Skip to main content

Continuity and the Flow of Time: A Cognitive Science Perspective

  • Chapter
Philosophy and Psychology of Time

Part of the book series: Studies in Brain and Mind ((SIBM,volume 9))

Abstract

Modern tools and methods of cognitive science, such as brain imaging or computational modeling, can provide new insights for age-old philosophical questions regarding the nature of temporal experience. This chapter aims to provide an overview of functional consciousness and time perception in brains and minds (Sect. 8.2), and to describe a computational cognitive architecture partially implementing these phenomena (Sects. 8.3, 8.4 and 8.5), and its comparison with data from human behavioral experiments (Sect. 8.6).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    Confirmed by Christof Koch in personal communication with one of the authors.

  2. 2.

    Masking involves the elimination of the visibility of one briefly presented stimulus by the presentation of a second brief stimulus (the “mask”).

  3. 3.

    Binocular rivalry involves presentation of different visual stimuli to the left and right eyes of subjects. In this paradigm, conscious perception alternates between the two stimuli—see also Fig. 8.1.

  4. 4.

    More specifically, LIDA often uses directed graphs composed of nodes and links to represent items (nodes) and relationships between them (links).

  5. 5.

    A codelet is a small, single purpose, independently running piece of code, corresponding to a process in Baars’ Global Workspace Theory. Structure building codelets build structures of nodes and links.

