Introduction

Chemical structure modifications to the prototypic monoamine transporter substrate amphetamine (Fig. 1) alter the relative selectivity of the compound to bind to the dopamine (DAT) versus serotonin (SERT) transporter. These modifications may alter the neurochemical or behavioral effects of the compound. For example, the addition of an N-methyl to amphetamine (methamphetamine) slightly attenuated the relative selectivity to release dopamine (DA) vs. serotonin (5-HT) in vitro (Rothman et al. 2001; Simmler et al. 2013) and also attenuated the potency to increase extracellular DA levels and enhanced the potency to increase extracellular 5-HT levels in the nucleus accumbens (Baumann et al. 2012; Baumann et al. 2011). However, in drug discrimination studies, both (+)-amphetamine and (+)-methamphetamine produce overlapping discriminative stimulus effects in monkeys (Banks et al. 2016; Woolverton and English 1997) and humans (Lamb and Henningfield 1994; Sevak et al. 2009), suggesting these neurochemical differences produced by an N-methyl addition minimally impact the discriminative stimulus of these drugs. Similar to the N-methyl substitution, addition of a 3,4-methylenedioxy moiety to amphetamine (3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine; MDA) also attenuated the DA vs. 5-HT selectivity to increase extracellular monoamine levels (KankaanpÄÄ et al. 1998; Nash and Nichols 1991). In rats and pigeons, (±)-MDA produced complete and full substitution for (+)-amphetamine in a discrimination procedure (Evans and Johanson 1986; Glennon and Young 1984a). However, in contrast to these rat and pigeon amphetamine discrimination results, (±)-MDA produced full (+)-amphetamine-like discriminative stimulus effects in two out of three monkeys (Kamien et al. 1986). Overall, these data suggest that chemical structure modifications may differentially impact the discriminative stimulus effects of monoamine transporter substrates depending upon where the chemical structure modification occurs and what type of moiety is being attached. These results also suggest potential species differences in the abuse-related behavioral effects of amphetamine analogs.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Chemical structure of amphetamine, methamphetamine, cathinone, and methcathinone and their 3,4-methylenedioxy analogs

Recently, synthetic cathinones have emerged as a heterogeneous class of abused compounds that function as either monoamine transport substrates or monoamine transport inhibitors (Baumann et al. 2012; Cozzi et al. 2013; Simmler et al. 2013). Cathinone is the beta-ketone analog of amphetamine (Fig. 1) that serves as the pharmacophore for these synthetic analogs and, like amphetamine, functions as a monoamine transporter substrate (Rothman et al. 2003; Simmler et al. 2013) to increase extracellular nucleus accumbens DA levels (Pehek et al. 1990). However, there is a paucity of in vivo studies that have determined the behavioral consequences of different chemical structure modifications to cathinone. Furthermore, most studies have focused on phenyl ring modifications to N-methylcathinone (methcathinone) (Bonano et al. 2015; Sakloth et al. 2015; Suyama et al. 2016) given that these analogs have been most prevalent in Drug Enforcement Administration seizures and forensic toxicology reports (DEA 2014). Moreover, only one study has examined the effects of a 3,4-methylenedixoy moiety addition to cathinone (3,4-methylenedioxycathinone; MDC) in rats (Dal Cason et al. 1997). In contrast to the full substitution of (±)-MDA in (+)-amphetamine-trained rats, (±)-MDC produced partial substitution in (+)-amphetamine-trained rats. Overall, this literature suggests that amphetamine and cathinone may be differentially sensitive to the same chemical structure modifications. However, direct comparisons of the same chemical structure modifications on the abuse-related effects of amphetamine and cathinone analogs in non-human primate models are lacking.

Given the potential for species differences noted above for MDA, the aim was to conduct structure activity relationship (SAR) studies to directly compare the effects of an N-methyl and 3,4-methylenedioxy addition on the cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of (±)-amphetamine and (±)-cathinone in rhesus monkeys. Preclinical drug discrimination procedures have been especially useful to improve our understanding of the pharmacological mechanisms and SARs of central nervous system active compounds (Glennon and Young 2011). Cocaine was chosen as the training drug because its discriminative stimulus effects have been extensively characterized in non-human primates (Banks et al. 2014; Garza and Johanson 1983; Kleven et al. 1990; Spealman 1995), and these results suggest that the cocaine training dose used in the present study is primarily mediated by dopaminergic mechanisms. Moreover, cocaine has also been used as the training stimulus in rodent drug discrimination studies examining some of the amphetamine and cathinone analogs examined in the present study (Gatch et al. 2015; Gatch et al. 2013; Glennon and Young 1984b), thus allowing for a direct translational comparison between rodents and non-human primates. For comparison, we also determined the effects of a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition on the cocaine-like effects of methamphetamine (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine; MDMA). We have previously published (±)-methcathinone and (±)-3,4-methylenedioxymethcathinone (MDMC) results in these same monkeys under the same cocaine discrimination procedure (Smith et al. 2016). These previously published results suggested that a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition to methcathinone did not significantly alter the potency or efficacy of methcathinone to produce cocaine-like effects but did prolong the time course. We hypothesized that the cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of amphetamine and cathinone would be attenuated by a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition. Furthermore, we hypothesized that the cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of methamphetamine would not be altered by a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition, similar to our previous methcathinone and MDMC results (Smith et al. 2016).

Methods

Subjects

Drug discrimination studies were conducted in four adult male rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) weighing between 9 and 13 kg. All monkeys had a cocaine discrimination experimental history (Banks 2014; Banks et al. 2013; Banks et al. 2015; Smith et al. 2016) and were maintained on a diet of biscuits (Lab Diet High Protein Monkey Biscuits, PMI Feeds, Inc., St. Louis, MO) and fresh fruit provided after the behavioral session. Experimental sessions were conducted in the monkeys’ home chamber and water was available throughout behavioral sessions. Additionally, the monkeys could earn 1-g banana-flavored pellets (5TUR grain-based precision primate tablets, Test Diets, Richmond, IN) during daily experimental sessions (described below). A 12-h light-dark cycle was in effect (lights on from 6 a.m. to 6 p.m.). Environmental enrichment consisting of various food puzzles, TV, or radio was provided daily at the conclusion of the behavioral sessions. Facilities were licensed by the US Department of Agriculture and accredited by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care. Experiments were conducted in accordance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Council 2011) and under Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee-approved research and enrichment protocols.

Cocaine discrimination procedure

Experimental sessions were conducted in each monkey’s home chamber as described previously in detail (Banks et al. 2013). On the front wall of each chamber were a custom operant response panel with three horizontally arranged square response keys, and only the left and right keys were used in the present studies. Attached to each panel was a pellet dispenser (Med Associates, ENV-203-1000, St. Albans, VT). Equipment operation and data collection were accomplished with a Windows-based computer and MED-PC software (Med Associates).

Monkeys were trained to discriminate 0.32 mg/kg cocaine intramuscularly (IM) from saline in a two-key, food-reinforced drug discrimination procedure as previously described (Banks et al. 2013). Discrimination training was conducted 5 days per week during daily sessions composed of multiple components. Each component consisted of a 5-min response period, during which the right and left response keys were transilluminated red and green, respectively, and the monkeys could earn up to 10 food pellets by responding under a fixed-ratio (FR) 30 schedule of food presentation. Key color and position were not counterbalanced between monkeys. Training sessions were composed of three components presented at 2-h intervals, and either saline or cocaine (0.32 mg/kg) was administered IM approximately 15 min prior to the start of each component. Thus, on training days, the monkeys would receive a sequence of saline (S) and cocaine (C) injections in the order SSS, SSC, SCS, CSS, SCC, CSC, CCS, or CCC. These training sequences were randomly presented to engender daily experience with randomized sequences of saline- and cocaine-appropriate components. The 2-h duration of inter-component intervals was selected to exceed the time course of discriminative stimulus effects produced by the cocaine-training dose in rhesus monkeys (Lamas et al. 1995) and to thereby minimize effects of cocaine administered in earlier trials on performance during later trials on the same day. Following administration of saline, only responding on the green key (the saline-appropriate key) produced food, whereas following administration of the 0.32-mg/kg cocaine, only responding on the red key (the cocaine-appropriate key) produced food. Responses on the inappropriate key reset the FR requirement on the appropriate key. The criterion for accurate discrimination was ≥85 % injection-appropriate responding before delivery of the first reinforcer, ≥90 % injection-appropriate responding for the entire component, and response rates ≥0.1 responses/s (sufficient to earn at least one pellet) for all components during seven of eight consecutive sessions.

