteristics should shape adaptive responses (Ricklefs and Wikelski 2002; Wikelski et al. 2003; Dammhahn et al. 2018). Thus, individuals of the same species yet occupying different sites may exhibit different behaviors due to differing environmental characteristics and adaptive pressures (Salzman et al. 2018), and future extensions of our work could explore site-level variation in risk-aversion and nesting behavior. Furthermore, our study only considered a single physiological trait (ectothermy) and only explored whether animals behaviorally select habitat in ways consistent with their ectothermic physiology. Numerous other physiological traits, such as metabolic rates, growth rates, and immune function, should also correlate with behavior and life history in long-lived species, and further work is needed to elucidate how often this is the case (Dammhahn et al. 2018; Salzman et al. 2018; Tieleman 2018).