References

  • Allport, David A. 1968. Phenomenal simultaneity and the perceptual moment hypothesis. British Journal of Psychology 59(4): 395–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Michael L. 2003. Embodied cognition: A field guide. Artificial Intelligence 149(1): 91–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, Adam K. 2005. Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 134(2): 258–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J. 1997. In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J. 2005. Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research 150: 45–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baars, Bernard J., Stan Franklin, and Thomas Zoega Ramsoy. 2013. Global workspace dynamics: Cortical “binding and propagation” enables conscious contents. Frontiers in Psychology 4: 200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, Alan D., and Graham J. Hitch. 1974. Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 8: 47–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1999. Perceptions of perceptual symbols. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(4): 637–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2008. Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, Ned. 1995. How many concepts of consciousness? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18(2): 272–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Block, Richard A. (ed.). 2014. Cognitive models of psychological time. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boltz, Marilyn G. 1995. Effects of event structure on retrospective duration judgments. Perception & Psychophysics 57(7): 1080–1096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buhusi, Catalin V., and Warren H. Meck. 2005. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6(10): 755–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casali, Adenauer G., Olivia Gosseries, Mario Rosanova, Mélanie Boly, Simone Sarasso, Karina R. Casali, Silvia Casarotto, Marie-Aurélie Bruno, Steven Laureys, and Giulio Tononi. 2013. A theoretically based index of consciousness independent of sensory processing and behavior. Science Translational Medicine 5(198): 198ra105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clay, E.R. 1882. The alternative: A study in psychology. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conway, Martin A. 2002. Sensory-perceptual episodic memory and its context: Autobiographical memory. In Episodic memory: New directions of research, ed. Alan Baddeley, Martin Conway, and John Aggleton. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, Francis, and Christof Koch. 1990. Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences 2: 263–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dainton, Barry. 2010. Temporal consciousness. In The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford: The Metaphysics Research Lab, Center for the Study of Language and Information, Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, Stanislas, and Jean-Pierre Changeux. 2003. Neural mechanisms for access to consciousness. In The cognitive neurosciences III, ed. Michael Gazzaniga, 1145–1157. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, Stanislas, Jean-Pierre Changeux, Lionel Naccache, Jérôme Sackur, and Claire Sergent. 2006. Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(5): 204–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene, Stanislas, Lucie Charles, Jean-Rémi King, and Sébastien Marti. 2014. Toward a computational theory of conscious processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 25: 76–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, Daniel C. 1988. Quining qualia. In Consciousness in contemporary science, ed. A. Marcel and E. Bisiach, 42–77. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dieter, Kevin C., and Duje Tadin. 2011. Understanding attentional modulation of binocular rivalry: A framework based on biased competition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 155. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doesburg, Sam M., Jessica J. Green, John J. McDonald, and Lawrence M. Ward. 2009. Rhythms of consciousness: Binocular rivalry reveals large-scale oscillatory network dynamics mediating visual perception. PloS One 4(7): e6142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drescher, Gary L. 1991. Made-up minds: A constructivist approach to artificial intelligence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dux, Paul E., and René Marois. 2009. The attentional blink: A review of data and theory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71(8): 1683–1700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagleman, David M. 2008. Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 18(2): 131–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, Gerald M., and Giulio Tononi. 2000. A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagination. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, Gerald M., Joseph A. Gally, and Bernard J. Baars. 2011. Biology of consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology 2: 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. Anders, and Walter Kintsch. 1995. Long-term working memory. Psychological Review 102(2): 211–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faghihi, Usef, Ryan McCall, and Stan Franklin. 2012. A computational model for attentional learning in cognitive agents. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures 2: 25–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, Stan, Tamas Madl, Sidney D’Mello, and Javier Snaider. 2013. LIDA: A systems-level architecture for cognition, emotion, and learning. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development. doi:10.1109/TAMD.2013.2277589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, Walter J. 2002. The limbic action-perception cycle controlling goal-directed animal behavior. Neural Networks 3: 2249–2254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, Joaquín M. 2002. Physiology of executive functions: The perception-action cycle. In Principles of frontal lobe function, ed. Donald T. Stuss and Robert T. Knight, 96–108. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, Joaquín M. 2004. Upper processing stages of the perception-action cycle. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 143–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, Joaquín M. 2006. The cognit: A network model of cortical representation. International Journal of Psychophysiology 60(2): 125–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuster, Joaquín M., and Steven L. Bressler. 2012. Cognit activation: A mechanism enabling temporal integration in working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16(4): 207–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, John, Russell M. Church, and Warren H. Meck. 1984. Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 423(1): 52–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, John, Chara Malapani, Corby L. Dale, and C.R. Gallistel. 1997. Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: Advances and challenges. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 7(2): 170–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, Arthur M. 1997. What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20(1): 41–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, Arthur M., and David A. Robertson. 2000. Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language 43(3): 379–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, Simon. 2010. Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 72(3): 561–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hintzman, Douglas L. 2011. Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the coordinates of truth. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6(3): 253–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ivry, Richard B., and John E. Schlerf. 2008. Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(7): 273–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, William. 1890. The principles of psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Karmarkar, Uma R., and Dean V. Buonomano. 2007. Timing in the absence of clocks: Encoding time in neural network states. Neuron 53(3): 427–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Chai-Youn, and Randolph Blake. 2005. Watercolor illusion induced by synesthetic colors. Perception 34(12): 1501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, Christof. 2004. The quest for consciousness. Englewood: Roberts & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kouider, Sid, and Stanislas Dehaene. 2007. Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 362(1481): 857–875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langley, Pat, John E. Laird, and Seth Rogers. 2008. Cognitive architectures: Research issues and challenges. Cognitive Systems Research 10: 141–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lau, Hakwan C., and Richard E. Passingham. 2007. Unconscious activation of the cognitive control system in the human prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 27(21): 5805–5811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, Kurt. 1951. In Field theory in social science: Selected theoretical papers, ed. D. Cartwright. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, Laura D., Veronica S. Weiner, Eran A. Mukamel, Jacob A. Donoghue, Emad N. Eskandar, Joseph R. Madsen, William S. Anderson, Leigh R. Hochberg, Sydney S. Cash, and Emery N. Brown. 2012. Rapid fragmentation of neuronal networks at the onset of propofol-induced unconsciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109(49): E3377–E3386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madl, Tamas, and Stan Franklin. 2012. A LIDA-based model of the attentional blink. In ICCM 2012 proceedings, 283–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madl, Tamas, Bernard J. Baars, and Stan Franklin. 2011. The timing of the cognitive cycle. PloS One 6(4): e14803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, Sander, and Brad Wyble. 2010. The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34(6): 947–957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melloni, Lucia, Carlos Molina, Marcela Pena, David Torres, Wolf Singer, and Eugenio Rodriguez. 2007. Synchronization of neural activity across cortical areas correlates with conscious perception. The Journal of Neuroscience 27(11): 2858–2865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merchant, Hugo, Deborah L. Harrington, and Warren H. Meck. 2013. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Neuroscience 36: 313–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michon, John A. 1990. Implicit and explicit representations of time. In Cognitive models of psychological time, ed. Richard A. Block, 37–58. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, Allen. 1973. You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In Visual information processing, ed. W.G. Chase, 283–308. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palva, Satu, Klaus Linkenkaer-Hansen, Risto Näätänen, and J. Matias Palva. 2005. Early neural correlates of conscious somatosensory perception. The Journal of Neuroscience 25(21): 5248–5258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pitts, Michael A., and Juliane Britz. 2011. Insights from intermittent binocular rivalry and EEG. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Potter, Mary C., Brad Wyble, Rijuta Pandav, and Jennifer Olejarczyk. 2010. Picture detection in rapid serial visual presentation: Features or identity? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 36(6): 1486–1494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, Geraint, Gabriel Kreiman, and Christof Koch. 2002. Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3(4): 261–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reuss, Heiko, Andrea Kiesel, Wilfried Kunde, and Bernhard Hommel. 2011. Unconscious activation of task sets. Consciousness and Cognition 20(3): 556–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Samsonovich, A.V. 2010. Toward a unified catalog of implemented cognitive architectures. In Proceedings of the 2010 conference on biologically inspired cognitive architectures, 195–244. Amsterdam: IOS Press. ISBN: 978-1-60750-660-7