Test sessions were identical to training sessions except that (a) responding on either the left or right key produced food, (b) monkeys received only one injection of vehicle or a test drug dose at the start of the session, and (c) 5-min response components began 10, 30, 56, 100, 180, 300, and 560 min after the injection to assess the time course of drug effects. The drugs and dose ranges tested were (±)-amphetamine (0.1–1.0 mg/kg), (±)-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetamine (MDA; 0.32–3.2 mg/kg), (±)-cathinone (0.032–1.0 mg/kg), (±)-3,4-methylenedioxycathinone (MDC; 1.0–10.0 mg/kg), (±)-methamphetamine (0.032–0.32 mg/kg), and (±)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA; 0.32–3.2 mg/kg). Test sessions were generally conducted on Tuesdays and Fridays with training sessions conducted on Mondays, Wednesdays, and Thursdays. Test sessions were conducted only if performance during the previous two training sessions met the criteria for accurate discrimination (described above). All test drug doses were determined once in each monkey. All doses of a given test drug were evaluated in a given monkey before testing the next drug, and vehicle (saline) test sessions were conducted before or after evaluation of each test drug. The order of drug doses and drugs was counterbalanced across monkeys.

Data analysis

The primary dependent measures were (1) percent cocaine-appropriate responding (%CAR) {defined as (number of responses on the cocaine-associated key divided by the total number of responses on both the cocaine-and saline-associated keys) * 100} and (2) response rates during each component. These dependent measures were then plotted as a function of time after drug or saline administration. Percent CAR and response rates were analyzed using linear mixed effect analysis with drug dose and time as the main fixed effects and subjects as the random effect (JMP Pro 11.1.1, SAS, Cary, NC). A significant drug × time interaction was followed by the Dunnett’s post hoc test for comparison to vehicle (saline) conditions within a given time point. Drug doses that produced ≥90 % cocaine-appropriate responding at any time point were considered full substitution. In addition, ED50 values were defined as the test drug dose that produced 50 % cocaine-appropriate responding at the 30-min time point. ED50 values were determined by linear regression when three data points were available or by interpolation when only two data points were available (one data point below and one data point above 50 % cocaine-appropriate responding) in each individual monkey, and individual values were averaged to yield mean ED50 values and 95 % confidence limits. Duration of action comparisons between equivalent drug doses was inferred by the number of time points significantly different from saline.

Drugs

(−)-Cocaine HCl, (±)-cathinone HCl, and (±)-methamphetamine HCl were supplied by the National Institute on Drug Abuse Drug Supply Program (Bethesda, MD). Bruce Blough (RTI) synthesized (±)-amphetamine fumarate, (±)-MDA HCl, and (±)-MDC HCl. David E. Nichols synthesized and generously donated (±)-MDMA HCl. Drug doses were calculated and expressed using the salt forms listed above.

