Introduction

Brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is a safe and important diagnostic imaging tool for pediatric clinical care and neuroscience research. Structural brain differences, as detected by MRI, are important indicators of the basis for child neurodevelopmental delays and neurologic disabilities [1, 2]. Furthermore, the expansion of MRI as a research technique to a variety of international settings is an important step in diversifying clinical research participant cohorts and increasing the applicability of clinical norms and references to populations around the globe [3].

Brain MRI in pediatric settings is challenging due to the need to have the child free of motion for the duration of the MRI protocol to avoid movement artifacts, which can limit interpretation of the images. In clinical settings, children often require sedation when they are too young to cooperate with staying still for the duration of an MRI, which adds a higher level of complexity to this procedure. In newborns, recent protocols have been developed which are becoming more widely used to obtain successful non-sedated neonatal MRIs during natural sleep [4]. MRIs done for pediatric research often cannot include sedation due to more than minimal risk with anesthesia, which limits its use to understand the developing brain over certain periods of child development [5, 6]. In addition, sedation, though effective in achieving MRI images of high quality, is expensive, time-consuming, and carries certain risks, even under optimal conditions [4, 6, 7]. Prior studies have suggested that there may be long-term effects of anesthesia on brain development [8,9,10]. Furthermore, the need for sedation may result in MRIs being less feasible and economically viable in settings with limited healthcare infrastructure or limited financial resources [3]. Thus, for both clinical and research scans, utilizing techniques to train children to successfully have an MRI without sedation is of great benefit.

Studies have shown that children are more likely to be successful in lying still for awake MRI scans by 6 to 8 years of age [11, 12]. Some children and even adults, however, may still have difficulties completing the MRI due to the nature of the MRI environment, including loud noises and narrow spaces that may induce claustrophobia [4, 11]. It has been shown that young children undergoing non-sedated brain MRI are most successful after they have received comprehensive training and familiarization with the MRI environment [13,14,15,16]. Previous studies have shown success rates ranging from 75 to 100% in non-sedated pediatric MRI following training with a shell mock MRI scanner [5, 6, 17, 18]; however, the definition of MRI “success” also varies across studies, as success from a clinical diagnostic perspective may differ from research study protocols that apply more stringent criteria for head motion, such as a maximum displacement parameter of ≤3 mm reported by de Bie et al. [6]. Barnea-Goraly et al. have demonstrated success using a novel, low-cost non-sedated MRI training protocol in the United States that does not require a commercial mock scanner, reporting 100% success in acquisition of T1-weighted scans and 92.5% success in acquisition of diffusion-weighted scans (defining success as completion of the entire scan and no more than mild motion artifact) [5].

In this study, we developed a low-cost MRI training protocol for use in a rural/semi-rural area of Latin America adapted from our procedures that have been successfully used in several other studies of child neurodevelopment in the United States [12]. Our objective was to assess the feasibility and success rates of our brain MRI training protocol in a typically developing pediatric research cohort of 7-year-old children from a rural/semi-rural region of Colombia. We examined factors within this sample that may affect MRI success, including associations between success in brain MRI scans and neurodevelopmental assessment scores in the domains of cognition, motor abilities, and executive function. The development of low-cost MRI training methods for children that can be used in an international setting is crucial for feasibility of global health pediatric MRI research in low-resource regions.

Materials and methods

Study sample

Fifty-seven healthy 7-year-old Colombian children in a neurodevelopment study [19] underwent MRI training in preparation for non-sedated scans. All children were Spanish speaking and resided in Colombia’s lowest socioeconomic stratum (a Colombian classification system based on residential housing characteristics including public service availability, road type, topography, and building characteristics [20]) in rural and semi-rural towns in the Municipality of Sabanalarga, in the Department of Atlántico, Colombia. These 57 participants were originally selected as non-exposed controls in a study of neurodevelopment following antenatal viral exposure; thus, they are considered healthy and typically developing children of their communities. As such, upon enrollment, all children met the inclusion criteria for the original study: they had no diagnosed medical, psychological, or behavioral conditions; no history of neurologic, hearing, or vision problems not corrected by glasses; no caregiver-reported developmental concerns; no history of physical or occupational therapy for developmental delays; and were not born preterm (≤36 weeks gestation) [19]. Children were also excluded if their mother reported exposure to an infectious disease during pregnancy (e.g., Zika, Dengue, Chikungunya) [19]. The participants were age-matched to cases at the time of evaluation and recruited from the same rural and semi-rural towns as the cases in the neurodevelopmental study via door-to-door recruitment and flyers distributed at the local schools. Upon enrollment, informed written consent was obtained from the parent or legal guardian of all participants. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Children’s National Hospital, Washington, DC and the Institutional Review Committee and the Independent Committee on Research Ethics (CIRCIE), Barranquilla, Colombia. This study is registered on ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT04398901.

MRI training procedures

Training occurred in groups of five to ten children at a community site prior to their MRI scans and was conducted by a trained research coordinator. Children were transported by a different study team member to the community site in groups and were able to watch their peers undergo the mock MRI training process (Fig. 1). The research coordinator demonstrated the scanning process using a Playmobil Radiology Playset that includes a miniature model MRI scanner and a toy “patient” and “radiologist” (Brandstätter Group, Zirndorf, Germany). As originally described by Barnea-Goraly et al. and previously used by our group, a colorful collapsible play tunnel simulated the scanning environment [5, 12]. The research coordinator guided each child through practicing lying still in the tunnel for 3 to 5 min as the child listened to typical sounds of an MRI through headphones connected to a phone or computer. A dishware container at the top of the play tunnel modeled a head coil (Fig. 1). During the practice session, the research coordinator provided gentle reminders to keep still if children moved their heads or limbs to help children understand the behavioral requirements for a successful scan. As part of the training, children also listened to a read-aloud of a customized booklet designed for the purpose of the neurodevelopment study which explained the MRI procedure and included photos of a child undergoing an MRI at the same radiology suite in Barranquilla, Colombia, where their imaging would occur (Fig. 2). Participants received a copy of the booklet to take home with them as well as a sticker reward for their cooperation. The total cost of the training supplies for all subjects was less than $100 USD.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Group MRI familiarization and training session. A group MRI training session in a community center in Sabanalarga, Colombia. A 7-year-old child lays in the play tunnel that served as a mock MRI scanner while wearing headphones to simulate the scanning environment. A member of the study team plays MRI sounds as the child lays still for 3-5 minutes. Another 7-year-old child in the training session observes the child practicing while waiting for their turn in the mock scanner

Fig. 2
figure 2

Spanish-language MRI booklet excerpts. a Cover page (translation, My MRI Day); 18 photos included in the booklet were taken at Sabbag Radiólogos, where children underwent brain MRI. (b) Sample page (translation, Now they are going to put this helmet over my head…/…and this warm blanket. I am very comfortable! I could take a good nap./I wear these headphones to hear and talk with the MRI person). Sample page explaining the steps of the MRI scan session. (c) Final customizable page (translation, What a fun day! We’ll see you on your MRI day! / My MRI day / Attach a photo here of yourself on your MRI day). The radiology team was equipped with an instant-print camera to take photos of children in the MRI suite to be added to their booklets. The full text of the booklet can be found in Supplementary Material 1

Non-sedated MRI acquisition

On a separate day, children and their parents were driven to the MRI site (Sabbag Radiólogos) in the city of Barranquilla by a study team member. Travel time was over an hour and often complicated by weather conditions such as heat and flash flooding. Two to three children were scheduled on each appointment day and were transported to the site together. Children were provided lunch and waited in a family-friendly area of the radiology center that was separate from clinical patients. Radiology staff took instant-print photos of the children in the radiology suite to be attached to their MRI booklets as part of a prize for their “MRI day,” in addition to a toy or other incentive for their participation (Fig. 2). All children were escorted by a parent (typically their mother) at all times of the MRI visit and a coordinator from the imaging center remained with the children and families throughout the entire process. Parents were invited to join their child in the scanning room during the scan with their own set of headphones on and were able to have their hand touching their child to ensure the child knew their parent was with them. Hesitant children or those who initially refused the scan were permitted to watch peers successfully complete the MRI before attempting or trying again. If a child was ultimately unsuccessful on their first scan session due to either poor image quality (attributable to excessive motion) or refusal, they had the opportunity to try again during a second scan session on a different date. If a child was hesitant or wanted to prematurely leave the scanner, they were praised for their attempt and the interaction was focused on maintaining and developing trust with the scanning team to leverage for any second attempt. Second attempts were also conducted in groups of two to three children. Mock scanner and other behavioral MRI trainings were not repeated prior to a second scan session, but all other aspects of the day of testing were the same. The length of time between training and MRI acquisition, as well as between first and second MRI attempts, varied due to radiology appointment and family availability; no minimum or maximum times were set.

