Introduction

Research has indicated that up to 8% of the general US population meet the criteria for an alcohol use disorders (AUD) (Grant et al. 2004). Because relapse is often the main characteristic of alcohol dependence, relapse prevention is critical for long-term treatment strategies. In fact, outcome studies establish the adverse effects of comorbid anxiety disorders in alcoholism and highlight the complication of the problem. For example, both AUD and generalized anxiety disorders (GAD) are highly prevalent in the general population (Hasin et al. 2007). In addition, the association between these two disorders has been well documented in previous studies (Conway et al. 2006; Grant et al. 2004). Because alcoholism is highly comorbid with stress-induced social anxiety, and because adolescence represents a period of significant plasticity in the mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic system, stress exposure during this period may have unique long-term effects on reward responsivity. For instance, when discrete-time survival analysis (DTSA) was used to examine whether real-time social stress responding and social anxiety symptoms predicted 12-month alcohol onset in an alcohol-naïve sample of young female adolescents, authors reported that girls with higher levels of social anxiety, compared with baseline, had earlier alcohol initiation (Cloutier et al. 2019), suggesting that, at least in adolescent girls, increased sensitivity to social stress might be correlated with earlier alcohol drinking.

There is strong evidence, from several preclinical studies, that social stress is a strong trigger for relapse and an important factor contributing to increased motivation for alcohol consumption in rodents’ laboratory animals. For example, when oral operant conditioning paradigm was used to assess the effects of repeated social defeat during adolescence on the reinforcing and motivational actions of EtOH in adult male open field (OF)1 mice, Rodriguez-Arias and colleagues reported that, while anxiety-like behavior in the elevated plus maze (EPM) test was not altered, social defeat in adolescence significantly increased EtOH consumption and motivation to drink (Rodriguez-Arias et al. 2016). Similarly, when outbred Carworth Farms Webster (CFW) male mice were socially defeated for 10 days, results have shown that EtOH consumption and preference following moderate stress were higher (13.3 g kg−1 day−1 intake) than both the mild stress group (8.0 g kg−1 day−1) and controls (7.4 g kg−1 day−1) (Norman et al. 2015), with the moderately stressed mice displaying escalated EtOH intake and self-administration during a schedule of intermittent access to alcohol (Norman et al. 2015). The same research group also reported an increase in voluntary EtOH consumption triggered by a history of social defeat stress and intermittent access to EtOH in male C57BL/6J mice compared with nonstressed mice (Hwa et al. 2016; Newman et al. 2018). Similarly, stressed adult male C57BL/6 mice increased their consumption of sweet solution after ten sessions of social defeat and exhibited reduced activity in the open arms of the EPM test that was associated to a significantly persistent increase of EtOH intake compared with controls (Albrechet-Souza et al. 2017). Male and female C57BL/6J mice exposed to adolescent chronic variable social stress (CVSS) showed significantly increased adult anxiety-like behavior and drank significantly more EtOH than controls (Caruso et al. 2018). Taken together, these findings indicate that, at least in different strains of mice, prior exposure to social stress is an important risk factor for later excessive alcohol intake and affective disorders’ alterations such as anxiety. Although it is obviously recognized that there is a strong association between stress-induced anxiety and excessive alcohol intake, the etiological nature of this relationship is still not fully understood and available pharmacological treatments for alcoholism have only modest long-term efficacy (Heilig et al. 2011). This prompts the question of whether pharmacotherapy can be used to tackle alcoholism. Therefore, environmental enrichment (EE) has been proposed as an alternative protective behavioral treatment for alcohol rehabilitation (for review, see Pang et al. 2019).

In studies involving laboratory animals, the nature of enrichment differs, but classically involves access to larger, more motivating/stimulating settings in cages larger and more “attractive” than those regularly used in the standard conditions, with a variety of sensory, cognitive, and motor stimuli to increase opportunities for voluntary physical activity (Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006; van Praag et al. 2000). Results have shown that exposure to EE results in significant effects on brain morphology and physiology, enhancing neural plasticity in multiple brain regions (for review, see Sale 2018; van Praag et al. 2000) and exerting positive effects in animal models of brain injury and neurodegenerative disorders (Nithianantharajah and Hannan 2006). In addition, EE have been shown to decrease reactivity to stress as it restored hippocampal cell proliferation and ameliorated cognitive deficits in chronically stressed rats (Veena et al. 2009), with enhanced synaptic plasticity and learning, as well as memory (Arai and Feig 2011; van Praag et al. 2000). Finally, there is compiling evidence that EE has the capacity to positively affect drug-seeking behavior. In fact, the rewarding effects of cocaine were inhibited in mice reared from weaning to adulthood in EE compared with mice reared in standard environments (SE), with the mice in EE showing less activation in response to repeated cocaine administration and reduced responses to cocaine challenges (Solinas et al. 2009). The same group also reported that EE (30 days) prevented the development of cocaine-induced behavioral sensitization, CPP acquisition, as well as cocaine-induced CPP reinstatement (Solinas et al. 2008). Also, heroin-induced CPP but not behavioral sensitization was reduced in EE mice compared with SE mice (El Rawas et al. 2009). However, and contrary with what was reported with other drugs such as cocaine and heroin, EE was unable to reduce the rewarding effects of methamphetamine (Thiriet et al. 2011).