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, Wolf. 2011. Consciousness and neuronal synchronization. In The neurology of consciousness: Cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology, ed. Steven Laureys and Giulio Tononi, 43–52. London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, Aaron. 1999. What sort of architecture is required for a human-like agent? In Foundations of rational agency, ed. Michael Wooldridge and Anand Rao, 35–52. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Snaider, Javier, Ryan McCall, and Stan Franklin. 2012. Time production and representation in a conceptual and computational cognitive model. Cognitive Systems Research 13(1): 59–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, M. 2006. Grouping of brain rhythms in corticothalamic systems. Neuroscience 137(4): 1087–1106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, John M. 1967. The fine structure of psychological time. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 138(2): 623–631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Bockstaele, Bram, Bruno Verschuere, Jan De Houwer, and Geert Crombez. 2010. On the costs and benefits of directing attention towards or away from threat-related stimuli: A classical conditioning experiment. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48: 692–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanRullen, Rufin, and Christof Koch. 2003. Is perception discrete or continuous? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(5): 207–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • VanRullen, Rufin, Benedikt Zoefel, and Barkin Ilhan. 2014. On the cyclic nature of perception in vision versus audition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 369(1641): 20130214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, Francisco J., Evan Thompson, and Eleanor Rosch. 1991. The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warner, Rex. 1963. trans. The Confessions of St. Augustine. New York: A Mentor Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wikimedia. 2009. Structures of brain: Wikimedia commons. http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File: Human–brain.SVG. Accessed 17 Apr 2014.

  • Wikimedia. 2010. Diagram of human brain: Wikimedia commons. http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:BrainCaudatePutamen.svg. Accessed 17 Apr 2014.

  • Wittmann, Marc. 2011. Moments in time. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 5: 66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, Dan. 1992. The role of attention in children’s time perception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 54(3): 355–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, Dan, and Richard A. Block. 1996. The role of attention in time estimation processes. Advances in Psychology 115: 143–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, Dan, and Richard A. Block. 1997. Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science 6: 12–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakay, Dan, Yehoshua Tsal, Masha Moses, and Itzhak Shahar. 1994. The role of segmentation in prospective and retrospective time estimation processes. Memory & Cognition 22(3): 344–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zarco, Wilbert, Hugo Merchant, Luis Prado, and Juan Carlos Mendez. 2009. Subsecond timing in primates: Comparison of interval production between human subjects and rhesus monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology 102(6): 3191–3202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tamas Madl .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Madl, T., Franklin, S., Snaider, J., Faghihi, U. (2016). Continuity and the Flow of Time: A Cognitive Science Perspective. In: Mölder, B., Arstila, V., Øhrstrøm, P. (eds) Philosophy and Psychology of Time. Studies in Brain and Mind, vol 9. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-22195-3_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics