Introduction

Musculoskeletal pain remains in the top ten most debilitating conditions worldwide (Vos et al. 2016). It is well established that functional alterations occur in the central nervous system in the presence of musculoskeletal pain. For example, functional alterations can involve changes in the size of muscle representations in the primary motor cortex (M1) when the muscle is in pain (Tsao et al. 2011; Schabrun et al. 2015b, 2017a). Furthermore, there is strong evidence that experimental skin pain (Valeriani et al. 1999, 2001; Farina et al. 2001; Svensson et al. 2003) and intramuscular pain (Le Pera et al. 2001; Schabrun et al. 2017b; Larsen et al. 2018) exert a robust reduction in corticomotor excitability, as assessed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) (Burns et al. 2016). The reduction in corticomotor excitability continues for up to 25 min after perception of pain disappears (Le Pera et al. 2001; Schabrun et al. 2015a). The functional impact on, e.g., motor function by reversing this reduction in corticomotor excitability during pain is currently unknown.

The experience of pain may persist after an injury to the musculoskeletal system, and be maintained by maladaptive cortical neuroplasticity (Graven-Nielsen and Arendt-Nielsen 2010). Currently, non-invasive approaches to modulate cortical excitability are limited; however, changes in corticomotor excitability occur during acquisition of novel skills, and re-acquisition of motor skills following injury (Gallasch et al. 2009), or through repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) to the M1 or the premotor cortex (PMC) (Rizzo et al. 2004; Rothkegel et al. 2010). For instance, ballistic motor practice of the hand induces a rapid increase in flexor pollicis brevis excitability (Muellbacher et al. 2001). Yet, the interaction between corticomotor excitability reduction induced by pain and learning of discrete motor skills is controversial. Some studies have demonstrated that motor skill acquisition remains unaffected in the presence of pain (Bouffard et al. 2014; Lamothe et al. 2014), albeit different motor strategies may be involved (Mavromatis et al. 2017), while others show a reduction in the gains in corticomotor excitability that would, otherwise, occur during motor skill acquisition (Boudreau et al. 2007). Simple repetitive movements performed after experimental muscle pain did not attenuate the pain-induced reduction in corticomotor excitability (Schabrun et al. 2017b). This is surprising given that prior studies report that repeated volitional movements increase corticomotor excitability (Muellbacher et al. 2001; Carroll et al. 2008; Gallasch et al. 2009).

This study explores an alternative approach to modulate pain-induced corticomotor excitability reduction through PMC activation. The presence of a neuroanatomical pathway linking the PMC to the M1 provides a means to influence corticomotor excitability through reciprocal connections (Tokuno and Tanji 1993; Takada et al. 1998). In humans, corticomotor excitability can be modulated by PMC stimulation, by applying TMS conditioning stimuli anterior to the M1 (Civardi et al. 2001). Furthermore, inhibiting the PMC with low-frequency rTMS or continuous theta burst stimulation suppresses first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle MEPs (Gerschlager et al. 2001; Münchau et al. 2002; Huang et al. 2009). In addition, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies show strong bilateral activity of the PMC when performing working memory tasks, such as the N-back task (Owen et al. 2005).

Therefore, this study aimed to investigate if performing a working memory task during experimental pain influences the magnitude and duration of corticomotor excitability reduction. It was hypothesized that performing the two-back task during pain would (1) attenuate corticomotor excitability reduction immediately after perceived pain disappeared, and (2) reduce the duration of the corticomotor excitability reduction.

Experimental procedures

Participants

Twenty-eight right-handed, pain-free participants (mean age ± SD: 22.1 ± 2.1 years; 15 women) with no history of musculoskeletal or neurological conditions were included. Participants were excluded based on the following criteria: pregnancy; regular use of analgesics; analgesics or alcohol consumption within the last 24 h; drug use/abuse; use of antidepressants, neuroleptics, and anticonvulsants; any recent history of pain (acute or chronic) affecting the upper limbs or torso. Prior to participation, all participants were screened using the TMS screening questionnaire (Rossi et al. 2009, 2011) to avoid contraindicative delivery of magnetic pulses. All participants were right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (mean laterality quotient ± SD: 0.76 ± 0.21) (Oldfield 1971). Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. The study was approved by the local ethics committee (VN-20170006) and conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental design