In a previous study from our laboratory, we reported that mice exposed to chronic psychosocial stress showed delayed ethanol-conditioned place preference (EtOH-CPP) extinction and increased alcohol-induced reinstatement of the EtOH-CPP behavior (Bahi and Dreyer 2014). We have also reported that EE reduced anxiety-like behavior that was associated with decreased voluntary EtOH consumption, as well as reduced of EtOH-CPP acquisition (Bahi 2017b). Others have shown that priming-induced reinstatement of EtOH-CPP was blocked by EE (Li et al. 2015). To the best of our knowledge, no previous study has examined the efficiency of EE at accelerating stress-delayed extinction and reinstatement of CPP for EtOH, so no direct comparison of the effectiveness of standard versus enriched environmental conditions on extinction and reinstatement of CPP has been attempted. We hypothesized that EE might attenuate/blunt chronic psychosocial stress-delayed EtOH-CPP extinction (1st experiment) and that EE would protect from stress-induced EtOH-CPP reinstatement (2nd experiment).

Materials and methods

Animals

Male C57BL/6 mice (6–7 weeks old) weighing approximately 22 g (experimental mice) or adults (resident mice) weighing 35–40 g were housed in standard Plexiglas observation cages. All mice were of identical genetic background and back-crossed to the parental inbreed mice each ten generations. All mice were bred locally and were kept under standard laboratory conditions (12/12 h light–dark cycle, lights off at 6 p.m., ~ 22 °C). The EtOH-CPP procedure and the anxiety-like behavior tests were performed during the light phase. A large number of published reports used the same lighting conditions with the housing room kept under a regular light/dark schedule (Lopatina et al. 2014; Vahid-Ansari et al. 2017). Bedding was autoclaved before use, and mice had free access to tap water and standard mouse chow diet obtained from the National Feed and Flour Production and Marketing Company LLC (Abu Dhabi, UAE). The chronic psychosocial stress procedure was approved by the local Animal Research and Ethics Committee (Protocol No. A01-12).

Drugs

EtOH (20%, v/v) was diluted in physiological saline and injected intraperitoneally (i.p.) during conditioning and reinstatement (2 g kg−1). EtOH doses were selected based on our previous studies (Bahi 2012, 2015; Bahi and Dreyer 2014).

EtOH-conditioned place preference

The general procedure of the EtOH-CPP test was conducted as described in our previous studies with minor modifications (Al Ameri et al. 2014; Al Maamari et al. 2014; Al Mansouri et al. 2014; Bahi 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2012b). In brief, eight identical wooden two-chambered CPP apparatus (30 × 30 × 30 cm3) with distinct visual and tactile cues and separated by a removable guillotine door were used. Each CPP test consisted of preconditioning (pre-CPP, day 1), conditioning (days 2–6), and postconditioning (post-CPP, day 7). Lighting within the behavioral chambers was provided by overhead lights in each of the testing compartments each measuring approximately 15 Lux at the testing floor.

During preconditioning (pre-CPP), the initial place preference was determined after each mouse was placed in the middle of the CPP box (facing the frame of the guillotine door) with 15-min free access to both compartments and the time spent in each compartment was scored. This session determined baseline preferences (whether the mouse spent significantly less/more time in one compartment than in the other, regarded as equipment bias). Mice that spent less than 35% and more than 65% of the time in either one of the compartments during preconditioning were excluded from the study. This allowed us to use an unbiased design in which both compartments were equally preferred before the conditioning session and to randomly assign the compartment paired with EtOH.

The conditioning (30 min session−1) was performed using an unbiased procedure with the guillotine door being closed. All mice received EtOH (2 g kg−1, i.p.) and saline (same volume) injections every session, and the injections were altered between morning and afternoon sessions in a balanced design. The first injections were performed in the morning, and the mice were confined to one conditioning compartment for 30 min after the injection and then returned to their home cages. The second injections were performed in the afternoon, and the mice were confined to the other compartment for 30 min. It is worth mentioning that conditioning trials were conducted in an unbiased method such that EtOH/environment pairings were counterbalanced for the time of EtOH injection (morning or evening) and compartment. Analysis of data from our previous studies using this design (Al Ameri et al. 2014; Al Maamari et al. 2014; Al Mansouri et al. 2014; Bahi 2017a, b; Bahi et al. 2013) showed no difference in the effects of morning vs. evening administration of EtOH indicating a lack of carryover effects of EtOH between sessions.

During the post-CPP day, each mouse was individually placed between the two compartments (facing the frame of the guillotine door) with free access to both compartments for 15 min. The expression of EtOH-CPP was calculated as the difference of total time (s) spent in the drug-paired compartment during the post-CPP and the pre-CPP days.

At 24 h after the CPP-test, mice were given extinction training daily as described previously with minor modifications (Bahi 2012, 2015; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). The procedure was similar to the pre-CPP; all mice were free to access the two chambers for 15 min each day, for 15 days, without any injection. The amount of time that mice spent in each compartment was monitored every other day. The expression of EtOH-CPP extinction was calculated as the difference of total time (s) spent in the drug-paired compartment during the extinction trial and the pre-CPP day.

The effects of an exposure to the conditioning chambers following stress exposure and housing in a SE or an EE were evaluated as described previously with minor modifications (Bahi 2012, 2015; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). The reinstatement test was the same as that for post-CPP test (free access to both compartments for 15 min). The expression of EtOH-CPP reinstatement was calculated as the difference of total time (s) spent in the drug-paired compartment during reinstatement and the last extinction trial.

Chronic psychosocial stress

The chronic psychosocial stress procedure was developed originally by Reber and colleagues (Reber et al. 2007; Reber et al. 2008) and was performed as described in our previous studies with few modifications (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). In brief and for 15 days, five experimental subordinate “intruder” mice were introduced into the home cage of larger dominant male mouse “resident” that had been housed with two to three female mice for 7 days to enhance its territoriality and aggression. Males that start to injure their opponents by harmful bites were not used. Being too aggressive, two residents’ mice were discarded. To prevent habituation, the five-intruder mice (chronic subordinate colony (CSC)) were weighed and transferred to a novel resident cage in days 5 and 10. In all colonies, the larger male mouse established a dominant status by chasing and attacking all four experimental mice. The intruders were considered as “subordinates” based on their defensive behavior, including flight, retreat, and submissive upright. In parallel, single housed control (SHC) mice remained undisturbed in their home cages except when transferred to clean cages on days 5 and 10.

Elevated plus maze test

Both the EPM and OF tests were conducted between 10:00 a.m. and 4:00 p.m. in a brightly lit room that was illuminated with four fluorescent overhead bulb lights, which produce consistent illumination within the room (approximately 100 Lux). The mice were habituated to the room for 1 h before starting the respective test.

To evaluate the effect of CSC on anxiety-like behavior, the EPM test was performed as described in our previous studies (Bahi 2017a, b; Bahi and Dreyer 2018, 2019) and originally developed by Pellow et al. (Pellow et al. 1985). In brief, the four-armed wooden EPM apparatus consisted of two open arms (40 × 6 × 0.2 cm3) and two closed arms (40 × 6 × 20 cm3) facing each other and connected by a central platform (6 × 6 cm2). The mice were brought into the testing room and allowed to acclimate for 60 min. At the beginning of the test, each mouse was placed in the central area and the number of entries into the open and closed arms (OA and CA respectively) were manually scored during the 5-min test period. After each test, the fecal boli were removed, and the maze was cleaned with a 70% EtOH solution to remove odor trails.

Standard and environment enrichment

The general procedure was performed as described previously with minor modifications (Bahi 2017b). After completion of the EPM test, and like the SHC, CSC mice were single housed in standard Plexiglas cages with regular sawdust bedding and cotton nesting material for SE conditions. Inside each EE cage, and during 15 consecutive days, mice were provided with colorful plastic devices made of LEGO® building bricks, hard paper, cotton nesting material, plastic tube (15 cm long and 3 cm in diameter), and plastic dolls exchanged every week. These objects have been previously used as enriched conditions for mice leading to significant molecular and behavioral differences in laboratory animals (Grinan-Ferre et al. 2018; Hess et al. 2008; Lima and Spinelli de Oliveira 2014; Lopez and Laber 2015).

Open field test

The OF test was used to evaluate to effect of EE on anxiety-like behavior and was performed as previously described (Bahi 2017a, b; Bahi et al. 2014a; Bahi and Dreyer 2012a, 2014, 2018, 2019). In brief, the open field (32 × 32 × 20 cm3) Plexiglass square arena was divided into 64 equal squares by black lines. The 16 central squares are regarded as the “center” of the field. Each mouse was placed in the corner of the open field arena and allowed to explore for 10 min. The time spent in the center of the arena, the number of fecal boli produced, and line crossings were scored. After each mouse was tested, the fecal boli produced were counted and removed, and the entire arena was sanitized with 70% EtOH to remove odor trails. A low time spent in the center of the arena and high levels of fecal boli production were interpreted as indicators of an anxiogenic-like phenotype (Britton and Britton 1981; Calvo-Torrent et al. 1999; Sanberg 1989).

Experimental design and timeline

Experiment 1

In this experiment, we tested the effects of EE on CSC-delayed extinction of EtOH-CPP. For this purpose, and after completion of the CPP test (days 1–7), the mice were either single housed (SHC, n = 8) or housed with a dominant male mouse (CSC, n = 20) for 2 weeks (days 8–22). The EPM test was performed 24 h later (day 23), and the CSC mice were single housed either in SE (CSC-SE, n = 10) or EE (CSC-SE, n = 10) for 2 weeks (days 24–38). During this period, and in order to induce EtOH-CPP extinction, the mice were exposed daily to the conditioning chambers without any injection and the time spent in the drug-paired box was monitored every other day as described above. It should be emphasized that, in this experiment, the OF test was performed on day 30, when the SHC group showed complete EtOH-CPP extinction (7th trial). The experimental timeline is depicted in Fig. 1a.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Experimental timeline, weight gain, and anxiety-like behavior in the EPM test in SHC and CSC mice. a Sequence and duration of experimental protocols of the effects of chronic psychosocial stress and environment on EtOH-CPP extinction. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the b weight evolution in days 1, 5, 10, and 15 of stress exposure, and c weight gain as the difference in animals’ weights between days 1 and 15. For anxiety-like behavior, the data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the d percentage of time spent on open arms (OA), e number, f percentage of entries into the on OA, and g number of entries into the closed arms (CA). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.005, and ***p < 0.0001 indicate significant differences between SHC and CSC groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC (n = 20)

Experiment 2

In this experiment, we tested the effects of EE on CSC-exacerbated reinstatement of EtOH-CPP after extinction. For this purpose, and after completion of the CPP test (days 1–7), the mice were subjected to eight sessions of CPP extinction (days 8–15). Then, the mice were either single housed (SHC, n = 8) or housed with a dominant male mouse (CSC, n = 20) for 2 weeks (days 16–30) before being tested for anxiety-like behavior using the EPM test (day 31). The CSC mice were then single housed either in SE (CSC-SE, n = 10) or EE (CSC-SE, n = 10) for 2 weeks (days 32–46) and anxiety-like behavior was tested using the OF test (day 47). Twenty-four hours later, all the mice were exposed to the conditioning chambers to reinstate EtOH-CPP (day 48). The experimental timeline is depicted in Fig. 4a.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS version 16.0 for Windows. All data are expressed as the mean ± standard error of the mean. The data representing the effect of stress on body weight gain and anxiety-like behavior in the EPM test were analyzed using one-way measure of variance (ANOVA). The data representing the effects of stress on weight and time on EtOH-CPP extinction were analyzed using one-way repeated measures ANOVA. The data of the EtOH-CPP, area under the curve (AUC) for EtOH-CPP extinction, OF, and EtOH-CPP reinstatement were analyzed using one-way ANOVA. Simple linear regression (Pearson) analysis was performed to examine the correlation between anxiety measures in the EPM test and weight gain after the stress exposure. Also, the same simple linear regression was performed to examine the correlation between anxiety measures in the OF test after the EE housing with EtOH-CPP extinction (AUC) and reinstatement. The data were checked for homogeneity of the variances with the Levene test. A probability of p ≤ 0.05 was considered significant. Figures were created with SigmaPlot 11.0.

