Introduction

Predation plays a major role in the regulation and structuring of prey populations and communities (Connell 1961; Luppi et al. 2001). On sedimentary bottoms, most predators and prey are mobile and have developed behavioral abilities to seek prey and avoid predators, respectively. Decapod crustaceans, in particular crabs, are an interesting group of predators given their broad range of interactions and the consequences of these interactions for other species. For example, some predatory crabs have been shown to affect size structure of bivalve prey (Peterson 1982), whereas others structure communities (Botto and Iribarne 1999) or modify the distribution or behavior of individual prey, including decapod species (McDonald et al. 2001). It is well established that the habitat in which predators and prey interact has a strong influence on the outcome of these interactions (Diehl 1992; Ebersole and Kennedy 1995; Finke and Denno 2002; Hill and Weissburg 2013). In complex habitats (sea grass, seaweed, or bivalve beds, for example), prey may seek refuge from predators more easily than in structurally simple habitats such as muddy or sandy sediments. Meanwhile, predators may become less efficient at foraging for prey (Crowder and Cooper 1982).

The spread of marine non-indigenous predators provides an opportunity to study predator–prey interactions in relation to habitat (Sih et al. 2010). Native species may be naïve or unprepared to avoid or overcome mortality due to new predators, or these predators may disrupt well-established interactions that often allow native species to persist (Ricciardi and Atkinson 2004; Paolucci et al. 2013). In many examples, the introduction of a new predator has also displaced or caused local extinctions of native prey, in some cases leading to declines in overall biodiversity (Mills et al. 1993; Cohen et al. 1995; Grosholz and Ruiz 1996). In this context, it is important to gain insight into how different factors, including new predators and habitat complexity, interact to affect the outcome of predator–prey interactions.

The European green crab (Carcinus maenas Linnaeus, 1758) is an invasive species in various parts of the world including North America, South Africa, Australia, South America, and Asia. Green crabs first invaded the eastern American coast in 1817 and expanded northward to Maine by the early 1900s (Audet et al. 2003). Over the next 50 years, green crabs continued their northward colonization up to the Bay of Fundy in Canada, reaching Prince Edward Island by 1997 (Audet et al. 2003). The green crab is a voracious predator that feeds on an array of small bivalves and small crustaceans, including younger life stages of its own species (Baeta et al. 2006). Previous research indicates that native prey survival when facing a predator like the green crab will be higher in habitats that are structurally more complex (Crowder and Cooper 1982; Fernández et al. 1993; Fernández 1999; Hill and Weissburg 2013; Hernández Cordero and Seitz 2014). However, it does not elucidate how habitat will affect green crab interspecific and intraspecific prey preference and predation rates.

Several studies have shown that green crabs prey upon or displace native crustaceans from their habitat (McDonald et al. 2001; Jensen et al. 2002; Rossong et al. 2006; Williams et al. 2009). This may become the case for mud crabs (Dyspanopeus sayi Smith, 1869), a small species native to Canada that is well established in habitats like sandy and muddy sediments and oyster beds. Because mud crabs and green crabs use some of the same habitats, it has been suggested that these species may interact negatively as green crab populations continue to grow and spread (Breen and Metaxas 2009). Our own observations (unpublished) and observations of others (Lloyd 1968; Cushing 1991) also suggest that adult green crabs are predators of mud crabs and juvenile green crabs, which raises two related questions: is there evidence of a spatial/temporal negative relationship between these species? And if so, are predator–prey interactions a mechanism mediating this relationship?

In this study, we assessed abundance data from a large dataset of beach seine surveys conducted in multiple estuaries in the southern Gulf of St Lawrence over the course of five years to explore potential negative relationships between green crabs and mud crabs. We then conducted experiments that manipulate habitat complexity to investigate how habitat influences green crab predation rates and prey preferences when presented with small native mud crabs and juvenile green crabs. In addition to mortality, we assessed injury rates in order to examine the incidence of sublethal effects. Our null hypothesis was that prey mortality and injury levels would be similar between prey species (mud crab and green crab) regardless of habitat type (no substrate, sandy sediments, and oyster bed mimics). However, based on previous studies that examined the effects of habitat complexity on predator–prey relationships, we expected prey mortality to decrease with increasing habitat complexity (Crowder and Cooper 1982; Fernández et al. 1993; Fernández 1999; Hill and Weissburg 2013; Hernández Cordero and Seitz 2014).

