Skip to main content

Neuroscience and Law: Conceptual and Practical Issues

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Neuroscience and Law
  • 1039 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter will address the potential contributions of neuroscience to law, with special emphasis on criminal justice and criminal responsibility because these are the areas that have received the lion’s share of neurolaw attention. The discussion will clearly generalize to other applications, however.

The neurosciences in question are the behavioral neurosciences, such as cognitive, affective and social neuroscience, because these are the types of neuroscience most relevant to law. There have been major advances in these fields since the beginning of the present century when non-invasive functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate brain function became widely available for non-clinical research. The central question for this chapter is the relevance of neuroscience to law. The general conclusion is that it is scarcely useful at present but may become more relevant as the science progresses.

Many readers of this chapter may not be lawyers, so the chapter begins with a brief explanation of the meaning of criminal responsibility that will be used throughout. It then speculates about the source of claims for the positive influence of neuroscience. The next section discusses the scientific status of behavioral neuroscience. Then it addresses two radical challenges to current conceptions of criminal responsibility that neuroscience allegedly poses: determinism and the death of agency. The question of the specific relevance of neuroscience to criminal law doctrine, practice and institutions is considered next. This is followed by a discussion of how neuroscience evidence is being used in criminal cases in five different countries, including the United States. The penultimate section points to some areas warranting modest optimism. A brief conclusion follows.

This chapter is an adapted and updated version of, Stephen J. Morse, Neuroscience and Criminal Law, first published in, Larry Alexander and Kimberly Kessler Ferzan, eds., The Palgrave Handbook of Applied Ethics and the Criminal Law, Palgrave Macmillan, 2019. It is reproduced with permission of SNCSC.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
$34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 189.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 249.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    Robinson v California (1962).

  2. 2.

    Morissette v United States (1952).

  3. 3.

    Menninger (1968).

  4. 4.

    The Economist (2002).

  5. 5.

    Pardo and Patterson (2013) and Gabriel (2017).

  6. 6.

    Hunter and Nedelsky (2018).

  7. 7.

    Feldman (2009).

  8. 8.

    Miller v Alabama (2012), n 5.

  9. 9.

    Adolphs (2015), p. 175; McHugh and Slavney (1998), pp. 11–12.

  10. 10.

    Wittgenstein (1953), § 621.

  11. 11.

    Bennett et al. (2009a), Button et al. (2013), Eklund et al. (2016) and Vul et al. (2009). But see Lieberman et al. (2009).

  12. 12.

    Francis (2009); Morse and Newsome (2013), pp. 150, 159–160, 167; Rego (2016).

  13. 13.

    Ioannides (2011).

  14. 14.

    Morse and Newsome (2013), p. 150.

  15. 15.

    Roskies (2013), p. 37; Hong et al. (2019).

  16. 16.

    For sophisticated, systematic and comprehensive reviews of the proper uses and limitations of fMRI, see Logothetis (2008) and Poldrack (2018).

  17. 17.

    Poldrack (2006).

  18. 18.

    Button et al. (2013) and Szucs and Ioannidis (2017).

  19. 19.

    Varoquaux (2018).

  20. 20.

    Faigman et al. (2014).

  21. 21.

    Simmons et al. (2011).

  22. 22.

    Poldrack et al. (2017).

  23. 23.

    Chin (2014).

  24. 24.

    Ioannides (2011).

  25. 25.

    Chin (2014) and Open Science Collaboration (2015), but see Gilbert et al. (2016).

  26. 26.

    Szucs and Ioannidis (2017).

  27. 27.

    Miller (2010).

  28. 28.

    Adolphs (2015).

  29. 29.

    Morse (2011).

  30. 30.

    But see Bennett et al. (2009b) for an amusing exception.

  31. 31.

    E.g., Pereboom and Caruso (2017).

  32. 32.

    Ibid.

  33. 33.

    Morse (2018) and Sehon (2016).

  34. 34.

    Mele (2009, 2014), Moore (2012), Morse (2015), Nachev and Hacker (2015), Schurger et al. (2012) and Schurger and Uithol (2015).

  35. 35.

    Sifferd (2006).

  36. 36.

    Morse (2011).

  37. 37.

    Mudrik and Maoz (2014).

  38. 38.

    Stone (1984).

  39. 39.

    Bennett and Hacker (2003).

  40. 40.

    Morse and Newsome (2013), p. 150.

  41. 41.

    Roskies et al. (2013) and Schweitzer et al. (2011).

  42. 42.

    Morse (2011).

  43. 43.

    Morse and Newsome (2013), p. 150.

  44. 44.

    Greely (2013), p. 120.

  45. 45.

    Husak and Murphy (2013), p. 216.