Results

Experiment 1—the effects of EE on CSC-delayed extinction of EtOH-CPP

Two grams per kilogram EtOH can successfully induce EtOH-CPP acquisition in mice

Although, one mouse was removed from the study due to an initial preference for one of the compartments (> 80% of the total session time spent in one compartment), we found no significant overall bias toward either of the compartments during the pre-CPP. In fact, the mice presented no preference for any of the compartments (447.46 vs. 452.54 s). However, the repeated administration of EtOH (2 g kg−1) produced a preference for the EtOH-paired compartment. One-way ANOVA with repeated measures demonstrated a significant effect of conditioning (F(1, 27) = 476.814, p < 0.0001) (data not shown). The mice were then randomly assigned to an experimental group (SHC, n = 8 and CSC, n = 20) as depicted in Fig. 1a.

Male CSC mice exhibit reduced weight gain and increased anxiogenic-like behavior

We have shown that exposure to the CSC stress has been associated with decreased weight gain (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). Therefore, the SHC and CSC mice were weighed on days 1, 5, 10, and 15 to assess the effect of stress on growth. The Levene test revealed that both experimental groups had similar homogeneity (day 1: F(1, 26) = 0.065, p = 0.800; day 5: F(1, 26) = 0.095, p = 0.760; day 10: F(1, 26) = 0.187, p = 0.669; day 15: F(1, 26) = 0.026, p = 0.872). As depicted in Fig. 1b, the one-way ANOVA with repeated measures revealed a main effect of time (F(3, 78) = 75.750, p < 0.0001) and stress (F(1, 26) = 9.666, p = 0.005), with a significant interaction between the two factors (F(3, 78) = 6.336, p = 0.001). Post hoc analysis indicated that mice in the CSC group had significantly lower body weights at days 5, 10, and 15 (p = 0.026, p = 0.01, and p < 0.001, respectively) but not at day 1 (p = 0.879). Consequently, and as depicted in Fig. 1c, extension of this analysis revealed that CSC mice had continued significant weight reduction compared with SHC controls (F(1, 26) = 13.861, p = 0.001) and the Levene test verified the homogeneity of variance assumption between the two groups (F(1, 26) = 0.042, p = 0.839).

We and others have shown that CSC exposure in rodents increases anxiogenic-like behavior as assessed by decreased investigation of open areas in a novel environment in the EPM and/or OF tests (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014; Reber et al. 2007). To determine the behavioral consequences of CSC exposure, a series of parameters were measured using the EPM test. The one-way ANOVA revealed that the CSC mice spent significantly less time than SHC controls in the OA of the maze (F(1, 26) = 15.122, p = 0.001) (Levene test; F(1, 26) = 1.921, p = 0.177) (Fig. 1d). Similarly, they also entered the OA significantly fewer times than SHC mice (F(1, 26) = 15.932, p < 0.001) (Levene test; F(1, 26) = 3.254, p = 0.083) (Fig. 1e) and (F(1, 26) = 14.176, p = 0.001) (Levene test; F(1, 26) = 3.473, p = 0.074) (Fig. 1f). Finally, and as depicted in Fig. 1g, analysis of total entries in the CA indicated that CSC and SHC mice were equally active (F(1, 26) = 0.004, p = 0.953) with a homogenic variance between the two groups (Levene test; F(1, 26) = 0.192, p = 0.665).

It should be emphasized that the Pearson test indicted that CSC-anxiogenic-like behavior correlated with decreased weight gain (data not shown).

EE reduced CSC-anxiogenic-like behavior and CSC-delayed EtOH-CPP extinction

After completion of the EPM test, the SHC mice were left undisturbed but a cotton nesting material was added to the cage. However, the CSC mice were housed in SE or EE (n = 10 each) for 15 days. During this period, the mice were exposed daily to the conditioning chambers to induce EtOH-CPP extinction. To be tested for their spontaneous, exploratory locomotion, and anxiety-like behaviors, the mice were subjected to the OF test after the 7th extinction trial (day 30) as described in the “Materials and methods” section.

Before CSC exposure, and as displayed in Fig. 2a, there was no significant differences, in the time spent in the EtOH-paired chamber, between the three experimental groups (F(2, 25) = 0.029, p = 0.972) and the Levene test verified the homogeneity of the variances (F(2, 25) = 1.078, p = 0.356).

