Introduction

As climate change and other anthropogenic factors continually alter the geographic ranges of terrestrial animals, the interactions of formerly partitioned species are anticipated to increase. These range shifts can reorganize ecosystems, causing variation in species abundance, feedback processes, and community composition (Van der Putten 2012). Examples of such shifts include the Comma butterfly and purple finch (Parmesan and Yohe 2003), both of which have shown a significant northward range shift in response to rising temperatures associated with global warming. In species such as cutthroat and rainbow trout, climate change-induced range shifts in the Western USA have brought the two species into contact, resulting in frequent hybridization (Muhlfeld et al. 2014). Hybridization among flying squirrels has similarly occurred in Canada as the southern species expands northward (Garroway et al. 2010). When newly interacting species share similar ecological roles, niche separation will likely require species to adjust their behaviors. When overlap involves reproductive systems, mating behaviors are likely to be adjusted to maintain reproductive efficiency and reduce hybridization (Lemmon 2009). This interference selects against individuals that spend time, energy, and gametes on heterospecific mates, and can be reinforced in subsequent generations by strong preference against hybrids. This process, known as reproductive character displacement (RCD), is expected to result in a divergence of communication traits in sympatric populations relative to allopatric conspecifics (Brown and Wilson 1956; Blair 1974).

The causes of RCD are not widely established, and its role as a mechanism in divergent evolution and sympatric speciation is debated (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003; Lemmon et al. 2004; Pfennig and Pfennig 2009). RCD, in theory, can be determined by the strength of selection against hybridization (through a pre-zygotic barrier), the degree of sympatry, and the relative density of each species (Gerhardt 1999; Goldberg and Lande 2006). Okamoto and Grether (2013) concluded that RCD is crucial in determining the rate of evolution, and that strong RCD increases the rate while weak RCD slows divergence. Research on this topic not only provides insight into the speed and progression of divergence but can also reveal the plasticity of certain characters. Evolutionary theory has long predicted that phenotypic plasticity could precede and promote adaptation, though the application of this hypothesis to character displacement remains under-explored (Waddington 1953; Pfennig and Pfennig 2010). In this “plasticity-first” model, displacement is expected to produce consistent patterns of evolution across multiple independent populations (Pfennig and Pfennig 2012). Moreover, plasticity is expected to drive divergence at a faster rate than genetic canalization (but see Sætre et al. (2017)).

The energetic cost and selective pressure on individual characteristics of the behavior should predict which behaviors undergo the most displacement. A behavior such as perch site selection, for example, is likely to be more plastic than a largely genetically controlled character such as call frequency, and would be expected to undergo displacement sooner (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). The importance of RCD studies, then, are their application to predicting how other species may adjust to future novel interactions.

Communication systems offer an opportunity for studying sympatric shifts in reproductive characters. Mating calls are crucial to reproduction in many species, and are often subject to RCD in the maintenance of isolation (Blair 1958). Therefore, mating signal interference between two similar species can rapidly affect the rate of divergent evolution to sustain reproductive isolation. Anurans are good candidates for the study of RCD, because there are many closely related species with overlapping ranges competing for a single parameter, acoustic space, which must be partitioned temporally or spectrally (Blair 1974; Leary 2001; Gerhardt and Huber 2002). In frogs, displacement can occur in features of the male’s call (call rate, call duration, and frequency of the spectral peaks), or other aspects of behavior (e.g., calling perch height).

Given that two interacting species are genetically compatible enough to reproduce, reduced survival and/or reproductive disadvantages of the hybrid offspring can result in increased selective pressure against mismating. For example, females of both barking tree frogs (Hyla gratiosa) and green tree frogs (Hyla cinerea) show a strong preference against the intermediate calls produced by hybrid males, which reinforces selection against mismating (Gerhardt 1974; Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). Hybrids of these two species also display reduced fitness (Schlefer et al. 1986). Direct selective pressure is more effective as a mechanism of niche separation in cases of RCD (Albert and Schluter 2004; Goldberg and Lande 2006), and the selective disadvantage of hybrid males that drives selection against hybridization has been shown to create divergence in female preference and male acoustic traits (Higgie and Blows 2008).