  46. 46.

    Miller (2010).

  47. 47.

    But see Vilares et al. (2017) for a “proof of concept” exception.

  48. 48.

    People v Weinstein (1992); Morse (1995), p. 527; Davis (2017) (“Spyder Cystkopf” was the pseudonym first used in the literature).

  49. 49.

    Farahany (2015) and Gaudet and Marchant (2016).

  50. 50.

    Alimardani and Chin (2019).

  51. 51.

    Catley and Claydon (2015).

  52. 52.

    Chandler (2015).

  53. 53.

    de Kogel and Westgeest (2015).

  54. 54.

    Farahany (2015), pp. 488–489.

  55. 55.

    See Rakoff (2016) for an analysis by a neuroscientifically informed federal judge.

  56. 56.

    Morse (2004), p. 81. But see Jones (2013) for a contrary view.

  57. 57.

    Ibid.

  58. 58.

    United States v Hinckley (1981), p. 1346.

  59. 59.

    Presidential Commission (2015).

  60. 60.

    Aharoni et al. (2013), p. 6223; Pardini et al. (2014), p. 73.

  61. 61.

    For example, a re-analysis of the Aharoni et al. study, 2013, n 35 by Russell Poldrack, a noted “neuromethodologist” demonstrated that the effect size was tiny. The study used good but not use the best behavioral predictive methods for comparison.

  62. 62.

    Rissman et al. (2010), p. 9849; Rissman et al. (2016), p. 604.

References

  • Adolphs R (2015) The unsolved problems of neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci 19(4):173–175

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aharoni E et al (2013) Neuroprediction of future arrest. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(15):6223–6228

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alimardani A, Chin JM (2019) Neurolaw in Australia: the use of neuroscience in Australian criminal proceedings. NeuroImage 195:384–395

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett MR, Hacker PMS (2003) Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Wiley-Blackwell, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett CM, Wolford GL, Miller MB (2009a) The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 4(4):417–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennett CM, Baird AA, Miller MB et al (2009b) Neural correlates of interspecies perspective taking in the post-mortem Atlantic Salmon: an argument for proper multiple comparisons correction. J Serendipitous Unexpected Results 1(1):S39–S41

    Google Scholar 

  • Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C et al (2013) Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:365–376

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catley P, Claydon L (2015) The use of neuroscientific evidence in the courtroom by those accused of criminal offenses in England and Wales. J Law Biosci 2(3):510–549

    Google Scholar 

  • Chandler J (2015) The use of neuroscientific evidence in Canadian criminal proceedings. J Law Biosci 2:550–579

    Google Scholar 

  • Chin JM (2014) Psychological science’s replicability crisis and what it means for science in the courtroom. Psychol Public Policy Law 2(3):225–238

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis K (2017) The brain defense: murder in Manhattan and the dawn of neuroscience in America’s courtrooms. Penguin, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • de Kogel CH, Westgeest EJMC (2015) Neuroscientific and behavioral genetic information in criminal cases in the Netherlands. J Law Biosci 2:580–560

    Google Scholar 

  • Economist (May 25, 2002) Open your mind. Economist: 93

    Google Scholar 

  • Eklund A, Nichols TE, Knutsson H (2016) Cluster failure: why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113(28):7900–7905

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faigman DL, Monahan J, Slobogin C (2014) Group to individual (g2i) inference in scientific expert testimony. Univ Chic Law Rev 81(2):417–480

    Google Scholar 

  • Farahany NA (2015) Neuroscience and behavioral genetics in US criminal law: an empirical analysis. J Law Biosci 2:485–509

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman R (2009) The role of science in law. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Francis A (2009) Whither DSM-V? Br J Psychiatry 195(5):391–392

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gabriel M (2017) I am not a brain: philosophy of mind for the 21st century. Wiley, Hoboken

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaudet LM, Marchant GE (2016) Under the radar: neuroimaging evidence in the criminal courtroom. Drake Law Rev 64(3):577–661

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert DT, King G, Pettigrew S et al (2016) Comment on “estimating the reproducibility of psychological science”. Science 351(6277):1037a

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greely HT (2013) Mind reading, neuroscience, and the law. In: Morse SJ, Roskies AL (eds) A primer on criminal law and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 120–149

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hong YW, Yoo Y, Han J et al (2019) False-positive neuroimaging: undisclosed flexibility in testing spatial hypotheses allows presenting anything as a replicated finding. NeuroImage 195:384–395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter JD, Nedelsky P (2018) Science and the good: the tragic quest for the foundations of morality. Yale University Press, New Haven

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Husak D, Murphy E (2013) The relevance of the neuroscience of addiction to the criminal law. In: Morse SJ, Roskies AL (eds) A primer on criminal law and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 216–239