Results

On all training days that preceded test days, percent injection-appropriate responding was 99.9 ± 0.1 % and 99.9 ± 0.1 % during cocaine- and saline-appropriate components, respectively. Rates of operant responding during cocaine and saline training components were 2.9 ± 0.2 and 2.6 ± 0.2 responses/s, respectively. Saline administration engendered <10 % cocaine-appropriate responding at all time points (Figs. 2, 3, and 4) and had no significant effect on rates of responding in all time course test sessions (Figs. 2, 3, and 4). (±)-Amphetamine, (±)-cathinone, and (±)-methamphetamine produced dose-dependent and full substitution, ≥90 % cocaine-appropriate responding, in all four monkeys (Figs. 2, 3, and 4). In contrast, (±)-MDA produced full substitution in two out of four monkeys, (±)-MDC produced full substitution in one out of three monkeys, and (±)-MDMA produced full substitution in three out of four monkeys. Table 1 shows the ED50 values to produce cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects at the 30-min time point as this time point represented peak effects for the majority of the compounds. Amphetamine and cathinone were equipotent. Methamphetamine and previously published (Smith et al. 2016) methcathinone results in these same monkeys were also equipotent. Amphetamine and methamphetamine were also equipotent; however, both methamphetamine and methcathinone were significantly more potent than was cathinone to produce cocaine-like effects as denoted by the non-overlapping confidence limits for methamphetamine or methcathinone and cathinone (Table 1). In general, the addition of a 3,4-methylenedioxy moiety attenuated the potency of the parent compound to produce cocaine-like effects. The one exception was (±)-3,4-methylenedioxymethcathinone (MDMC; Smith et al. 2016). However, exact potency shifts for all 3,4-methylenedioxy analogs were complicated due to the inability to calculate individual ED50 values in all monkeys for all compounds. Limited MDC supply also precluded testing in all four monkeys and complicated potency comparisons.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Potency and time course of (±)-amphetamine (a, c; 0.1–1.0 mg/kg, IM) and (±)-3,4-methylenedioxyamphetmaine (MDA) (a, d; 0.32–3.2 mg/kg, IM) in male rhesus monkeys (n = 4) trained to discriminate cocaine (0.32 mg/kg, IM) vs. saline. Abscissae: time in min after injection (log scale). Top panel ordinates: percent cocaine-appropriate responding. Bottom panel ordinates: operant response rates in responses per second. Symbols above “S” and “C” represent the group averages for all saline- and cocaine-training sessions preceding test sessions, respectively. Filled symbols indicate statistical significance compared to saline at a given time point (p < 0.05). Number in parentheses indicate the number of subjects contributing to that data point if less than the total number of subjects tested an indicative of a time point where a monkey failed to complete at least one ratio requirement

Fig. 3
figure 3

Potency and time course of (±)-cathinone (a, c; 0.032–1.0 mg/kg, IM) and (±)-3,4-methylenedioxycathinone (MDC) (b, d; 1.0–10.0 mg/kg, IM) in male rhesus monkeys (n = 3–4) trained to discriminate cocaine (0.32 mg/kg, IM) vs. saline. Cathinone was tested in four monkeys whereas MDC was tested in three monkeys. Abscissae: time in min after injection (log scale). Top panel ordinates: percent cocaine-appropriate responding. Bottom panel ordinates: operant response rates in responses per second. Symbols above “S” and “C” represent the group averages for all saline- and cocaine-training sessions preceding test sessions, respectively. Filled symbols indicate statistical significance compared to saline at a given time point (p < 0.05). Number in parentheses indicate the number of subjects contributing to that data point if less than the total number of subjects tested an indicative of a time point where a monkey failed to complete at least one ratio requirement

Fig. 4
figure 4

Potency and time course of (±)-methamphetamine (a, c; 0.032–0.32 mg/kg, IM) and (±)-3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA) (b, d; 0.32–3.2 mg/kg, IM) in male rhesus monkeys (n = 4) trained to discriminate cocaine (0.32 mg/kg, IM) vs. saline. Abscissae: time in min after injection (log scale). Top panel ordinates: percent cocaine-appropriate responding. Bottom panel ordinates: operant response rates in responses per second. Symbols above “S” and “C” represent the group averages for all saline- and cocaine-training sessions preceding test sessions, respectively. Filled symbols indicate statistical significance compared to saline at a given time point (p < 0.05). Number in parentheses indicate the number of subjects contributing to that data point if less than the total number of subjects tested an indicative of a time point where a monkey failed to complete at least one ratio requirement

Table 1 Mean ED50 values for test drugs in rhesus monkeys trained to discriminate 0.32 mg/kg intramuscular cocaine from saline

Cocaine-like effects of amphetamine and its 3,4-methylenedioxy analog MDA

Figure 2 shows the potency and time course of (±)-amphetamine and (±)-MDA to produce cocaine-like effects (Fig. 2a, b) and alter rates of responding (Fig. 2c, d). Amphetamine (0.32 mg/kg) produced full substitution in two monkeys and 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine produced full substitution in all four monkeys. Amphetamine produced significant dose- and time-dependent cocaine-like effects, and 1.0 mg/kg amphetamine produced cocaine-like effects that were significant from saline for 180 min (amphetamine dose × time: F 18, 80 = 5.0, p < 0.0001). For MDA, 1.0 mg/kg produced full substitution in two monkeys, and 3.2 mg/kg produced full substitution in one monkey. In contrast to amphetamine, there was only a significant effect of MDA dose (MDA dose: F 3, 44 = 4.8, p = 0.0053). Amphetamine (dose: F 3, 81 = 2.9, p = 0.0417) significantly increased rates of responding and MDA (MDA dose: F 3, 81 = 10.3, p < 0.0001) significantly decreased rates of responding (Fig. 2c, d).

Cocaine-like effects of cathinone and its 3,4-methylenedioxy analog MDC

Figure 3 shows the potency and time course of (±)-cathinone and (±)-MDC to produce cocaine-like effects (Fig. 3a, b) and alter rates of operant responding (Fig. 3c, d). Cathinone (0.1 mg/kg) produced full substitution in one monkey, 0.32 mg/kg cathinone produced full substitution in three monkeys, and 1.0 mg/kg cathinone produced full substitution in all four monkeys. Cathinone produced significant dose- and time-dependent cocaine-like effects, and 1.0 mg/kg cathinone produced cocaine-like effects that were significant from saline for 56 min (cathinone dose × time: F 24, 101 = 7.5, p < 0.0001). For MDC, both 3.2 and 10 mg/kg produced full substitution in a single monkey. In contrast to cathinone, there was only a significant effect of MDC dose (MDC dose: F 3, 49.3 = 3.7, p = 0.0177). Larger MDC doses could not be tested because of limited drug supply. Cathinone significantly increased rates of responding (dose: F 4, 102 = 4.1, p = 0.0037; dose × time: F 24, 102 = 1.9, p = 0.0181) (3c). MDC did not significantly alter rates of responding up to 10 mg/kg, although two out of the three monkeys failed to complete at least one response requirement during the 10-min component after administration of 10 mg/kg MDC (3d).

Cocaine-like effects of methamphetamine and its 3,4-methylenedioxy analog MDMA

Figure 4 shows the potency and time course of (±)-methamphetamine and (±)-MDMA to produce cocaine-like effects (Fig. 4a, b) and alter rates of operant responding (Fig. 4c, d). Methamphetamine (0.1 mg/kg) produced full substitution in two monkeys, and 0.32 mg/kg methamphetamine produced full substitution in all four monkeys. Methamphetamine produced dose- and time-dependent cocaine-like effects, and 0.32 mg/kg methamphetamine produced cocaine-like effects that were significant from saline for 100 min (methamphetamine dose × time: F 18, 81 = 3.8, p < 0.0001). In addition, methamphetamine produced longer lasting cocaine-like effects compared to previously published methcathinone results at equivalent doses (Smith et al. 2016). For MDMA, 0.32 mg/kg produced full substitution in one monkey, 1.0 mg/kg produced full substitution in three monkeys, and 3.2 mg/kg produced full substitution in two monkeys. In contrast to methamphetamine, there was only a significant effect of MDMA dose (dose: F 3, 53.2 = 6.1, p = 0.0013). Methamphetamine significantly increased rates of responding (dose: F 3, 81 = 4.1, p = 0.0094). In contrast, MDMA dose-dependently decreased rates of responding (dose: F 3, 81 = 12, p < 0.0001; dose × time: F 18, 81 = 2.8, p = 0.0007).