The speed of character divergence due to RCD remains an open question. Theoretical modeling addresses population changes in characters over simulated generations (Lemmon et al. 2004; Pfennig and Ryan 2006; Okamoto and Grether 2013). Genetic models of character displacement predict slow evolutionary rates during the earliest phases of trait shifts (Slatkin 1980; Taper and Case 1985; Doebeli 1996). While simulated displacement is often slow, recent empirical studies of living sympatric populations show surprisingly accelerated rates. For example, displacement was detected in sympatric species of mosquitoes after only 20 years of range overlap (Bargielowski et al. 2013), and studies using lab-reared populations of species in the Drosophila serrata complex have demonstrated that displacement can be detected after only 9–14 generations (Higgie et al. 2000; Higgie and Blows 2008). Vertebrates have also shown relatively rapid displacement with changes consistent with character displacement in sympatry detected following 22 years in finches (Grant and Grant 2006), 27 years in spadefoot toads (Pfennig 2003), and after 20 generation in Anolis lizards (Stuart et al. 2014). However, many prior studies of character displacement in sympatric species have been conducted in regions where the duration of overlap is unknown, and as a result, they do not address evolutionary rates (e.g., Höbel and Gerhardt 2003; Albert and Schluter 2004; Lemmon 2009).

We identified a potential case of RCD between the green tree frog, H. cinerea, and its sister species the barking tree frog, H. gratiosa, in a multi-county region of range overlap in western Kentucky where the duration of sympatry is either known or limited to a few years. Additionally, the general migration pattern of H. cinerea into formerly allopatric regions of H. gratiosa is known in this region (Lodato et al. 2015). These species are known to hybridize (Mecham 1960; Gerhardt 1974; Gerhardt et al. 1980a), and, at least in H. cinerea, have shown displacement of male call and female preference strength in syntopy consistent with RCD and reinforcement (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). In western Kentucky, H. cinerea were known to have initially invaded native populations of H. gratiosa 8 years prior to our data collection (Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources 2014; Lodato et al. 2015; J. MacGregor and B. Palmer-Ball, personal communication), and evidence suggests a continued migration since then, presenting even more recent invasions along a northwest-southeast sympatric transect.

Previous work has investigated RCD in sympatric H. cinerea and H. gratiosa (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003); however, call characters in H. gratiosa were not examined, so the possibility that RCD occurred in this species was tested. Our analysis of H. cinerea acts as a measure of the repeatability of RCD in this species. We analyzed syntopic and allotopic populations for four characteristics of male calls (high-frequency peak, low-frequency peak, call rate, and call duration) and advertising call perch height. We expected that RCD would have occurred within some of these characters and used these data to test the hypothesis that an 8-year period of overlap is sufficient for the emergence of detectable displacement of male characters.

Methods

Allotopic and syntopic choruses of both H. gratiosa and H. cinerea were studied during their breeding seasons (May–July, 2014). Data were collected from choruses within Crittenden, Livingston, Lyon, and Trigg counties in Kentucky where syntopic calling sites have previously been reported (Fig. 1) (Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources 2014; J. MacGregor and B. Palmer-Ball, personal communication). Choruses were located by road cruising and listening for active calling sites after dusk, 5–6 days a week, and mapping coordinates of sites using a GPS. It was not possible to record data blind, because our study involved focal animals in the field.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of Kentucky, USA, detailing collection sites over the five county areas surveyed, and the year of county-level invasion by H. cinerea