Methods

2009–2013 Crab monitoring in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence

The relative abundance of crab species (including green crab and mud crab) was estimated in 29 estuaries of the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence using beach seine nets as part of the Community Aquatic Monitoring Program (CAMP) (Fig. 1). The CAMP program is a Citizen Science monitoring program that collects data about fish, macro-crustaceans (including crabs), and local physical parameters in estuaries of the Canadian Maritimes: New Brunswick (NB), Nova Scotia (NS), and Prince Edward Island (PEI). A detailed description of the study area and sampling methodology can be found in Weldon et al. (2005); briefly, six stations per estuary were sampled once per month during June, July, and August of each year by pulling a 30 × 2 m (6 mm mesh) beach seine in the shallow subtidal of each station. Sandy sediments and scattered oyster beds, two of the habitats represented in the laboratory experiments (see below) in addition to eelgrass beds and salt marshes, are prominent features on a large majority of these estuaries.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Outline of the study area and map of the 29 sample locations. Estuary codes are as follows: ANTI: Antigonish; BASH: Basin Head; BOUC: Bouctouche; BRUD: Brudenell; CHET: Cheticamp; COCA: Cocagne; DESA: Desable; HILL: Hillsborough; JOUR: Jourimain; LAME: Lameque; MABO: Mabou; MALP: Malpeque Bay; MIRA: Miramichi; MURR: Murray River; PELE: Cap Pelé; PICT: Pictou; PINE: Pinette; POKE: Pokemouche; PUGW: Pugwash; RICH: Richibouctou; SCOU: Scoudouc; SHED: Shediac; SHIP: Shippagan; SOUR: Souris; STLO: St. Louis de Kent; SUMM: Summerside; TABU: Tabusintac; TATA: Tatamagouche; TRAC: Tracadie

For each estuary, the number of crabs per month (regardless of size) was integrated into relative abundances of each species per summer season (year). Hence, relative abundances per species correspond to percentages over the total number of crabs (not the mean number of crabs) collected during that season. In the estuaries included in CAMP, mud crabs refer to members of the family Panopeidae. However, a large majority of these mud crabs (>95 %) belong to Dyspanopeus sayi, whereas the white-fingered mud crab Rhithropanopeus harrisii (Gould, 1841) was a considerably less common second species. In estuaries of southern PEI, where large number of samples of mud crabs were collected over three consecutive summers (2009–2011), D. sayi represented ~97 % of all the mud crabs collected (Pickering 2011).

Crab collection and tank setup for experimental work

During July and August of 2011, large adult green crabs (70–80 mm carapace width) were periodically collected from the Souris River estuary on the eastern shore of PEI (Fig. 1). In order to avoid biases associated with gender or the molting process, only intact (uninjured) males without signs of molting were retained and subsequently used as predators. Simultaneously, juvenile green crabs and adult D. sayi mud crabs (both 25–30 mm carapace width) were regularly collected from North River (Hillsborough estuarine system, southern PEI; Fig. 1). Only intact individuals of both species were retained and used as prey in the experiments described below. Habitats in which both species were collected included extensive sedimentary bottoms (particularly sandy sediments) associated with oyster, mussel, and eelgrass beds. These sites were also considerably similar in terms of water quality and tide regimes. Predators and prey were not “naïve” to each other or to the habitats used in the experiments. Experiments were run in glass tanks with dimensions 21.6 cm × 41 cm × 25 cm high, filled with prepared seawater made from chlorinated well water from the University of Prince Edward Island and Instant Ocean (25 ppt, 18–20 °C). Each tank had an oxygen stone, and its top and sides were covered to minimize external visual stimuli and prevent crab escape (Palacios and Ferraro 2003). Three distinct habitat mimics representing increasing habitat complexity were prepared in these tanks: no substrate (water only), sandy sediment habitat (tanks were fitted with a 3-cm layer of cleaned sandy sediments), and oyster bed habitat (tanks were fitted with a 3-cm layer of clean oyster shells). Although tanks with water were admittedly an artificial habitat, they provided the conditions in which prey could not physically hide from predators. As for the two more complex habitats, sandy sediments were originally collected from Brackley Bay, PEI (fine to medium sands, ~0.5–1.0 mm grain size), whereas oyster shells (Crassostrea virginica) were collected from North River (~2–4 cm SL). Before all experiments, both sandy sediments and oyster shells were repeatedly washed and filtered in order to remove any live organisms that could act as alternative prey. Water and substrate mimics were replaced after each individual experiment.

Experimental procedure

Two separate experimental designs aimed to assess predator feeding rates and preference for prey, both in relation to habitat. The first design assessed the effects of a predator (large green crab) on five small preys (either mud crabs or juvenile green crabs). The second design examined predator preference upon both prey species combined: three mud crabs and three juvenile green crabs in the same tank. For each design, 15 replicates per treatment were conducted. Individual predators were starved for 48 h prior to the experiment in order to standardize hunger levels (e.g., Mascaró and Seed 2001). In addition, new predators were used for each experiment to avoid the risk of biased results due to learning (Cunningham and Hughes 1984). Two response variables were measured: prey mortality rate (i.e., the number of individuals of each prey species that were found dead after 24 h) and prey injury rate (the number of individuals with missing claws or legs or signs of damaged carapace after 24 h). All the experiments were initiated in the morning (~10 am), and although they lasted 24 h, systematic observations were made after 0.5, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 24 h in order to identify potential trends in timing or period of most intense foraging (e.g., Pickering and Quijón 2011). We used a natural light/dark cycle, but sides and top of each tank were covered to control for most light exposure. As expected, most predation occurred during the night hours, and given the lack of any consistent trends during the first 5 h, results are reported (and statistically analyzed) for the 24-h period only.

Statistical analyses

2009–2013 Crab monitoring in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence

We explored the relationships among all crab species collected each year and from each of the 29 estuaries that had consistent records between 2009 and 2013. Data were analyzed using the multivariate method called “principal coordinate analysis” (PCoA) available in PRIMER version 6. routines (see Clarke and Gorley 2006; Anderson et al. 2008). The PCoA was used as a linear ordination of species annual relative abundances in the 29 estuaries of the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence. The PCoA creates a matrix of distances between points using the well-known Bray–Curtis similarity index based on square-root-transformed data. PCoA processes data like a principal component analysis (PCA) which entails linear combinations of the variance of multivariate data. However, unlike PCA the PCoA has the advantage of being able to use any distance measure (including Bray–Curtis) and not just Euclidean distance to identify correlations (see Anderson et al. 2008). These correlations among crab species and axes strengths are reported as Pearson’s correlations (r). The program also displays vectors that correspond to the increasing relative abundance of the crab species under analysis. The outcome of the analysis provides a visual picture of the relative similarity (spatial clustering) of data points and captures relevant between-year variation.

Mean annual abundance estimates of mud crab and green crab across the region were modeled over time using simple linear regression in Minitab 17 (2010) with the goal of identifying potential changes over time for the entire study area. In the analysis the mud crabs (Panopeidae Ortmann, 1893) species were pooled due to difficulties distinguishing them apart. Crab relative abundances were square root transformed to better meet the assumptions of linear models. For this analysis, only 21 (of the original 29) estuaries were considered, given that in eight of these estuaries there were no records of green crab invasion/establishment during the study period (i.e., for the regression analysis, n = 105). Regardless, the outcome of a parallel analysis including all 29 estuaries was virtually identical.

Laboratory experiments

For the first design (predator feeding rates on individual prey species), we used a two-way ANOVA model (two-sided test) to examine mortality and injury rates as response variables:

$${\text{Response}}\,{\text{variable}} = {\text{Prey}} + {\text{Habitat}} + {\text{Prey}}*{\text{Habitat}} + {\text{Error}},$$

where Prey (mud crab or green crab) and Habitat (water, sediment, or oyster bed) were considered fixed factors. When the interaction term was significant, pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s honest significant differences were subsequently applied to elucidate the influence of each main factor separately. The test corrected P values for multiple comparisons.

For the second design (predator feeding rates on both prey species combined, hereafter referred as “preference”), we adapted a two-way ANOVA model (two-sided test) again using mortality and injury rates as response variables:

$${\text{Response}}\,{\text{variable}} = {\text{Prey}} + {\text{Habitat}} + {\text{Prey}}*{\text{Habitat}} + {\text{Tank}}\,\left({\text{Habitat}} \right) + {\text{Error}},$$

where Prey and Habitat were considered fixed factors and Tank (Habitat) was considered a random factor. In this model, tank was nested within habitat (given that not every habitat is in every tank) as was considered the unit of replication in order to avoid potential pseudoreplication. When the interaction term was significant (i.e., effect of prey type on mortality differed by habitat type), pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s honest significant differences were applied to elucidate the influence of each main factor. Statistically significant differences for all linear models, including field and experimental analysis, were defined as P ≤ 0.05.

Results

2009–2013 Crab monitoring in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence

At least four crab species were identified from the beach seine samples: the invasive green crab and the native mud crab (primarily Dyspanopeus sayi, and potentially a small fraction of the white-fingered mud crab Rhithropanopeus harrisii), rock crab (Cancer irroratus Say, 1817), and lady crab (Ovalipes ocellatus Herbst, 1799). In the analysis of the relationships among crab species, the PCoA explained 99.8 % of the variance (Fig. 2). The first axis of the PCoA explained 75.7 % of the relative abundance in crab species and was primarily associated with the variation of green crab (r = 1.0), mud crab (r = −0.8), and rock crab (r = −0.7). All correlations in this analysis were statistically significant (P < 0.05, n = 145). The second axis explained 24.1 % of the variance and was associated with rock crab (r = −0.7) and mud crab (r = 0.6). The relative abundance of green crabs and mud crabs was negatively correlated (r = −0.8). Likewise, green crab and rock crab were negatively correlated (r = −0.7), and rock crabs and lady crabs were less strongly but positively correlated (r = 0.5) (Fig. 2). The main clustering of data points was observed across the first axis toward the right side, where green crabs were numerically dominant (Fig. 2). Toward the end of the green crab vector, a circle identifies an overlap of 60 data points, including all five annual samples collected from seven different estuaries: Antigonish, Basin Head, Cheticamp, Mabou, Murray River, Pictou, and Souris. Figure 2 also illustrates between-year variation in several estuaries. For instance, Bouctouche samples were dominated by mud crabs early during the sampling period (2009–2011) but are subsequently displaced toward the right side as green crabs enter the system and begin to dominate in abundance (Fig. 2). In contrast, samples from Malpeque Bay, an area not yet invaded by green crabs, remained associated with high densities of mud crabs, toward the left side of axis 1 (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Results of the principal coordinate analysis (PCoA) of four crab species in 29 estuaries of the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence from 2009 to 2013 (shortened to two digits). Years were removed from the encircled area due to the overlap of 60 data points, including all the points from ANTI, BASH, CHET, MABO, MURR, PICT, and SOUR and most (4 out of 5) data points from BRUD, DESA and HILL. Estuary codes are similar to those shown in Fig. 1. Mud crab refers to Dyspanopeus sayi (and to a much lesser extent to Rhithropanopeus harrisii), Rock crab refers to Cancer irroratus, Lady crab refers to Ovalipes ocellatus, and Green crab refers to Carcinus maenas

The annual green crab abundance per estuary ranged from 0 to 1048 individuals (the highest abundance for the study period was recorded in Basin Head in 2013). For mud crabs, annual abundances ranged from 0 to 1084 crabs, with the highest estimate recorded in Bouctouche in the 2010 season. Temporal trends for green crabs and mud crabs from a subset of 21 estuaries in which both species occur (Fig. 3) showed a significant increase in the number of green crabs over time (r 2 = 0.8, F 1,3 = 13.8, P = 0.034). Relative number of green crabs for the entire study area increased from 59 % to 82 %. Conversely, mud crabs did not significantly decrease over the same period (r 2 = 0.4, F 1,3 = 1.7, P = 0.279), although a slight negative trend was apparent (Fig. 3). Temporal trends using data from the full set of 29 estuaries (not shown) were virtually identical.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean (±SE) relative abundance of green crabs and mud crabs (open and filled symbols, respectively) in 21 estuaries of the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence over 5 years (n = 105 for each point)

Green crab feeding rates and prey preference

For both mortality and injury rates, the results of the two-way ANOVA model indicated that habitat type, prey species, and their interactions were all significant (P < 0.05, Fig. 4; Table 1). Post hoc Tukey’s honest significant difference tests showed that levels of mortality and injury for mud crabs were almost twice those for green crabs, except in oyster shell habitat, where they were not significantly different (Fig. 4; Table 1). Mortality and injury rates were similar across habitat types for juvenile green crabs. With regard to experiments assessing preference (Fig. 5), the results of the two-way ANOVA model indicated that for both mortality and injury rates, habitat type and prey species were significant and that their interaction was significant for mortality (Table 1). Post hoc Tukey’s honest significant difference tests showed that in general, mortality and injury values for mud crabs were twice those for green crabs (Fig. 5; Table 1). Mortality and injury values were similar across habitat types for juvenile green crabs.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean (±SE) mortality and injury rates for trials run with individual prey species (five mud crabs or five green crabs) in three habitat types (n = 15). Bars with different letters indicate a statistically significant difference (P < 0.05)

Table 1 Results from four separate two-way ANOVAs examining the effects of prey species, habitat type, and their interactions on mortality and injury rates of mud crab and small green crab preyed by large green crabs
Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean (±SE) mortality and injury rates for trial run with combined prey species (three mud crabs and three green crabs) in three different habitat types (n = 15). Bars with different letters indicate a statistically significant difference (P < 0.05). For injury rates, the interaction term was nonsignificant, so letters refer to significant differences among habitats (prey species was significant on each habitat)

Discussion

Patterns in estuaries of the Canadian maritimes

Green crab populations continue to spread and grow in some areas of the Canadian Atlantic region (e.g., PEI and Newfoundland; Audet et al. 2003; Blakeslee et al. 2010). It is in these areas where green crab populations have been hypothesized to be most aggressive (Rossong et al. 2012) and therefore most likely to negatively interact with native species. The large CAMP dataset spanning 5 years and 29 estuaries provides comprehensive evidence of a negative relationship between this invasive species and at least two native crab species, including the mud crabs (primarily D. sayi), the focus of this study. Such negative relationship, of course, may not be causally related to green crab aggressiveness or their predation on mud crabs. In fact, for a spatial scale like the one studied here, a negative relationship is most likely linked to multiple factors, related or not to predation. Negative relationships between green crabs and rock crabs have already been partially addressed in a few laboratory and field studies (Bélair and Miron 2009; Gregory and Quijón 2011; Matheson and Gagnon 2012). However, the strongest negative correlation detected here, between green crabs and mud crabs, has not previously been studied in detail. Several possible mechanisms could explain this negative relationship including but not restricted to physiological tolerance, habitat variation, competition for food or other resources, and predation.

Differences in physiological tolerance would easily segregate coastal populations and species (Hunt and Behrens Yamada 2003). However, such differences are unlikely to drive a negative relationship between these two species. Mud crab and green crab temperature, salinity, and depth tolerances are both quite broad and fairly similar to each other (Breen and Metaxas 2009). In addition, CAMP sampling sites are purposely located within a range of salinities to ensure samples from each estuary come from brackish waters and not from freshwater and marine conditions (Weldon et al. 2005). Alternatively, spatial or temporal variations in productivity or the availability of good quality habitat may easily contribute to the variation in crab abundances. For instance, species like mud crabs and green crabs use habitats like oyster, mussel, or eelgrass beds recurrently (Kneib et al. 1999), so their presence and distribution are likely critical for some stage of the crabs life cycle (e.g., Thiel and Dernedde 1994). As discussed below, oyster beds are highly structured in comparison with habitats like sandy flats and should be preferred habitat not only for the purpose of predator refuge (Grabowski 2004). Highly structured habitats embedded within more uniform sandy sediments reduce risk of desiccation during low tide conditions and may enhance local food availability (Thiel and Dernedde 1994). Oyster beds and sandy sediments are widespread in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence. Nonetheless, it is difficult to quantify their area or availability in the 29 estuaries included in this study due to the lack of consistent reports. Most likely, variations in these habitat characteristics (presence, area, distribution) would interact with slight changes in water characteristics and influence crab distribution and dynamics.

Negative mud crab–green crab interactions may be also associated with competition for food or habitats as those identified above. These interactions are common among crustaceans (e.g., Hunt and Behrens Yamada 2003) including green crabs (Rossong et al. 2006). In addition to the shallow subtidal, mud crabs thrive on mid-low intertidal oyster, mussel, and seaweed beds that are also a preferred substrate of juvenile green crabs (Day and Lawton 1988; Hedvall et al. 1998). It follows that potential interactions between small crabs of both species can occur if such beds are small, patchy, or limited in number. The likely consequence of these interactions would be displacement or local exclusion of one of the species. The same applies to potential migrations of either type of small crab into deeper waters or upper intertidal areas (beyond seine net reach) as a response to competition. A fourth mechanism to explain the negative relationship between green crabs and mud crabs is the one further addressed in this study: predation. Predator–prey interactions were expected to operate in this system given the obvious differences in size between large and small crabs (potential predator and prey), the aggressive nature of adult green crabs (Rossong et al. 2006), and anecdotic evidence suggesting a green crab preference for mobile prey such as small crabs (P Quijon, unpublished). As addressed in detail below, it is also plausible that habitat and the occurrence of green crab cannibalism events (Moksnes 2004; Almeida et al. 2011) play a role in these interactions, and that was the reason to assess both factors in our experiments.

The results of PCoA and regression analyses do not necessarily imply that predation (or for that purpose competition or habitat variation) is the factor driving the dissimilar patterns of green crab and mud crab populations. These analyses provide further evidence of the growth and expansion of green crabs in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence (Audet et al. 2003; Blakeslee et al. 2010): While the regression shows a significant increase in the relative abundance of green crabs, the PCoA provides local evidence of changes in crab composition as a result of the recent invasion of green crabs. The changes associated with this expansion are reflected in the negative relationship with mud crabs, particularly along the prominent mud crab–green crab axis of the PCoA. However, we must be cautious with regard to the interpretation of these numeric changes: Although we provide experimental evidence of one of the mechanisms by which green crab can affect mud crabs (see below), the temporal decline in mud crab relative abundance was not significant.

Predation as a mechanism to explain negative relationships between species

Our results did not support the null hypothesis that prey mortality and injury rates would be similar between species and among habitats. Large green crabs consumed and injured significantly more mud crabs than juvenile green crabs when presented with a single type of prey and when given the choice between both species. Furthermore, we detected clear mortality and injury differences among habitat types for the native mud crab.

The influence of prey

Prior studies examining brachyuran crabs as prey have already identified some of the possible mechanisms behind preference and feeding rates. Kuroda et al. (2005) suggested that differences in predation rates upon two preys were related to their different burrowing abilities to escape predation. Similarly, Kneib et al. (1999) suggested that the less preferred prey in their study was quicker and more difficult to capture than the alternative (preferred) prey. Alternative mechanisms include differences in prey palatability, caloric value, predator’s search time, and prey defensive capabilities (Ellis et al. 2012). There is no published evidence comparing palatability and caloric values of mud crabs vs. juvenile green crabs, and it would be unwise to consider them similar simply because they are of similar size. However, we do suggest that there are differences associated with predator search time and prey profitability in relation to habitat that could explain some of the preferences in our experiments (see below). Studies on the ability of prey to escape or defend themselves are uncommon in decapods, but Ellis et al. (2012) found that these two factors explained seagulls’ preference for Jonah crab (Cancer borealis Stimpson, 1859) over green crab and rock crab.