Each participant participated in one session and was randomized into either a two-back task (TBT) group or a rest (REST) group (Fig. 1). Initially, participants were asked to fill out questionnaires including the pain catastrophizing scale (PCS) (Sullivan et al. 1995) and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI/S-T) (Spielbeger 1983). They were then seated comfortably in a chair with their right arm resting on a pillow. Elbow flexion was kept at approximately 45° angle flexion. A computer screen was placed immediately in front of them (mid-point of screen at 90 cm distance away from nasion of the participant). All participants underwent a familiarization round consisting of 40 trials (one letter presented = one trial; duration: 3 min) to reach ~ 80% accuracy in all trials. This was done to ensure that the participants focused on the online monitoring and updating of working memory (Owen et al. 2005) while performing the TBT. After the familiarization round, a baseline assessment was performed of the TBT, in which three rounds of 30 trials (6 min) were performed. Subsequently, baseline measures of TMS MEPs were recorded. Experimental pain was induced in the right FDI muscle by injection of hypertonic saline. Pain intensity ratings were recorded every 2 min from 30 s to 10 min after injection, and then every minute until pain-resolve (i.e. first pain rating of 0; PR). The TBT group performed the TBT for 10 min (5 rounds of 30 trials) starting immediately after the first pain rating and then one round until next pain rating, etc. Conversely, the REST group remained at rest between pain ratings for 10 min. After hypertonic saline injection, pain intensity was recorded every other minute (every minute after the first 10 min of rating). At PR, TMS MEPs were recorded again. The mind-wandering scale and effort ratings were recorded immediately after recording MEPs at PR. In both groups, a 10 min break was kept after PR and MEPs were recorded again. This was repeated at 20 and 30 min after PR after which the TBT was re-assessed in both groups (3 rounds of 30 trials). McGill’s Pain Questionnaire was completed at the end of the experiment.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Participants were randomized into a two-back task (TBT) group or a rest (REST) group. Both groups were extensively familiarized with the TBT, and baseline TBT performance and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) were recorded. Hypertonic saline was injected into the first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle. Pain intensity ratings were obtained throughout the experiment. The TBT group performed the TBT throughout the pain period, whereas the REST group remained seated quietly. Follow-up MEPs were recorded at pain-resolve (PR), PR + 10 min, PR + 20 min, and PR + 30 min. At PR + 30 min, both groups performed the TBT again

Two-back task

During the TBT, participants were presented with 30 English letters (equal 1 round) including all consonants. The letters were presented on the screen for 3 s, with interstimulus intervals of 500 ms (Vermeij et al. 2012). The participants used their left hand to press a key (i.e., using the numeric keypad 1) whenever the presented letter corresponded to the letter shown two times back (target), or (numeric keypad 2) when it did not match (non-target). Therefore, each letter presented would be accompanied by a button pressed and reaction time and accuracy (i.e., correct or incorrect keypad pressing) were recorded. For each round, 20 non-targets and 10 targets were presented in a random order. Reaction time reflects the time from letter presentation on the screen, to the key press, whereas accuracy is whether the correct button was pressed. The left hand was chosen to avoid on-going ipsilateral motor activity (associated with keypad pressing) of the left M1 affected by pain, which could potentially interfere with the reduction of corticomotor excitability.

Questionnaires

To ensure that the pain reporting was not influenced by cognitive factors such as pain catastrophizing (Turner and Aaron 2001), state and trait anxiety (Tang and Gibson 2005), and mind-wandering (Kucyi et al. 2013), the PCS, STAI, and mind-wandering scale were recorded for each participant. The 13 items of the PCS are categorized into “Rumination” (sum of items 8–11), “Magnification” (sum of items 6, 7, and 13), and “Helplessness” (sum of items 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, and 12) (Sullivan et al. 1995). For each statement, the participants reported the degree to which the sentence corresponded to their own thoughts and feelings when experiencing pain (not at all, to a slight degree, to a moderate degree, to a great degree, and to a large degree). The anxiety score was determined by the STAI (Spielbeger 1983) consisting of 40 questions, half of which are related to state anxiety, and the other half to trait anxiety. Each statement was rated on a 0–3 scale with anchors ‘Almost never’ to ‘Almost always’. The mind-wandering scale was adopted from Kucyi et al. (2013), and consisted of four questions designed to determine the level of mind-wandering occurring during the TBT performance or during rest. Participants were asked “How confident are you that you can accurately assess whether your attention was focused on the task/staying at rest, or on something other than the task/staying at rest; To what degree was your attention on one of the following when not on the task/staying at rest, (1) external/sensory distractions, (2) task-related interferences, and (3) mind-wandering”. Each question was rated on a 0–7 Likert scale ranging from 1 (not confident at all/never) to 7 (extremely confident/always). In addition, the participants were asked for the level of effort which they had to put into the task (TBT) or to staying at rest (REST) during pain (1, no effort; 7, maximum effort). To characterize the experimental pain condition, the short-form McGill Pain Questionnaire-2 (MPQ) was completed at the end of the experiment (Dworkin et al. 2009). The most commonly used words to describe the experimental pain were extracted.

Recording motor-evoked potentials

All TMS methods will be described in accordance with guidelines on reporting TMS methodology (Chipchase et al. 2012). A focal figure-of-eight coil (D702, Magstim Company, UK) was used to deliver monophasic TMS pulses supplied by a magnetic stimulator (Magstim BiStim2, Magstim Company, UK). The coil handle was pointing backwards, laterally, and at a 45° angle to the sagittal plane, inducing a posterior–anterior directed current, to elicit TMS MEPs from the FDI muscle. An interstimulus interval of 6 s was used for all the stimulations. The participants were fitted with a swimming cap and the optimal scalp position was marked on a pre-defined grid (1 × 1 cm squares orientated to vertex) to standardize orientation and location. The optimal scalp position (hotspot) for the FDI muscle was determined using 50% maximum stimulator output and defined as the site yielding consistent and highest peak-to-peak amplitude MEPs in three trials. The intensity needed to evoke MEPs of ~ 1 mV was tested by increasing and decreasing stimulus intensity until ~ 1 mV was consistently evoked in the FDI muscle (in 10 trials). This stimulation intensity was employed for the remaining of the experiment. For each assessment, 15 MEPs were recorded from the right FDI muscle.

Surface electromyography (EMG) was recorded from the muscle belly of the FDI muscle using bipolar Ag/AgCl electrodes (Neuroline 720, Ambu® A/S, DK). Electrodes were placed with an approximate 20 mm interelectrode distance with the reference electrode located at the styloid process. The EMG data were pre-amplified (1000 × gain), analog band-pass filtered (5 Hz–1 kHz), and sampled at 4 kHz by a 16-bit data-acquisition card (National Instruments, NI6122). Peak-to-peak MEPs were shown online by custom-made LabView software (Mr. Kick III, Aalborg University). Peak-to-peak amplitude was extracted for each MEP and averaged across the 15 recorded MEPs at each time point for further analysis.

Experimental muscle pain

The sites of injections were determined by palpation of the contracted FDI muscle, and the skin was cleaned with alcohol. A bolus injection of sterile hypertonic saline (0.2 mL, 5.8% NaCl) was administered into the FDI muscle using a 1 mL syringe with a disposable needle (27 G) (Le Pera et al. 2001; Larsen et al. 2018). The right FDI was chosen due to the body of evidence already available on the effect of pain on FDI M1 excitability (Le Pera et al. 2001; Schabrun et al. 2013; Larsen et al. 2018, 2019). Hypertonic saline excites group III/IV muscle afferents (Cairns et al. 2006) and an earlier study demonstrated that hypertonic saline injection temporally reduces primary somatosensory cortex (S1) excitability before M1 excitability (Schabrun et al. 2013). To assess the intensity of saline-induced pain, participants were asked verbally to rate the pain intensity on a 0–10 numerical rating scale (NRS), with ‘0’ representing ‘no pain’ and ‘10’ representing ‘worst imaginable pain’.

Statistical analysis

Data are presented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) unless otherwise stated. Normal distribution was tested using Shapiro–Wilk’s test for normality. Participant demographics were compared between groups using independent samples t tests (age, handedness, and threshold1mV) and Chi-square test (gender ratio). The PCS, STAI-S/T, MPQ, and mind-wandering scale data were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U tests. The MEPs were tested in a two-way mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (TBT or REST) as between-groups factor and time (baseline, PR, PR + 10 min, PR + 20 min, and PR + 30 min) as within-subjects factor. NRS scores were analyzed with a two-way mixed-model ANOVA with group (TBT or REST) as between-groups factor and time (30 s after injection and 2–17 min) as within-subjects factor. The TBT performance data were analyzed by two separate repeated-measures multivariate ANOVAs (one for targets and one for non-targets; MANOVA), with two dependent variables (accuracy and reaction time), one within-group factor (baseline versus PR + 30), and one between-group factor (TBT versus REST). The MANOVAs for target and non-target were corrected by familywise error rate correction (0.05/2 = 0.025). Note that the TBT performance data obtained during pain are not included in these analyses, since the REST group did not perform the TBT during pain. Spearman ranked correlation analyses were performed to test whether there were associations between the pain intensity NRS rating and percentage MEP reduction at peak pain for both the REST and the TBT group. Sidak correction was applied where appropriate. All analyses were carried out in Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS; version 25, IBM). A p value < 0.05 was considered significant.

Results

Participants and TMS parameters

The two groups did not differ in age [22.7 ± 0.6 years vs. 22.9 ± 0.6 years, t(26) = − 0.18, p = 0.86], handedness [laterality quotient, TBT group: 0.76 ± 0.06 vs. REST group: 0.75 ± 0.05, t(26) = 0.09, p = 0.93], or gender ratio [9/14 women in TBT group vs. 6/14 in REST group, χ2(1) = 1.29, p = 0.26]. The stimulator output intensity needed to produce MEPs of ~ 1 mV amplitude was 55.3 ± 14.3% in the TBT group which was not statistically different from the 57.5 ± 11.7% in the REST group [t(26) = − 0.43, p = 0.67].

Pain catastrophizing and anxiety questionnaires

The median PCS score for the TBT group was 16.5 [interquartile range (IQR) = 22.75] and 13.5 (IQR = 8.5) for the REST group, which did not differ significantly, U = 92.5, n1 = n2 = 14, z = −0.25, p = 0.8. Similarly, there were no differences between groups for ‘Rumination’ scores [TBT versus REST: 6 (IQR = 9.75) vs. 5 (IQR = 4.5), U = 91, n1 = n2 = 14, z = − 0.32, p = 0.75], ‘Magnification’ [3 (IQR = 6) vs. 3.5 (IQR = 4.5), U = 92, n1 = n2 = 14, z = − 0.28, p = 0.78], or ‘Helplessness’ [4.5 (IQR = 8.5) vs. 6 (IQR = 5.25), U = 89.5, n1 = n2 = 14, z = − 0.39, p = 0.69]. Similarly, no significant differences between groups were found for the STAI-S [TBT versus REST: 31.5 (IQR = 10.5) vs. 36 (IQR = 9); U = 71.5, n1 = n2 = 14, z = − 1.2, p = 0.22] or STAI-T [40.5 (IQR = 16.25) vs. 35 (IQR = 16.5); U = 86, n1 = n2 = 14, z = − 0.6, p = 0.6].

Mind-wandering scale

One participant from the REST group had missing data from the mind-wandering scale and was excluded from the analysis. The groups did not differ in their confidence in assessing their level of attention towards the task/staying at rest (TBT median: 6 versus REST median: 6, U = 64, n1 = 14, n2 = 13, z = − 1.36, p = 0.17), and did not rate sensory or mind-wandering interference with their attention to either task differently (TBT median: 4 and 3 versus REST median: 4 and 4, both U > 74, n1 = 14, n2 = 13, z > − 0.27, p > 0.4). Conversely, the TBT group rated the task-related interferences with their attention to the task higher than the REST group did (TBT median: 2 versus REST median: 0, U = 0.0, n1 = 14, n2 = 13, z = − 4.72, p < 0.005). Furthermore, the TBT group rated their effort higher than the REST group, when performing their respective task (i.e., performing the TBT as opposed to staying at rest for the REST group) (TBT median: 5 versus REST median: 2, U = 26.5, n1 = 14, n2 = 13, z = − 3.17, p = 0.001).

Pain intensity ratings and profile

A significant two-way interaction was found for the pain intensity NRS scores (F12,338 = 2.3, p = 0.008, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.08). The REST group gave NRS scores lower than the TBT group at 4 min post-injection (p = 0.009; Fig. 2). Pain reduced significantly for both groups at 8 and 10 min compared to immediately after (I.A.) injection, for the group performing the TBT and the REST group, respectively (p < 0.05, Fig. 2). For all pairwise comparisons, refer to Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean (+ SEM) numerical rating scale (NRS) scores following injection of hypertonic saline in the two-back-task (TBT) and resting (REST) groups. NRS scores were significantly reduced in both groups from 8–17 or 10–17 min (TBT and REST, respectively) compared to I.A. (p < 0.05). All significant NRS rating reductions as compared to 2–8 min are indicated in the figure. A between-group difference in NRS was found at 4 min (#p < 0.05). PR pain-resolve

The most common words to describe the hypertonic saline-induced pain sensation in both groups were sharp (82%), cramping (85.7%), aching (75%), heavy (67%), and numbness (67%).

Corticomotor excitability

The two-way mixed-model ANOVA of MEPs did not yield a significant group × time interaction (F4,104 = 0.51, p = 0.73, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.19) or between-group difference (F1,26 = 0.7, p = 0.4, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.03), but a strong main effect of time (F4,104 = 8.3, p < 0.0005, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.24). Post hoc tests showed that corticomotor excitability of the FDI muscle was reduced at PR (p = 0.002) as well as PR + 10 min (p = 0.002), PR + 20 min (p = 0.008), and PR + 30 min (p = 0.002) as compared to baseline (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean (+ SEM) motor-evoked potentials (MEPs). No significant differences in MEP reduction between the two groups were found. At PR up until PR + 30 min, MEPs were reduced for both groups (*p < 0.008). PR pain-resolve

Two-back task performance

One and two participants in the TBT and REST group, respectively, had missing data during the performance of the TBT. Therefore, their data were omitted during the analysis of the TBT performance (TBT group, n = 13; REST group, n = 12).

The repeated-measures MANOVA did not yield any time × group interactions for non-target accuracy (F1,20 = 0.21, p = 0.65, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.01; Fig. 4a) or reaction time (F1,20 = 0.2, p = 0.66, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.01; Fig. 4b). Similarly, target accuracy (F1,20 = 0.01, p = 0.94, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) < 0.001; Fig. 4c) and target reaction time (F1,20 = 0.03, p = 0.86, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.002; Fig. 4d) did not show any time × group interactions. A main effect of time was found for target accuracy (F1,20 = 6.55, p = 0.019, \(\eta_{\text{partial}}^{2}\) = 0.25), indicating an increase in accuracy between baseline and PR + 30 (Fig. 4c).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean (+ SEM) target and non-target accuracy and reaction time for the two-back-task TBT) and resting (REST) groups. A main effect of time (baseline versus PR + 30 min) was found for target accuracy (*p = 0.019), suggesting that both groups had a similar increase in accuracy on targets

Correlation analyses

No significant correlations were found for the percentage change in MEP amplitude and peak NRS scores of pain intensity for the REST group (ρ = − 0.15, p = 0.6) or the TBT group (ρ = − 0.4, p = 0.89).

Discussion

There is growing interest to find approaches capable of attenuating the pain-induced reduction in corticomotor excitability, since it may prove useful during musculoskeletal pain rehabilitation (Pelletier et al. 2015). The present findings confirm earlier findings of a robust reduction in FDI corticomotor excitability following experimental muscle pain. The pain-induced reduction in MEPs was unaffected by the TBT performance. The TBT group rated higher pain during the performance of the TBT as compared to the group that remained at rest.

Corticomotor excitability reduction was unaffected by performance of the two-back task

Contrary to our hypotheses, the pain-induced reduction in corticomotor excitability and the duration of this reduction were unaffected by performing a working memory task during acute experimental pain. As previously reported (Le Pera et al. 2001; Schabrun and Hodges 2012; Larsen et al. 2018), hypertonic saline-induced pain reduced FDI corticomotor excitability, as reflected by MEP amplitudes, even after pain-resolve. This reduction in corticomotor excitability is considered to be mediated through an increase in gamma-aminobutyric acid inhibitory and decreased glutamate-mediated facilitatory (N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor acting on glutamatergic interneurons) intracortical mechanisms (Schabrun and Hodges 2012), and may serve as a protective mechanism for avoiding further injury by splinting the affected limb (Hodges and Tucker 2011; Burns et al. 2016). The current study aimed at targeting the link between PMC and M1, since the TBT is known to engage prefrontal and premotor cortices during performance (Owen et al. 2005). However, our findings suggest that more research is needed to establish if engaging the PMC by performing the TBT affects M1 excitability, given the lack of a pain-free TBT group. Several factors influence the corticomotor excitability response to pain, such as (1) prefrontal and subcortical brain areas (Mink 1996; Owen et al. 2005; Seminowicz and Moayedi 2017); (2) right PMC-to-contralateral M1 inhibition (Mochizuki et al. 2004); or (3) transcallosal M1–M1 inhibition (Ferbert et al. 1992). It is, therefore, not possible to rule out if performing the task with the left hand may have induced either right PMC-to-M1 or M1–M1 inhibition, thereby counteracting any potential facilitation of M1 excitability. However, considerable bilateral PMC activation has been demonstrated by fMRI across several n-back task paradigms (Owen et al. 2005), and PMC-to-M1 inhibition is, therefore, unlikely to be the sole responsible factor for the lack of MEP facilitation. Instead, the human PMC–M1 relationship remains somewhat elusive, and the notion that PMC may drive M1 excitability is based on empirical evidence, suggesting that rTMS to PMC alters M1 excitability (Gerschlager et al. 2001; Mochizuki et al. 2004; Rizzo et al. 2004). In addition, since M1 receives multiple inputs from, e.g., the S1 (Hatsopoulos and Suminski 2011), it is possible that MEPs are not only reflecting changes at the level of M1. This notion is supported by the finding that S1 excitability changes occur before that of M1 excitability (Schabrun et al. 2013).

Another possibility is that the PMC activation observed during performance of a two-back task is mainly related to attention (Owen et al. 2005). Yet, given the strong motor preparation component of the TBT, prefrontal areas such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), the PMC, and M1 are likely involved (Carlson et al. 1998; Neige et al. 2018), and being purely attention-related activation is unlikely to be the main factor in the lack of M1 excitability change during pain. Based on the current findings, we are unable to infer whether performance of the working memory task influences corticomotor excitability. It is possible that the familiarization and baseline testing on the TBT had already increased MEPs during baseline TMS testing. However, the numerical values of the MEPs were similar to those reported in earlier research utilizing 120% RMT (Schabrun and Hodges 2012; Larsen et al. 2018). Furthermore, the pain-induced reduction in MEPs was similar to those earlier reported (Le Pera et al. 2001; Schabrun and Hodges 2012; Burns et al. 2016; Larsen et al. 2018). Therefore, the familiarization and baseline TBT testing are unlikely to have had any major impact on the MEPs.

Modulating corticomotor excitability through the PMC is an interesting approach, as shown in the other areas of research on action observation (Fadiga et al. 1995; Strafella and Paus 2000) and motor imagery (Stinear and Byblow 2003). Perhaps, these approaches may be more effective for normalizing pain-induced corticomotor excitability reduction.