Discussion

The present study aim was to compare and contrast the effects of a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition on (±)-amphetamine, (±)-cathinone, and (±)-methamphetamine cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects in rhesus monkeys. There were two main findings. First, amphetamine, cathinone, and methamphetamine produced full, ≥90 %, cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects. At equivalent doses, methamphetamine produced longer lasting cocaine-like effects than did amphetamine or cathinone. However, comparisons between equally efficacious doses revealed a time course rank order of amphetamine > methamphetamine > cathinone from longest to shortest. These findings demonstrate that neither an N-methyl nor a β-ketone addition attenuates the potency or efficacy of (±)-amphetamine to produce cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects. Second, a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition attenuated both the potency and efficacy of amphetamine, cathinone, and methamphetamine to produce cocaine-like effects and the rank order of sensitivity was cathinone ≥ amphetamine > methamphetamine. Previously, we reported a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition to methcathinone (MDMC) did not alter the potency or efficacy to produce cocaine-like effects (Smith et al. 2016). These results suggest an N-methyl group attenuated the impact of a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition on amphetamine- and cathinone-induced discriminative stimulus effects. Overall, these behavioral results in non-human primates suggest that the impact of chemical structure modifications on the subjective-like discriminative stimulus effects of cathinone analogs cannot be predicted based on results from similar structural modifications to its amphetamine analog.

Effects of amphetamine, cathinone, and their 3,4-methylenedioxy analogs

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report of (±)-amphetamine to produce cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects. Racemic amphetamine was approximately threefold less potent than (+)-amphetamine to produce cocaine-like effects (Banks et al. 2014; Banks et al. 2015). The dose-dependent and complete substitution profile of (±)-cathinone for a cocaine discriminative stimulus in the present study was consistent with previous studies in both rats (Huang and Wilson 1986; Young and Glennon 1993) and monkeys (Garza and Johanson 1983). The present results extend upon these previous findings by reporting the time course of (±)-amphetamine and (±)-cathinone discriminative stimulus effects. At equivalent doses, (±)-amphetamine produced a longer duration of cocaine-like stimulus effects than (±)-cathinone did. However, (±)-cathinone and (±)-amphetamine were equipotent to produce cocaine-like effects in monkeys based on the ED50 values. The overall consistency of the present results with the scientific literature provide an empirical foundation upon which to determine the effects of a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition.

The 3,4-methylenedioxy addition attenuated the cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of both (±)-cathinone and (±)-amphetamine in the present study. The (±)-MDC substitution profile for cocaine in the present study is generally consistent with a previous (±)-MDC study in rats trained to discriminative (+)-amphetamine (Dal Cason et al. 1997) and extends upon this finding by determining the time course of MDC effects and by testing in non-human primates. Moreover, the present (±)-MDA results are also consistent with and extend upon previous MDA results in monkeys trained to discriminate (+)-amphetamine (Kamien et al. 1986) by reporting the time course of MDA effects. In rats, (±)-MDA completely cross-generalizes with cocaine (Glennon and Young 1984b; Glennon et al. 1984) and (+)-amphetamine (Glennon and Young 1984a). Direct potency and time course comparisons between the present (±)-MDC and (±)-MDA results are complicated by the lower than expected (±)-MDC potency and limited drug supply that prohibited testing in all four monkeys and the assessment of larger doses. However, in the three monkeys that did receive both (±)-MDC and (±)-MDA, both (±)-MDC and (±)-MDA produced full substitution in one monkey, only (±)-MDA produced full substitution in another monkey, and neither (±)-MDC nor (±)-MDA produced full substitution in the last monkey. In summary, the present results are generally consistent with and extend upon the range of experimental conditions upon which a 3,4-methylendioxy addition attenuates the abuse-related subjective-like effects of amphetamine and cathinone and highlight the importance of individual subject differences.

Effects of methamphetamine and its 3,4-methylenedioxy analog

The dose-dependent and complete substitution profile of (±)-methamphetamine in the present study extends upon results in rats trained to discriminate (+)-amphetamine (Glennon et al. 1987) in two ways. First, the present results extend these previous findings to non-human primates trained to discriminate cocaine. Furthermore, racemic methamphetamine and (+)-methamphetamine were equipotent to produce cocaine-like stimulus effects in monkeys (Negus et al. 2007). Second, the present results determined the time course of (±)-methamphetamine discriminative stimulus effects, which had not been previously reported to the best of our knowledge. (±)-Methamphetamine produced longer lasting cocaine-like effects compared to our previously published (±)-methcathinone results (Smith et al. 2016) at equivalent doses. Also similar was that (±)-methamphetamine and (±)-methcathinone were equipotent to produce cocaine-like effects in monkeys, and this result was consistent with the equipotency of both compounds to produce (+)-amphetamine-like discriminative stimulus effects in rats (Glennon 1986; Glennon et al. 1987). The overall consistency of the present racemic methamphetamine results with the scientific literature provided an empirical foundation upon which to determine the effects of a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition.

The substitution profile of (±)-MDMA for a cocaine discriminative stimulus in nonhuman primates has not been previously reported to the best of our knowledge. The present (±)-MDMA results demonstrating full cocaine-like effects in three out of four monkeys are somewhat consistent with previous cocaine discrimination studies in rats reporting that (±)-MDMA produced significant, but partial, substitution (Khorana et al. 2004; Kueh and Baker 2007). In (±)-MDMA-trained rats, cocaine produced full substitution in one study (Khorana et al. 2004) but only partial substitution in another study (Kueh and Baker 2007). Whether these inconsistent MDMA results are the consequence of species differences or cocaine training dose differences remains to be fully elucidated. Furthermore, the present (±)-MDMA results are also generally consistent with previous (±)-MDMA results in monkeys trained to discriminate (+)-amphetamine (Kamien et al. 1986), although (±)-MDMA produced full substitution for (+)-amphetamine in all monkeys. One potential reason for the differential MDMA substitution profile between cocaine and (+)-amphetamine in nonhuman primates could be related to the noradrenergic component of the training drug discriminative stimulus. For example, the selective norepinephrine transporter (NET) inhibitor nisoxetine produced full substitution in an (+)-amphetamine (Kamien and Woolverton 1989), but not cocaine (Kleven et al. 1990), discrimination procedure in rhesus monkeys. However, the noradrenergic component of the cocaine discriminative stimulus is dose-dependent, such that NET inhibitors produced greater substitution for smaller compared to larger cocaine training doses in squirrel monkeys (Spealman 1995). The present cocaine training dose (0.32 mg/kg) was toward the larger cocaine training doses used in the squirrel monkey study. Given that (±)-MDMA is approximately equipotent at NET and SERT (Rothman et al. 2001), the present results and the existing literature provide further evidence for a noradrenergic component of the MDMA discriminative stimulus.

Implications

Overall, these structure activity relationship studies suggest three main conclusions regarding the behavioral consequences of structural modifications involving an N-methyl, β-ketone, or 3,4-methylenedioxy addition alone or in combination on the subjective-like discriminative stimulus effects of amphetamine. First, an N-methyl addition to both (±)-amphetamine and β-ketoamphetamine (±-cathinone) slightly increased the potency to produce cocaine-like stimulus effects, although only (±)-methcathinone was significantly more potent than cathinone as denoted by the non-overlapping confidence limits. Furthermore, the N-methyl addition shortened the duration of cocaine-like effects to a greater extent for cathinone (Smith et al. 2016) compared to amphetamine across equally efficacious doses. Second, a β-ketone addition did not alter the potency of either amphetamine or methamphetamine to produce cocaine-like stimulus effects but tended to shorten the duration of cocaine-like effects. Finally, a 3,4-methylenedioxy addition attenuated the cocaine-like discriminative stimulus effects of amphetamine and cathinone to a greater extent than of methamphetamine and methcathinone (Smith et al. 2016). Overall, these results suggest that the behavioral pharmacology of substituted cathinone analogs cannot be solely predicted based on their amphetamine counterparts and that the substituent on the N-terminus might be important for determining the expression of subjective-like discriminative stimulus effects.