Green crabs have been found to be less susceptible to predation than other species because of their cryptic coloration against dark backgrounds like mussel beds (Dumas 1993). Although there are no studies comparing green crabs with mud crabs, the fact that green crabs are skillful at hiding (using coloration or other mechanisms) may confer an advantage. Greater difficulty in detecting prey generally translates into higher energy costs for the searching and potentially handling of that prey. If mud crabs are less skillful at hiding than green crabs, optimal foraging theory (Pyke et al. 1977) and the concept of profitability (Norberg 1977) would explain the preferences observed.

Another intuitive explanation for our results relates to inclusive fitness (sensu Schausberger 2003). In general, predators are expected to prey upon heterospecific prey rather than upon conspecifics (cannibalism). Inclusive fitness reduces the likelihood of killing related individuals (Schausberger 2003), and this should be advantageous for species undergoing population growth, as invasive species are. In our experiments, juvenile green crab mortality and injury rates were low in the “no sediment” trials where they had no possibility to hide or disguise themselves, which suggests that avoidance of cannibalism may be occurring. Cannibalism has been recorded in green crabs at low rates (Baeta et al. 2005; Ropes 1968), which suggests that cannibalism avoidance may be another possible mechanism to explain the preference and feeding rates observed in this study.

The influence of habitat

Mud crab mortality and injury rates decreased with increasing habitat complexity. This result is consistent with a well-developed body of evidence that suggests that increasing habitat complexity reduces prey mortality (e.g., Fernández et al. 1993; Dittel et al. 1996; Fernández 1999; Langellotto and Denno 2006; Stoner et al. 2010; Hill and Weissburg 2013; Hernández Cordero and Seitz 2014). The most common mechanism to explain the influence of habitat complexity is a decrease in the rate of predator–prey encounters. In complex habitats, prey may seek refuge from predators more easily, or predators may be less mobile or efficient at finding and catching prey compared with less structured habitats (Crowder and Cooper 1982; Grabowski 2004).

Surprisingly, the pattern was quite different for the other prey species; juvenile green crab mortality and injury rates were unaffected by habitat complexity. We hypothesize two opposing mechanisms that could explain this result. First, juvenile green crabs are vulnerable to predation like any other small decapod seeking refuge in complex coastal habitats (Ellis et al. 2012). However, unlike most native species, juvenile green crabs may be as good at escaping predation in less structured habitats as they are in complex habitat. A second mechanism to explain why juvenile green crab mortality levels were unaffected by habitat is the exact opposite; juvenile green crabs may lack the ability to effectively use complex habitat to escape predation. Long et al. (2015) suggested that responding to predator presence by engaging in cryptic behavior (i.e., hiding in complex habitat) may be a learned behavior, and in some crab species, refuge-seeking behavior is known to develop with size and age (Johnson et al. 2008; Stoner et al. 2010; Pirtle et al. 2012). Under this hypothesis, we might expect that ‘naïve’ juvenile mortality would indeed be similar across habitat types, while the mud crabs (already adults in our experiment) would be expected to be more experienced and have a greater affinity for hiding in complex environments. Although both mechanisms are plausible and consistent with the mortality and injury rates reported in our study, we do not have direct evidence for either, so we call for further experiments to elucidate these mechanisms.

A major implication of this study is the potential detrimental effects of the loss of complex habitats, which, according to our results, would have worse effects on native mud crabs than on juvenile green crabs. Two factors that have had adverse effects on numerous habitats in the east coast of North America, as well as other regions, are habitat destruction and invasion by the green crab. Green crabs predate upon bivalves (Palacios and Ferraro 2003; Miron et al. 2005; Pickering and Quijón 2011), including habitat-building species like oysters and mussels, leading to decreases in habitat complexity. Green crabs also uproot and graze on eelgrass (Malyshev and Quijón 2011), which has a detrimental effect on associated species and services (Heck et al. 2003). As green crabs continue to spread (Carlton and Cohen 2003), changes to native species, and in particular to habitats, will likely continue. This study documents a negative relationship between green crabs and mud crabs in the southern Gulf of St. Lawrence and provides evidence that predation is one of the several mechanisms that may explain the association. Our results suggest that if mud crabs are being displaced into habitat that is less structurally complex, their survival rates at the local scale may further decrease.