Scans were acquired on a GE 1.5-T scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI) using an eight-channel head coil. The 30-40-min MRI protocol included resting-state functional and structural images. Sequences included sagittal 3D T1-weighted spoiled gradient recalled (SPGR), sagittal 3D T-2-weighted CUBE, axial resting-state fMRI, axial diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), and axial T-2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images. After 10 min of resting-state functional sequences, children watched a pre-selected movie through MRI-compatible video goggles (Resonance Technology Inc., Los Angeles, CA) during the remaining structural sequences. To help minimize motion, children were wrapped in a blanket and had foam placed inside the head coil to hold their head in place. Sequences with visible motion were repeated during the same scanning session when able. The MRI data were transferred to Children’s National Hospital, Washington, DC, where the data were reviewed for quality and interpreted by a pediatric neuroradiologist (GV).

Neurodevelopmental assessment

As a part of the neurodevelopmental study in which these children were enrolled as controls, children completed a battery of cognitive and motor assessments on the same day as their MRI training [19]. Cognitive skills were assessed using the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI), which was administered by trained pediatric neuropsychologists [21]. Full-scale IQ (FSIQ) was calculated according to the Spanish manual norms used as a measure of overall intelligence. The standard score of the Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP, the Spanish adaptation of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test) was used to determine child receptive language development. The Movement Assessment Battery for Children 2nd edition (MABC) was used for motor skills evaluation in the domains of manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance [22]. We calculated MABC overall scores as per the manual. The TVIP and MABC were administered by trained research coordinators. The Spanish versions of the parent forms Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), and Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (ABAS) were used to assess executive functioning, psychosocial functioning, and adaptive behavior [23,24,25]. The BRIEF General Executive Composite (GEC), CBCL Total Problems score, and the ABAS General Adaptive Composite (GAC) were used in analysis. Trained research coordinators assisted parents and caregivers in filling out all parent forms as needed. The age-corrected standard scores for the Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test and the Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test from the NIH Toolbox were also analyzed following administration by trained pediatric neuropsychologists.

Statistical analysis

Following each set of MRI scan sessions, MRI sequences acquired and reasons for missing sequences, if any, were documented in REDCap [26] for each participant. Neurodevelopmental assessment scores and demographic information were stored in REDCap. Sociodemographic data were summarized as means and standard deviation for continuous variables and as count and percentage for categorical variables. A successful MRI was defined as one in which the child remained in the scanner for the duration of all sequences and any motion artifacts were mild and did not detract from the diagnostic accuracy of the images. We used logistic regression to evaluate the extent to which any given variable predicts whether a subject had an MRI success on their first attempt. We used this approach since it is equivalent to using a negative binomial model for the number of attempts needed for a successful MRI scan that includes censoring for those who do not have a success. Predictor variables included sex, age at MRI, MABC overall score, TVIP standard score, ABAS GAC, BRIEF GEC, CBCL Total Problems, NIH Toolbox Flanker age-corrected standard score, NIH Toolbox Pattern Comparison age-corrected standard score, and WPPSI FSIQ. Separate models were used to evaluate each predictor variable. As a sensitivity analysis, we also examined how much results changed for each predictor variable when the following covariates were added to the model singly: sex, age, and the gap in time between MRI date and training date.

Results

MRI acquisition outcomes

Children were a mean (SD) age of 7.2 (0.2) years (median 7.2 years, interquartile range [IQR] 7.1-7.3) at the time of their first MRI attempt. Twenty-six (45.6%) participants were male. MRI training occurred between 1 and 122 days (median 21 days) before the first MRI attempt. The mean (SD) age at MRI training was 7.1 (0.1) years (median 7.1 years, IQR 7.0-7.1 years). Between November 2022 and June 2023, after a single visit 44 of 57 (77%) of children successfully completed the MRI; success rates improved to 51 of 57 (89%) children after a second visit (Fig. 3). The 13 initial failures were due to either refusal upon arrival at the imaging center (n=10) or excessive motion during image acquisition (n=3) (Fig. 3). All three children with excessive motion in their initial scans were successful in a second attempt, as were four children who had refused the first attempt. Of the remaining six children whose first MRI was unsuccessful due to refusal, two made a second attempt but did not successfully complete the MRI due to refusal, and four children declined a second attempt (Fig. 3). Second scan sessions occurred between 41 and 229 days after initial MRI training.

Fig. 3
figure 3

MRI success outcomes over two scan sessions

Factors associated with MRI success

Neither age at MRI (P=0.18) nor child sex (P=0.19) predicted whether participants successfully completed the MRI. Days between MRI preparation and first MRI appointment also did not differ between unsuccessful and successful participants (P=0.90). MRI success was not significantly associated with performance on any of the neurodevelopmental assessments (Table 1). The range of neurodevelopmental assessment scores in this sample can be found in Supplementary Material 2.

Table 1 Associations (P-values) between neurodevelopmental evaluations and MRI success

Discussion

We developed a low-cost, portable, and engaging Spanish-language brain MRI training protocol and demonstrated its success at preparing 7-year-old children from a low-resource area of Latin America for non-sedated brain MRI, with 89% overall success in usable image acquisition (defined as completion of all sequences with no motion artifacts that detracted from the diagnostic accuracy of the images) in our cohort of 57 children. To our knowledge, this is the first time that pediatric MRI success rates with comprehensive training in an international low-resource setting have been published. While it has been demonstrated that MRI training and familiarization improve pediatric MRI success rates, most studies investigating pediatric MRI training success take place in high-resource settings such as academic medical centers in the United States, the United Kingdom, Canada, and Australia [18]. Many studies also include clinical patient populations who may be more familiar with healthcare settings prior to their MRI than participants in this research cohort. To our knowledge, this is the first Spanish-language protocol that has been published. Nonetheless, the success levels demonstrated in this report are comparable to those obtained from children seen in medical centers such as Stanford University, St. Michael’s Hospital/University of Bristol, and Alberta Children’s Hospital following mock MRI preparation, highlighting the potential for low-resource clinical and research sites to achieve comparable pediatric MRI results to those achieved in a high-resource environment [5, 27, 28]. The ability to successfully harness brain MRI for clinical diagnostic purposes and for research may be particularly important in lower-resource settings with less healthcare infrastructure and where children may be at greater risk of abnormal neurodevelopment from influences such as adverse infectious and environmental exposures, nutritional deficits, and violence-related posttraumatic stress [29,30,31,32,33,34,35].

This study did not find factors that influence MRI success, which may be due to the overall low number of children that did not achieve MRI success as well as the low variability in age and some of the other variables we examined. Additionally, given that the children in this study were all typically developing controls, lower variability in neurodevelopmental assessment scores may also contribute to the lack of association observed between their scores and MRI success. Some studies that examined relationships between developmental factors and MRI success found associations between neurodevelopmental outcomes and MRI compliance [5, 27, 36, 37]. Higher cognitive assessment scores were associated with MRI success in a study of typically developing children by Thieba et al., but intelligence was not a significant predictor of MRI completion in a neuro-oncology patient population examined by Weiler-Wichtl et al. or in healthy controls or pediatric type I diabetes patients as seen in Barnea-Goraly et al. [5, 36, 37]. Parent-reported hyperactivity was found to be related to MRI success in the study conducted by Weiler-Wichtl et al., but not by Barnea-Goraly et al. or in a study of children cooled for neonatal encephalopathy and age-matched controls by Woodward et al. [5, 27, 36]. Motor ability, specifically balance, was found to be predictive of MRI success by Woodward et al., but Thieba et al. did not observe a relationship between motor scores and MRI completion [27, 37]. Other factors that have been associated with MRI success, such as memory [36], processing speed [27], and language ability [37], have not been widely studied. Furthermore, none of the aforementioned studies was conducted in low- and middle-income countries or a low-resource region outside of the United States. There are significant gaps in the literature as to how children in low-resource international healthcare settings comply with non-sedated brain MRI.

In our study, we did not find that the number of days between MRI training and the MRI was a significant factor for predicting MRI success. We had hypothesized that children whose MRI training was closer to the MRI day would be more successful, based on prior studies, but this was not shown by our data. In other studies that have examined the efficacy of MRI training and familiarization protocols, training occurred immediately prior to the MRI or a few days before [15, 28]. Thus, our observation that the time between training and scanning did not significantly affect likelihood of success is a particularly important finding. In remote or low-resource settings where appointment scheduling and travel to a hospital or imaging center may be more difficult, our findings support flexibility in the timing of training appointments and scan sessions. The variability in timing for our research participants was due to multiple factors including the COVID-19 pandemic, family schedules, MRI and research team transportation availability, and school and holiday schedules. Future studies may assess participant MRI training skills retention over time from MRI training to determine if there is a time in which participants would require MRI re-training. Future researchers interested in implementing a similar MRI training intervention may also consider conducting training on the day of the MRI to mitigate logistical challenges and standardize the consistency of time between training and MRI scan acquisition.

This study has several limitations. First, the MRI trainings and results reported here were a component of our original study designed to evaluate the impact of antenatal viral exposure on child development. This was a standalone case study of the feasibility and success of this MRI training protocol in a very specific pediatric population of typically developing 7-year-old children from rural and semi-rural regions of Colombia. Thus, all children completed the MRI training protocol, and we are unable to provide a baseline MRI success rate in similar children who did not receive the training. The imaging center where these scans were completed had never acquired non-sedated MRI images in children as young as 7 years old prior to this collaboration for research purposes, so while our low-cost training protocol resulted in high success in MRI acquisition, we are unable to compare success rates following this protocol to success rates in clinical scan acquisition in comparable children at this institution. Due to the lack of an appropriate control group for this study, we are unable to say conclusively whether we could have achieved similar rates of MRI success in this population without the use of our MRI training protocol or another preparation method. To our knowledge, there are no published data regarding non-sedated MRI success rates in 7-year-old children in Colombia, or anywhere else in Latin America, which further limits our ability to determine the impact of our training protocol on these high rates of success. We did not collect data on number of repeat sequences or study length, nor did we grade individual sequence quality; all of these would be useful metrics in future studies of non-sedate MRI acquisition. Our criteria in grading motion artifact were purely qualitative; the implementation of more precise criteria when grading image quality would also improve comparability of the results of studies such as this one. Further, we did not collect data on the success rate in this population using a commercial mock MRI scanner, as opposed to a play tunnel, since this was not available, so we cannot compare the success rates of these two approaches to MRI training. However, Fletcher et al. did not find a significant difference in success rates between mock scanners and other familiarization techniques such as play-based training in a sample of 122 children aged 3-7 years undergoing clinical MRI scans [28].

This study only conducted MRI training in a group setting, and children were always brought to the radiology center with peers, so we are unable to independently evaluate the contributions of the peer support environment and the MRI training protocol itself to the success rate of our population. It is possible that the efficacy of the MRI training protocol would have differed had the training been conducted in a one-on-one format with research staff and children, as most other studies have done. Finally, we did not collect information on the amount of time, if any, that parents or caregivers spent reviewing the “My MRI Day” booklet with their child, practicing lying still, or otherwise contributing to their child’s preparation prior to the scan, in addition to the group training conducted by the study team. Parents in our cohort have variable literacy skills [38], which may have also impacted whether children benefitted from additional preparation outside of the training session, as parents with lower literacy skills may have been less likely to review the MRI booklet with their child. We also did not collect information on satisfaction with the MRI booklet or to what extent participants found the booklet engaging. These factors may have made a difference in the success of each child and should be monitored more closely in future studies.

Future researchers can use this MRI training protocol in other low-resource settings to provide effective, easy-to-implement familiarization prior to an MRI. It is important to note that numerous studies have found age to be an important predictive feature of MRI scan success [15, 28, 39], so this protocol may need to be adapted for other age groups given that it was exclusively piloted with 7-year-olds. We did not study the efficacy of this protocol on a diverse age range of children. It is important to note that this case study presents data from a highly homogeneous, typically developing pediatric population recruited specifically to match the sociodemographic characteristics of cases in a separate study of child neurodevelopment. This protocol may require adaptation for clinical usage, as well as for research conducted in populations with different cultural, socioeconomic, or geographical characteristics, or to improve compatibility with distinct healthcare infrastructures.

Conclusion

In this case study of MRI training feasibility, children from a rural/semi-rural setting in Colombia demonstrated similar or greater success in non-sedated MRIs after a low-cost group training as compared to similar-aged children seen in high-resource urban medical centers, with 89% overall scan success. The MRI familiarization methods described in this report may help to maximize non-sedated MRI success among children in low-resource and international settings and the group format that allows for peer modeling may be an effective method for increasing MRI success across pediatric studies. Future research with a well-matched control group that does not receive MRI training, or receives a different type of training, prior to scan acquisition is needed to accurately determine the impact of this familiarization protocol on non-sedated MRI success.