Pain intensity ratings differed between groups

In this study, those performing the working memory task rated their perceived pain intensity higher than the resting group during pain. In contrast, pain intensities are rated lower during performance of an attention task (Bushnell et al. 1985; Miron et al. 1989), a Stroop task (Bantick et al. 2002; Seminowicz and Davis 2007a), and a three-back task (Buhle and Wager 2010) in previous studies. A reduction in pain intensity during task performance is considered to be mediated through attenuation of activation of pain-related brain regions such as the S1, S2, and the posterior insula, and the ipsilateral (to the pain) anterior insula (Petrovic et al. 2000; Bantick et al. 2002; Seminowicz et al. 2004; Wiech et al. 2005; Bingel et al. 2007). The discrepancy between the current and previous studies in pain ratings may depend on the choice of experimental pain model. Phasic short-lasting (0.8–7 s application) heat pain models applied to the face, the hand, and the left forearm (Bushnell et al. 1985; Bantick et al. 2002; Buhle and Wager 2010) yield moderate pain intensities (4–7) scored on a visual analog scale, whereas hypertonic saline-induced pain in the hand is rated at ~ 6 NRS in this study and earlier work (Larsen et al. 2018). It is, therefore, unlikely that pain intensity alone can explain the discrepancy. Alternatively, performing the task during pain may have shifted the attention towards pain. However, we find this unlikely since (1) the two groups did not rate external/sensory distractions differently with respect to task execution and (2) earlier evidence suggests that task performance remain unaffected when performed during pain (Seminowicz and Davis 2007b). Instead, the difference in quality of the painful sensation, i.e., tonic muscle pain versus phasic skin pain, may impact pain perception during the working memory task performance. Indeed, capsaicin application to the skin before painful heat stimulation (heat allodynia) enhances forebrain responses related to areas such as the anterior insula and dorsolateral prefrontal areas, when compared to similar pain intensity heat stimulation alone (Lorenz et al. 2002). It could be argued that the two groups were different because of interindividual susceptibility to pain perception, given the sizeable heterogeneity in pain ratings amongst healthy volunteers to standardized nociceptive input (Coghill 2010). Although we cannot rule out the possibility of group differences in pain susceptibility, the groups did not differ in age, gender, PCS, or STAI-S/T scores.

Accuracy in the two-back task increased at PR + 30 as compared to baseline

The performance increase in target accuracy was similar for the TBT and REST group. This verifies that no additional learning occurred at PR + 30 for the TBT group compared to the REST group, despite having performed the task for an additional 10 min. This supports the notion that the TBT during pain can be considered a working memory task. The similar increase in target accuracy is plausibly due to the familiarization performance, where the two groups achieved a similar level of target accuracy during the 40 trials. The aim of the current study was to employ the TBT to influence M1 excitability indirectly by engaging the PMC and DLPFC (Owen et al. 2005) and not induce learning per se. Since earlier research showed performance gains in N-back tasks (accuracy and reaction time) in response to facilitated DLPFC excitability by high-frequency rTMS [for review, see Brunoni and Vanderhasselt (2014)], the present findings support that a working memory task that engages the DLPFC, improves accuracy on targets, but corticomotor excitability remains unchanged. The increased accuracy from baseline to PR + 30 is in agreement with earlier lines of evidence on motor learning, which demonstrated that performance still increases when motor practice is performed during pain (Boudreau et al. 2007; Lamothe et al. 2014; Bouffard et al. 2016; Mavromatis et al. 2017).

The lack of facilitation of M1 could be explained by the effort needed to perform the working memory task during pain. It is known that, for instance, motor tasks need to be at a certain level of difficulty for learning to be optimal and engage M1 (Boudreau et al. 2010). Participants in the TBT group consistently rated the effort to complete the TBT higher during pain, but effort is not a direct measure of task difficulty. It, therefore, remains unclear if the task was able to modulate corticomotor excitability, since a TBT group without pain was not included. Nonetheless, it is known that increased task difficulty enhances corticomotor excitability to a greater extent during performance of, e.g., motor imagery (Roosink and Zijdewind 2010), and task difficulty in the current study should therefore be considered.

Limitations

The lack of a TBT group with no pain during performance makes it difficult to assess if the TBT influences corticomotor excitability when performed during pain. Earlier studies where pain-free participants were tested have consistently demonstrated that isotonic saline and no pain perception do not interfere with the magnitude of MEPs (Le Pera et al. 2001; Svensson et al. 2003; Schabrun et al. 2015a), but there are no equivalent data on the TBT alone. The lack of a pre-familiarization TMS baseline does not allow for the relative impact of the familiarization/baseline n-back task performance on MEPs to be assessed.

This is the first study to explore if performing a working memory task modulates the pain-induced reduction in corticomotor excitability. Pain-induced reduction in FDI corticomotor excitability was unaffected by engaging in a working memory task during pain, despite an enhanced pain perception.