Fig. 2
figure 2

EtOH-CPP expression and anxiety-like behavior in the OF test in SHC and CSC mice housed in standard (SE) or enriched (EE) environment. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the a EtOH-CPP expression before stress exposure. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the b time spent in the center of the arena, c number of fecal boli produced, and d number of line crossings during the OF test. *p < 0.0001 indicates significant differences between SHC and CSC-SE groups. #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 indicate significant differences between CSC-SE and CSC-EE groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC-SE (n = 10); CSC-EE (n = 10)

Analyses of behavioral data collected during OF testing reveled a main effect of housing on the time spent in the center of the arena (F(2, 25) = 11.932, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2b), but the Levene test for variance homogeneity was not found significant (F(2, 25) = 1.726, p = 0.199). Bonferroni post hoc evaluations revealed that stress exposure resulted in less time spent in the inner area of the field (SHC vs. CSC-SE; p < 0.0001). However, housing in an EE resulted in more time spent in the inner area of the field (CSC-SE vs. CSC-SE; p = 0.018). Also, and as expected, we found a main effect of housing on the number of fecal boli (F(2, 25) = 11.808, p < 0.0001; Fig. 2c), and the equality of variances, using the Levene test, was verified (F(2, 25) = 0.752, p = 0.482). Bonferroni post hoc evaluations indicated that CSC-SE mice produced more fecal boli in the OF test (SHC vs. CSC-SE; p < 0.0001). However, CSC-EE mice defecated less than CSC-SE animals in the novel cages (CSC-SE vs. CSC-SE; p = 0.007). As depicted in Fig. 2d, the total distance traveled during testing was not altered in the 3 experimental groups (F(2, 25) = 0.069, p = 0.934). The Levene test verified the homogeneity of variances assumptions (F(2, 25) = 0.064, p = 0.938).

We have shown in a previous study that CSC mice showed impaired EtOH-CPP extinction (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). Therefore, in this experiment, we asked whether chronic psychosocial stress-delayed EtOH-CPP extinction can be facilitated “shortened” with EE housing.

During the extinction phase, the mice were placed between the two compartments every day until CPP was extinguished. In fact and as depicted in Fig. 3a, the ANOVA with repeated measures showed significant effect of extinction training (F(7, 175) = 50.007, p < 0.0001) and housing (F(2, 25) = 6.317, p = 0.006) with a significant interaction (F(14, 175) = 1.926, p = 0.026). Bonferroni post hoc evaluations indicated that, compared with SHC, CSC-SE mice showed delayed extinction on days 5, 7, 9, 11, and 13 (p = 0.008, p = 0.001, p = 0.005, p < 0.001, and p = 0.028, respectively). However, EE significantly facilitated EtOH-CPP extinction in CSC mice on days 7 and 11 (p = 0.027 and p = 0.004, respectively) with a strong tendency on day 5 and 9 (p = 0.084 and p = 0.067, respectively). The individual AUC for each group are presented in Fig. 3b. There was a significant effect of housing on AUC (F(2, 25) = 7.653, p = 0.003) with the CSC-SE group experiencing an increase in extinction AUC as compared with the SHC group (p = 0.001), and the CSC-EE group experiencing a facilitated extinction compared with the CSC-SE group (p = 0.044). The Levene test, used to analyze the homogeneity of variances, was not found significant (F(2, 25) = 0.276, p = 0.761).

Fig. 3
figure 3

EtOH-CPP extinction in SHC and CSC mice housed in standard (SE) or enriched (EE) environment and Pearson correlations between area under the curve (AUC) and anxiety-like behavior in the OF test. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the a extinction of EtOH-CPP and b AUC for EtOH-CPP extinction. The data represent simple scatter regression between the AUC with c time spent in the center of the arena and d number of fecal boli produced during the OF test. ***p < 0.0001, **p < 0.01, and *p < 0.05 indicate significant differences between SHC and CSC-SE groups. #p < 0.05, ##p < 0.01, and ###p < 0.005 indicate significant differences between CSC-SE and CSC-EE groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC-SE (n = 10); CSC-EE (n = 10)

A simple linear regression analysis was performed to examine the correlation between the data of anxiety-like behavior obtained from the OF test and the AUC for EtOH-CPP extinction and the results are displayed in Fig. 3. The Pearson analysis showed a significant negative correlation between the AUC and the time spent of the arena (r = − 0.504; F(1, 26) = 8.867, p = 0.006; Fig. 3c). However, the AUC correlated positively with the number of fecal boli produced (r = 0.533; F(1, 26) = 10.313, p = 0.004; Fig. 3d), but not with spontaneous locomotor activity (r = − 0.111; F(1, 26) = 0.327, p = 0.572; Data not shown).

Taken together, these results demonstrated that EE-decreased measures of anxiety-like behavior, following CSC exposure, were strongly associated with facilitated “accelerated” EtOH-CPP extinction with no apparent effect on locomotor activity.

Experiment 2—the effects of EE on CSC-impaired EtOH-CPP reinstatement after extinction

We have shown in a previous report that CSC exposure exacerbated EtOH-CPP reinstatement (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). Hence, in this experiment, we asked whether, EE can decrease CSC-intensified reinstatement of EtOH-CPP after extinction. To this end mice were first conditioned with 2 g kg−1 and tested for EtOH-CPP as described above. The timeline of the experiment is displayed in Fig. 4a.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Experimental timeline, EtOH-CPP expression and extinction, and weight gain in SHC and CSC mice. a Sequence and duration of experimental protocols of the effect of chronic psychosocial stress and environment on EtOH-CPP reinstatement after extinction. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the b EtOH-CPP expression and c extinction. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the d weight evolution in days 1, 5, 10, and 15 of stress exposure and e weight gain as the difference in animals’ weights between days 1 and 15. *p < 0.01 and **p < 0.005 indicate significant differences between SHC and CSC groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC (n = 20)

Results have shown that mice displayed no initial preference for any of the compartments (451.57 vs. 448.43 s). In addition, the ANOVA with repeated measures indicated that the mice displayed an EtOH-induced preference compared with pre-CPP (F(1, 27) = 313.814, p < 0.0001) (data not shown). However, there was no significant effect of cohort, indicating that mice displayed similar magnitudes of CPP (F(2, 25) = 0.009, p = 0.991; Fig. 4b). Then and as displayed in Fig. 4c, the EtOH-CPP was successfully extinguished as indicated by the ANOVA with repeated measures (F(7, 175) = 81.767, p < 0.0001), with no significant differences between the three cohorts (F(2, 25) = 0.063, p = 0.939). The differences of the AUC of EtOH-CPP extinction for the three cohorts were not found significant (F(2, 25) = 0.045, p = 0.956; data not shown).