We defined syntopic sites as ponds where both H. gratiosa and H. cinerea were actively calling together, while allotopic sites had only one of either of the two species calling. Status of each site (syntopic/allotopic), air temperature, weather (wind, cloud cover, precipitation), and the presence of all calling species including H. gratiosa and H. cinerea were recorded upon arrival at each site. An approximately 2-min acoustic recording of the entire chorus was also taken upon arrival. All acoustic data were recorded on a Zoom H4n digital recorder using both a Sennheiser ME 62 microphone and MZW64 Windscreen with an SME PR-1000 parabolic reflector and a Sennheiser ME66 microphone with a KP-6 power module in two separate recorder channels for redundancy. Individuals in a chorus were located using acoustic cues and a headlamp before each recording. Once a calling male had been located, we advanced to within a few meters of the male, turned off all headlamps, and waited at least 3 min to allow the male to acclimate to our presence and resume calling regularly before recording with both the parabolic and directional microphones. The recording of each individual encompassed at least 20 consecutive calls. We recorded 27 H. cinerea and 17 H. gratiosa at seven syntopic sites. We also recorded 25 H. cinerea and 20 H. gratiosa at five and four allotopic sites, respectively. On average, each pond yielded usable data for four males of each species (range: one to nine males of a species per pond). One site did not clearly fit into our dichotomous variable of allotopy/syntopy. Though previously documented as being syntopic in 2006 (Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife Resources 2014; J. MacGregor and B. Palmer-Ball, personal communication), our data collection yielded only H. cinerea. Because we could not account for any character displacement occurring prior to the loss of H. gratiosa at this site, it was excluded from subsequent analyses. After recording, each individual was hand captured to take physical measurements. Mass was determined by placing an individual in a plastic bag and suspending the bag from a Pesola spring scale with a 50 g/2.5 N capacity. Snout/vent length (SVL) and tibia/fibula length (TFL) were measured using a clear, plastic ruler pressed firmly against the frog. Perch temperature was determined by placing a quick reading thermometer on the surface on which the frog was sitting immediately after capture. Perch height was recorded relative to the surface of the water. For example, if an individual was captured while floating on the surface of the water, its perch height was recorded as 0 cm.

The advertisement calls of H. gratiosa and H. cinerea are distinguishable by the rate, length, and spectral composition of each species’ call (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). The mean call duration of H. cinerea is shorter than H. gratiosa (Oldham and Gerhardt 1975; Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). The spectral composition of both species’ calls is characterized by two frequency peaks: a low-frequency peak (LFP) and a high-frequency peak (HFP). Both spectral peaks of H. gratiosa are significantly lower than those of H. cinerea (Gerhardt 1974; Oldham and Gerhardt 1975). Hybrid crosses between H. cinerea and H. gratiosa are discernable, because the frequencies of the two spectral peaks of hybrids are intermediate to those of the two parent species (Gerhardt 1974; Gerhardt et al. 1980b).

To determine if the calls of H. gratiosa were affected by the presence of H. cinerea and vice versa, the calls of H. gratiosa and H. cinerea were analyzed for temporal and spectral variation using Audacity software (http://audacity.sourceforge.net/). The calls were evaluated to determine HFP, LFP, call rate, and call duration. For each individual recorded, the same 20 consecutive calls were used in the analysis of all call characteristics to ensure consistency. Consecutive calls were typically in groups divided by long pauses. Call duration was defined as the time period from the start to the end of a single call. Call rate was the number of calls that was emitted per minute within the grouping of 20 consecutive calls. If the calls of two individuals overlapped, the owner of each call was discerned by ear and by spectrographic characteristics (one individual would have a slightly higher or lower frequency peak than the other throughout their calls, aiding in their identification) and then analyzed separately.

Call parameters may be affected by fluctuations in air temperature (Gerhardt and Mudry 1980). Consequently, we performed linear regression analyses to test if temperature significantly correlated with both frequency peaks, call duration, and call rate. Datasets found to be affected by ambient temperature (perch height and call rate in H. gratiosa; low frequency and call rate in H. cinerea) were then adjusted to a common 25 °C prior to further analysis. Though body size may also affect advertisement calls, we did not correct acoustic data for size differences in our initial analyses, because call variation due to body size would be experienced by selecting females, and these differences should be maintained in analysis. In addition, there is evidence that in some anurans, call divergence is not a result of pleiotropic size effects (Loftus-Hills and Littlejohn 1992). We did, however, analyze size differences between syntopic and allotopic populations using the scaled mass index (SMI) as a measure of body size (Peig and Green 2009), because there may be additional selective pressures on body size as a result of syntopy that are independent of female choice. The SMI adjusts for males that might be especially thin or fat for a given mass-length relationship and hence is a preferable metric for amphibians.

ANOVA was performed on call duration to determine if shifts had occurred between allotopic and syntopic populations of conspecifics. Due to normality issues (skewedness), a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test was used to test for effects of allotopy/syntopy on all of the remaining acoustic parameters. Because data on perch height similarly failed to meet normality requirements, a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test was used to determine if syntopic populations showed detectable perching differences when compared to allotopic populations. We used data from the Kentucky Department of Fish and Wildlife herpetological records, field notes of Kentucky state herpetologist John MacGregor, and personal observations to estimate county-level year of invasion for H. cinerea into our study ponds. Given a likely general northwest to southeast migration pattern, ANOVA and regression analysis were performed by comparing call data across years of observed invasion and by linear distance from our northwestern-most site.

Results

Among H. cinerea, body size (SMI) was found to correlate with both high- (F 1, 47 = 7.879, P = 0.0073) and low- (F 1, 48 = 7.698, P = 0.0079) frequency peaks, while in H. gratiosa, only perch height correlated with SMI (F 1, 32 = 6.842, P = 0.0135). Mean SMI also differed between topic populations in both species, with syntopic males being significantly smaller than allotopic males (H. cinerea; F 1, 47 = 4.905, P = 0.0317, H. gratiosa; F 1, 32 = 5.345, P = 0.0274).

Among invading H. cinerea, evidence of a significant difference in syntopic conditions was detected only for its high-frequency peak (χ 2 1 = 11.05, P = 0.0009). This consisted of an average upward frequency shift of 199 Hz (an 8.3% change). Because high frequency peaks are affected by body size in this species, we confirmed that the observed topic difference was not an artifact of size difference by performing a Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test using frequency data corrected for SMI (χ 2 1 = 4.889, P = 0.027). There was no difference between syntopic and allotopic populations of H. cinerea in low-frequency peak, call duration, call rate, or perch height (Table 1).

Table 1 Means and standard errors of reproductive characters

H. gratiosa displayed evidence of syntopic character displacement in its average call duration (F 1, 37 = 5.26, P = 0.028). This shift resulted in a call duration increase of 12 ms (a 6.7% change). Allotopic and syntopic H. gratiosa did not differ in high-frequency peak, low-frequency peak, call rate, or perch height (Table 1).

In our analysis of potential changes in behavioral traits over time across an assumed northwest to southeast migration corridor (the most likely direction of migration based on observation records and geography), no statistically significant migration trends were found (data not shown). Minimum duration of syntopy by county ranged from 0 to 8 years. Distances from the northwestern-most collection site ranged from 0 to 79 km (Fig. 1). There was no effect of distance from the assumed first site of syntopy or of the minimum number of years in syntopy for either H. cinerea or H. gratiosa on any call or behavioral characters (data not shown).

We suspected that natural variability in vegetation around chorus ponds would set stricter limits on perching behavior in some sites while allowing greater range of perching options in others. Only the dataset of perching heights for H. cinerea varied significantly by site (Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test, H. cinerea (χ 2 1 = 28.35, P = 0.0029), H. gratiosa (χ 2 1 = 22.29, P = 0.014)). To correct for the effect of variation in perch height options, a bivariate rank sum test of H. cinerea perch height by pond type was performed; sites were classified as either low (flooded fields with no brush or trees available) or high (presence of brush and/or trees). The difference between the means (62 cm) was added to all data points in the “low” category, and a final population-type comparison of H. cinerea perch heights was performed using the corrected data. Syntopy and allotopy did not differ significantly (χ 2 1 = 0.59, P = 0.44). H. gratiosa was not corrected for perch height, because there was no difference in means between “high” and low pond types (F 1, 37 = 0.0, P = 1.0). This was expected, since H. gratiosa typically float on the surface of the water while calling, so the presence of high or low vegetation around a chorus pond was unlikely to have an effect on their perching habits.

Discussion

We have shown that 8 years is a sufficient amount of time for shifts in traits associated with RCD to occur in both H. gratiosa and H. cinerea. Additionally, our results support the hypothesis that the risk of heterospecific hybridization in syntopic populations causes a shift in reproductive characteristics associated with communication relative to allotopic populations.

The effect of syntopy between these two species has been examined previously; however, the duration of syntopy was unknown and data were only presented on how the interaction influenced H. cinerea (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003). Höbel and Gerhardt (2003) found that H. cinerea had both a significantly higher LFP and calling perch height in syntopy relative to allotopy, whereas the H. cinerea included in our study had no significant change in LFP or perch height. It is possible that the 8 years of syntopy at our study sites are insufficient to cause a shift in LFP or perch height—though shifts in LFP have been documented in H. cinerea after more than 20 years of syntopy (Schlefer et al. 1986). Additionally, this may also suggest that these characters are not as plastic as we suspected or there is not enough selective pressure on these traits in our populations. Höbel and Gerhardt did not specifically present data on the effect of syntopy on H. cinerea HFP, but HFP was one of three spectral properties loaded within a principal component (PC1) for PCA. The frequency components of PC1 were found to be significantly higher in syntopic H. cinerea populations relative to allotopic populations. This is in agreement with the upward shift in syntopic HFP relative to allotopic HFP that we observed. In both our study and Höbel and Gerhardt’s, no significant difference in call duration was observed between allotopic and syntopic H. cinerea populations.

Our study had the advantage of comparing the changes observed in both H. cinerea and H. gratiosa as a result of syntopy within a known time frame (8 years). Both species exhibited a significant shift in only one acoustic characteristic during these 8 years; however, both traits shifted in a manner that increases character differences between species as predicted by RCD. H. cinerea exhibited significantly higher HFPs in syntopy relative to allotopy, whereas no significant difference in LFP was observed. H. gratiosa exhibited no change in either HFP or LFP. Low-frequency peak plays an important role in mate selection in H. cinerea, and presumably plays a similar role in H. gratiosa. Gerhardt (1976) suggests that LFPs attract female conspecifics from greater distances than HFPs, which function in more proximal species recognition. Altering HFP rather than LFP may be a means for females to distinguish conspecific males from heterospecifics while still maintaining a LFP that is able to attract females from a distance (Gerhardt 1976).

Because body size is known to be negatively correlated with call frequencies (Oldham and Gerhardt 1975), the reduced body size among syntopic H. cinerea males may be related to the observed upward HFP shift. Considering that differences in HFP between allotopic and syntopic populations exist both with and without corrections for body size, our results may indicate indirect selection for smaller males in addition to direct selection for higher HFP.

Call duration of syntopic H. gratiosa males was significantly longer than both allotopic conspecifics and syntopic H. cinerea calls, whereas H. cinerea had no significant change in call duration between allotopy and syntopy. Lengthening the duration of a call increases the likelihood that a calling male will be heard by a female (Brumm and Slabbekoorn 2005), thereby improving that individual’s chances of mating. Female preference for longer calls is common in anurans (reviewed in Gerhardt and Huber (2002)). Longer calls may provide females with a greater opportunity to assess the attractiveness of conspecifics, as well as differentiate between conspecifics and heterospecifics at syntopic sites to improve mating efficiency. Selective pressure against hybridization and sexual selection via female preference may have both contributed to the lengthening of syntopic H. gratiosa call duration.