After extinction, chronic psychosocial stress reduced weight gain and increased anxiogenic-like behavior

After EtOH-CPP extinction, cohorts of mice (n = 20) were housed with a dominant mouse for 15 days. According to the Levene test, the groups had similar homogeneity at all time points in term of weight gain (day 1: F(1, 26) = 1.059, p = 0.313; day 5: F(1, 26) = 0.171, p = 0.682; day 10: F(1, 26) = 0.248, p = 0.623; day 15: F(1, 26) = 0.467, p = 0.501). As expected, the ANOVA with repeated measures revealed a main effect of time on body growth performance (F(3, 78) = 81.949, p < 0.0001), and the mice exposed to 2 weeks of CSC gained significantly less weight than those in the SHC group (F(1, 26) = 4.928, p = 0.035) with a significant time × stress interaction (F(3, 78) = 7.943, p < 0.0001) (Fig. 4d). Bonferroni post hoc evaluations indicated that CSC mice showed less weight after 10 and 15 days of stress (p = 0.006 and p = 0.003 respectively), but not after 5 days (p = 0.478). Also and as displayed in Fig. 4e, the animals in the CSC model group displayed significantly reduced body weight gain (F(1, 26) = 15.249, p = 0.001) with the variance homogeneity distribution checked by the Levene test (F(1, 26) = 1.784, p = 0.193).

The mice were then tested for their anxiety-like behavior using the EPM test and results indicated that chronic psychosocial stress increased anxiety, as evidenced by decreased time spent on the OA of the maze (F(1, 26) = 25.420, p < 0.0001) (Levene test: F(1, 26) = 0.005, p = 0.942) (Fig. 5a). The CSC mice displayed less number (F(1, 26) = 27.351, p < 0.0001; Fig. 5b) and percentage (F(1, 26) = 16.038, p < 0.0001; Fig. 5c) of entries in the OA with the homogeneity of variances assumptions being verified by the Levene test for both parameters ((F(1, 26) = 0.004, p = 0.952) and (F(1, 26) = 2.570, p = 0.121), respectively). Lastly and as depicted in Fig. 5d, we demonstrated that stress exposure had no effect on spontaneous locomotor activity as SHC and CSC mice showed equal number of visits to the CA of the EPM (F(1, 26) = 0.114, p = 0.739) (Levene test for homogeneity of variance between the two groups, F(1, 26) = 1.373, p = 0.252).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Anxiety-like behavior in the EPM test in SHC and CSC mice. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the a percentage of time spent on open arms (OA), b number, c percentage of entries into the on OA, and d number of entries into the closed arms (CA). *p < 0.001 indicates significant differences between SHC and CSC groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC (n = 20)

The relationship between weight gain and anxiety-like behavior was explored through Pearson correlation analysis and the results indicated that CSC-anxiogenic-like behavior correlated with decreased weight gain the (data not shown).

EE ameliorated CSC-induced anxiety-like behavior and reduced EtOH-CPP reinstatement

After the EPM test, the CSC mice were housed in SE or EE for 15 days and anxiety-like behavior was tested using the OF test and the results are displayed in Fig. 6. As expected, there was main effect of housing on the time spent in the center of the arena (F(2, 25) = 10.187, p = 0.001). In fact, as depicted in Fig. 6a, compared with the SHC controls, chronic stress exposure significantly shortened the time the mice spent in the center of the OF (p < 0.0001). However, post hoc evaluations comparing CSC-SE and CSC-EE showed higher responses by the latter in center squares time (p = 0.031) with equal variances (Levene test: F(2, 25) = 2.979, p = 0.069). Also, 15-day stress exposure affected the number of fecal boli (F(2, 25) = 11.325, p < 0.001) (Fig. 6b) and the homogeneity of variances was verified with the Levene test (F(2, 25) = 2.772, p = 0.082). Bonferroni post hoc evaluations indicated that CSC mice housed in SE produced more fecal boli during the OF test than SHC mice (p < 0.001). However, CSC-EE shed fewer fecal boli than CSC-SE mice in the OF (p = 0.050). As shown in Fig. 6c, neither the CSC nor the EE affected spontaneous locomotor activity (F(2, 25) = 0.213, p = 0.810) (Levene test: F(2, 25) = 0.148, p = 0.863).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Anxiety-like behavior in the OF test, EtOH-CPP reinstatement in SHC, and CSC mice housed in SE or EE and Pearson correlations. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the a time spent in the center of the arena, b number of fecal boli produced, and c number of line crossings during the OF test. The data are expressed as mean ± SEM for the d EtOH-CPP reinstatement CPP in SHC and CSC housed in SE or EE. The data represent simple scatter regression between EtOH-CPP reinstatement with e time spent in the center of the arena and f number of fecal boli produced during the OF test. ***p < 0.0001, **p < 0.001, and *p < 0.005 indicate significant differences between SHC and CSC-SE groups. #p < 0.05 and ##p < 0.01 indicate significant differences between CSC-SE and CSC-EE groups. SHC (n = 8); CSC-SE (n = 10); CSC-EE (n = 10)

We next examined how EE- and CSC-resulted memory retrieval disruption influences subsequent EtOH-induced reinstatement of the CPP. In this test, the animals spent significantly more time in the drug-paired compartment when compared with the last extinction session (F(1, 27) = 94.862, p < 0.0001) with a significant effect of housing (F(2, 25) = 5.786, p = 0.009) (Fig. 6d) and nonsignificant Levene test (F(2, 25) = 0.548, p = 0.585). Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that, as expected, in a SE, CSC exacerbated EtOH-CPP reinstatement (p = 0.003 vs. SHC). However, the CPP reinstatement, observed in the mice housed in an EE, was significantly lower compared with the CPP reinstatement observed in the mice housed in a SE (p = 0.028).

Taken together, EE induced a significant deficit in EtOH-associated memory retrieval as CSC-increased reinstatement was considerably decreased by EE.

Lastly, a Pearson correlation test to determine the strength of association between EtOH-CPP reinstatement and anxiety-like behavior in the OF test showed a strong negative correlation between CPP reinstatement and the time spent in the center of the arena (r = − 0.611, F(1, 26) = 15.493, p = 0.001; Fig. 6e), and a moderate, yet significant positive correlation between reinstatement and defecation in the OF (r = 0.469, F(1, 26) = 7.339, p = 0.012; Fig. 6f). However, both linear regression and Pearson correlations demonstrated no significant relationship between CPP reinstatement and line crossings, i.e., spontaneous locomotor activity (r = 0.187, F(1, 26) = 0.944, p = 0.340; data not shown).

The results indicated that EE-decreased reinstatement of EtOH-CPP could be correlated with EE-anxiolytic-like effect.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first to investigate the effects of EE following chronic psychosocial stress on EtOH-CPP extinction and reinstatement. Indeed, two major findings arise from the current investigation. First, we found that stress-delayed EtOH-CPP extinction was facilitated upon exposure to an EE. Second, when EtOH-CPP was extinguished, EE blocked EtOH-CPP reinstatement.

In agreement with previous studies from our laboratory (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014), the data presented in the current study provide strong indication that subordinate colony housing (CSC) is a suitable procedure to model chronic psychosocial stress in male mice. As expected, we have observed that stress exposure induced a significant reduction in body weight gain which is in line with our previous reports (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014) and with others using the same model (Fuchsl et al. 2014; Nyuyki et al. 2012; Reber et al. 2007; Reber et al. 2008; Singewald et al. 2009; Slattery et al. 2012; Veenema et al. 2008). Moreover, repeated defeat (once a day for 7 days) decreased body weight gain in Lewis rats in a time-dependent manner (Berton et al. 1998). It has been shown in rats that the extent of body weight gain is related to the rank of the animal within a social group (Stefanski 2001). In mice, several studies have reported that subordinate males are characterized by severe and significant weight loss (Blanchard et al. 1995; Tamashiro et al. 2004). One possible explanation is that weight loss in subordinate mice during social stress was due to the loss of adipose tissue. In fact, previous data have shown that while subordinate mice lost adipose tissue during stress exposure, they retained a higher percentage of visceral fat compared with controls (Tamashiro et al. 2007). Although, visceral fat is known to be an important risk factor in the development of different signs of the metabolic syndrome (for review, see (Eckel et al. 2005)), the mechanisms contributing to its development are not well characterized and more research is needed to elucidate such mechanisms.

We also observed that exposure to CSC significantly increased anxiety-like behavior as indicated by a decrease in the percentage of time spent on the open arms, and in the percentage of open arm entries compared with control mice. The number of entries into the closed arms, indicative of spontaneous locomotor activity, was not affected by CSC exposure as we have reported in our previous studies (Bahi 2013, 2017b; Bahi and Dreyer 2014). The current results are in line with previous studies showing that prolonged periods of stress result in higher anxiety levels. In fact, elevated level of anxiety is a strong indicator of acute or chronic stress because restraint (1 h) produced an anxiogenic-like phenotype in the elevated X-maze (McBlane and Handley 1994). In rats, exposure to the aggressive resident, leading to the rapid social defeat of the intruder, reduced subsequent exploration of open maze arms in males (Heinrichs et al. 1992; Nephew and Bridges 2011; Rygula et al. 2008) and females (McCormick et al. 2008). Similarly, in male DBA/2 mice, defeat by an experienced aggressive conspecific dominant mouse enhanced anxiety as indicated by decreases in percent open arm entries and percent time spent on the open arms (Rodgers and Cole 1993). Also, in DBA/2OlaHsd mice, chronic social defeat induced an increased avoidance behavior and significant reduction in directed exploration, and general exploration, as well as locomotion (Erhardt et al. 2009). Taken together, the findings confirmed stress vulnerability for subordinate mice and further establish the strength to use the current CSC procedure as a model for anxiety-related disorders.

In the 1st experiment, we asked the question whether EE could reverse and accelerate stress-delayed EtOH-CPP extinction. EtOH (2 g kg−1) elicited a reliable and consistent CPP response. In fact, and after a period of conditioning, the mice spent significantly more time in the chamber paired with EtOH. These results are consistent with earlier studies, from our laboratory (Al Ameri et al. 2014; Al Maamari et al. 2014; Bahi and Dreyer 2014; Bahi et al. 2014b) and from others, in which 2 g kg−1 EtOH elicited a robust CPP in both mice (Huang et al. 2018; Marcus et al. 2017; Xie et al. 2019), and rats (Fu et al. 2016; Okhuarobo et al. 2018; Zuo et al. 2017). Next, we found, in the 1st experiment, that repeated daily exposure to both conditioning chambers successfully extinguished this CPP. It is interesting to note that, in control animals (SHC group), only 7 to 8 consecutive days were required to produce a reliable extinction. However, when mice were housed in a SE, after being exposed to chronic social stress (CSC-SE group), CPP extinction was significantly delayed and was achieved only after 14 days of repeated exposure to the conditioning chambers. Most importantly, EE accelerated EtOH-CPP extinction by 4–5 days in stress-exposed mice. In the 2nd experiment, we observed that, following extinction, EE attenuated stress-induced EtOH-CPP reinstatement.

It is worth mentioning that an experimental group receiving no social stress followed by EE is lacking from this study. Therefore, we should acknowledge that without such a control, we cannot rule out the general effects of EE on learning and motivation, rather than effects specific to EtOH-CPP, or drug reward in general. Nevertheless, the current findings confirm and extend a large body of literature that utilized self-administration, as well as CPP, to show that EE could reverse stress- and drug-elicited reinstatement following extinction with other drugs of abuse and psychostimulants. In fact, Gauthier et al. (2017) have reported that brief periods of EE programmed at appropriate times in combination with cocaine-cue extinction sessions facilitated extinction and prevented subsequent reacquisition of cocaine self-administration in male Wistar rats. Also, EE attenuated cocaine-seeking behavior compared with isolated conditions in rats (Thiel et al. 2012). Similarly, the effects of EE on nicotine-induced CPP in male rats were assessed and the results indicated that, in enriched conditions, rats showed extinction of the nicotine-induced CPP response by the 5th extinction session (Ewin et al. 2015). Also, while animals in standard and enriched housing conditions intravenously self-administered similar amounts of heroin over 14 days, EE diminished extinction response in male Sprague-Dawley rats (Imperio et al. 2018). In addition, 30 days of EE significantly and consistently reduced cocaine seeking behavior during extinction sessions and significantly reduced cue and stress-induced reinstatement of cocaine self-administration (Chauvet et al. 2009).

It should be emphasized that chronic social stress decreased body weight in these animals by 10–15%. Such dramatic changes could lead to alterations in motivational mechanisms generally. In fact, a growing body of literature suggested that food restriction following CPP training enhances CPP reinstatement. For example, unlike those tested under the ad libitum feeding condition, food restricted rats showed a persistent cocaine-CPP (Zheng et al. 2012). Similarly, rats that were food restricted displayed a strong increase in cue-induced heroin seeking behavior and, interestingly, refeeding prior to testing or shortening the food restriction duration abrogated the increase of heroin seeking behavior (D’Cunha et al. 2013). Moreover, food-restricted rats performed significantly more active lever presses and earned more sucrose reinforcements than ad libitum fed rats (Cason and Aston-Jones 2014). Also, a mild food restriction regimen, that results in the rats’ body weights maintained at 90% of their nonrestricted body weight, resulted in a significantly increased rates of heroin seeking on test day (Sedki et al. 2013a; b). In the light of these findings, we cannot exclude that weight loss is still a potential cause of enhanced EtOH-CPP reinstatement and delayed extinction. Similarly, the amelioration of that weight loss might cause a reduction of CPP reinstatement seen following EE. Although earlier studies had focused on the “protective/preventive” effects of EE, it has been reported that EE can also have “curative/restorative” effects on psychostimulants’ addiction as cocaine-induced CPP was sustained over long periods of abstinence in mice housed in SE but is gone in mice housed in EE (Solinas et al. 2008). Taken together, these findings indicate that EE can accelerate CPP extinction and remove already established addiction-related behaviors in rodents and suggest that EE may be an important aspect in facilitating abstinence and preventing relapse to EtOH addiction.

Although the current investigation demonstrates the ability of EE to facilitate CPP extinction and reduce stress-triggered EtOH-CPP reinstatement, it limits insight into the possible mechanisms underlying this effect. Nevertheless, we speculate that EE enhanced extinction learning, as suggested previously for EE effects on reducing cue-elicited sucrose-seeking behavior (Grimm et al. 2008). Indeed, EE improves learning on a number of behavioral task measures such as spatial learning, reinforcement, place preference, and discrimination tasks (Bardo et al. 1995; Pham et al. 1999; Rosenzweig and Bennett 1996; Smith et al. 2005; van Praag et al. 2000). Therefore, we hypothesize that, following stress exposure, EE mice may have learned more quickly that EtOH-associated cues no longer predicted EtOH administration, suggesting that EE might attenuate the incentive motivational effects of the EtOH-paired stimuli. It should be emphasized that this explanation might not be totally in line with Chauvet et al.’s (2009) findings which indicated that EE blunts cocaine-seeking behavior on the 1st day of extinction, and afterward throughout re-exposure, without seeming to facilitate extinction learning (i.e., rate of extinction across days) per se. On another hand, a large body of evidence suggested that the rewarding effects of EtOH are due to increases in dopamine levels in the mesolimbic system (Di Chiara 1997), indicating that dopaminergic neurotransmission is a critical factor in both drug reward and relapse. Therefore, it seems that EE might “protect” against subsequent increases in brain dopamine elicited by stress exposure. In fact, stress-increased dopamine release in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) was significantly lower in enriched animals when compared with controls (Segovia et al. 2008). Also, handling-induced dopamine release in the PFC was significantly reduced in an EE (Segovia et al. 2008), suggesting that EE might reduce the activity of the mesocortical dopaminergic system. Regardless of the mechanisms underlying these effects, the implications of this study offer promise for alcohol-dependent individuals.

In conclusion, following stress exposure, EE facilitates extinction and reduces the risk of relapse in mice. Although, caution must be taken when generalizing these results to a human population, these findings highlight how environmental situations of people looking to quit drug taking could possibly contribute to the positive result of their efforts. In fact, studies in which different forms of stimulation have been used were shown to be useful for the treatment of addiction (Schnabel 2009). Therefore, the use of environmental stimulation together with pharmacotherapy as well as behavioral therapy may be of fundamental importance in the achievement of effective alcoholism treatment programs.