Our data for H. gratiosa support the hypothesis that displacement in call characteristics tends to occur in a direction that increases energetic costs (Lemmon 2009). An explanation for this trend is that males with higher fitness are able to produce more attractive and more energetically costly sexual signals than less fit males. Changes in call frequency in anurans are largely dependent on the size of the larynx and not energy use (Walkowiak 2007; Gingras et al. 2013). Hence, the changes we observed in H. cinerea do not support the argument that displacement is increasing energetic costs in this species.

Call effort represents the inverse relationship between call rate and call duration (Lemmon 2009). We would expect that the increase in call duration that we observed in syntopic H. gratiosa would result in a decrease in call rate; however, no significant change in call rate was observed. Since females prefer faster call rates, it would be beneficial for males to increase call rate, though it is energetically costly to do so without concurrently decreasing call duration (Gerhardt 1994; Lemmon 2009). Considering that in our study call duration increased, it is suggestive that selective pressures on syntopic H. gratiosa in this study were great enough for the males to expend more energy by increasing call duration without decreasing call rate. Call rate may also be maintained in order to prevent auditory masking by call overlap between males (Grafe 1996), or because of the leader advantage phenomenon observed in many anurans including H. cinerea (Höbel and Gerhardt 2007; Richardson et al. 2008). Male H. cinerea have been shown to adjust the timing of their calls (and hence, their instantaneous call rate) to avoid call overlap with both conspecifics (Höbel and Gerhardt 2007), and H. gratiosa (Höbel 2015) and sympatric males are less likely to overlap their calls than allopatric males. Issues of signal timing remain unexplored in H. gratiosa.

Though we did not measure population abundance, the difficulty with which we found native H. gratiosa choruses in western Kentucky supports the suspicion that this species may be outcompeted before isolation is reached (Mecham 1960; Schlefer et al. 1986). Additionally, we did encounter a previously syntopic pond that contained only H. cinerea, representing a loss or supplanting of native frogs. Change in characters due to RCD can be asymmetric, with greater displacement found among the rarer populations (Lemmon et al. 2004). Consequently, we anticipate that in the future, relatively greater displacement will be observed in H. gratiosa in the populations we observed. Further research may clarify the capacity for RCD to prevent the elimination of behaviorally disadvantaged species.

Conclusion

Our data show that changes in behavioral characters consistent with RCD occur in syntopic populations of both H. cinerea and H. gratiosa within 8 years of initial species range overlap: an upward shift in HFP in H. cinerea, and an increased call duration in H. gratiosa. One likely reason for this displacement is pre-zygotic selection against the production of sexually unattractive and less fit hybrids, though female preferences would need to be tested to confirm this mechanism.

While our data do support RCD as the mode of action for the changes observed in these acoustic characteristics, our data also are consistent with a mechanism of behavioral plasticity, especially in those characters that are not strictly restrained by body size, temperature, or perch availability. Considering that plasticity is hypothesized to mediate character displacement during early phases of sympatric divergence, RCD and plasticity are not mutually exclusive explanations (Pfennig and Pfennig 2012). Indeed, rapid RCD driven by phenotypic plasticity (the “plasticity first” hypothesis of Pfennig and Pfennig (2012)) seems plausible for two reasons. First, the plasticity first hypothesis is supported when the changes induced by invasion of a sister species occur simultaneously in individual members of a population, as seen here. Second, it is likely that the capacity to adjust behavior to novel heterospecifics existed prior to syntopic invasion, resulting from adaptations for within-species competition. Furthermore, given that the changes we observed between recently syntopic and allotopic populations were intermediate between those observed in long established allotopic and syntopic populations (Höbel and Gerhardt 2003), we argue that the observed patterns are consistent with both RCD and explanations based on plasticity (Pfennig and Pfennig 2012). Ultimately, however, a common garden experiment will be necessary to determine to what extent behavioral plasticity may alter acoustic characteristics.