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Ioannides JP (2011) Excess significance bias in the literature on brain volume abnormalities. Arch Gen Psychiatry 68(8):773–780

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones OD (2013) Seven ways neuroscience aids law. In: Battro A, Dehaene S, Singer W (eds) Neurosciences and the human person: new perspectives on human activities. Scripta varia, vol 121. Pontifical Academy of Sciences, Vatican City

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman MD, Berkman ET, Wager TD (2009) Correlations in social neuroscience aren’t voodoo: a commentary on Vul et al. Perspect Psychol Sci 4(3):299–307

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logothetis NK (2008) What we can and cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453:869–878

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McHugh PR, Slavney P (1998) The perspectives of psychiatry, 2nd edn. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Mele AR (2009) Effective intentions: the power of conscious will. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mele AR (2014) Free: why science hasn’t disproved free will. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Menninger K (1968) The crime of punishment. The Viking Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA (2010) Mistreating psychology in the decades of the brain. Perspect Psychol Sci 5(6):716–743

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller v Alabama (2012) 132 S Ct 2455

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore MS (2012) Responsible choices, desert-based legal institutions, and the challenges of contemporary neuroscience. Soc Philos Policy 29(1):233–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morisette v United States (1952) 342 US 246

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ (1995) Brain and blame. Georgetown Law J 84(3):527–549

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ (2004) New neuroscience, old problems. In: Garland B (ed) Neuroscience and the law: brain, mind and the scales of justice. Dana Press, New York, pp 157–198

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ (2011) Lost in translation? An essay on law and neuroscience. In: Freeman M (ed) Law and neuroscience, vol 13(28). Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 529–562

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ (2015) Neuroprediction: new technology, old problems. Bioethica Forum 8(4):128–129

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ (2018) The neuroscientific non-challenge to meaning, morals, and purpose. In: Caruso G, Flanagan O (eds) Neuroexistentialism: meaning, morals, and purpose in the age of neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 333–357

    Google Scholar 

  • Morse SJ, Newsome WT (2013) Criminal responsibility, criminal competence, and prediction of criminal behavior. In: Morse SJ, Roskies AL (eds) A primer on criminal law and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 150–178

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mudrik L, Maoz U (2014) “Me & my brain”: exposing neuroscience’s closet dualism. J Cogn Neurosci 27(2):211–221

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nachev P, Hacker P (2015) The neural antecedents to voluntary action: response to commentaries. Cogn Neurosci 6(4):180–186

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Open Science Collaboration (2015) Psychology: estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science 349(6251):aac4716–aac4711

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pardini DA, Raine A, Erickson K et al (2014) Lower amygdala volume in men is associated with childhood aggression, early psychopathic traits, and future violence. Biol Psychiatry 75(1):73–80

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pardo M, Patterson D (2013) Minds, brains, and law: the conceptual foundations of law and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • People v Weinstein (1992) 591 NYS 2d 715

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereboom D, Caruso GD (2017) Hard-incompatibilist existentialism: neuroscience, punishment, and meaning in life. In: Caruso GD, Flanagan O (eds) Neuroexistentialism: meaning, morals, and purpose in the age of neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 193–222

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack R (2006) Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends Cogn Sci 10(2):59–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack R (2013) How well can we predict future criminal acts from fMRI data? Available via Russpoldrack.com. http://www.russpoldrack.org/search?q=aharoni. Accessed 11 Jan 2018

  • Poldrack RA (2018) The new mind readers: what neuroimaging can and cannot reveal about our thoughts. Princeton University Press, Princeton

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J et al (2017) Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci 18:115–126

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Presidential Commission for the Study of Bioethical Issues (2015) Gray matters: topics at the intersection of neuroscience, ethics and society, vol 2. Presidential Commission for the Study of Bioethical Issues, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Rakoff JS (2016) Neuroscience and the law: don’t rush in. N Y Rev Books LXIII:30–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Rego MD (2016) Counterpoint: clinical neuroscience is not ready for clinical use. Br J Psychiatry 208(4):312–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rissman J, Greely HT, Wagner AD (2010) Detecting individual memories through the neural decoding of memory states and past experience. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107(21):9849–9854

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rissman J, Chow TE, Reggente N et al (2016) Decoding fMRI signatures of real-world autobiographical memory retrieval. J Cogn Neurosci 28(4):604–620

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson v California (1962) 370 US 660

    Google Scholar 

  • Roskies AL (2013) Brain imaging techniques. In: Morse SJ, Roskies AL (eds) A primer on criminal law and neuroscience. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 37–74

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Roskies AL, Schweitzer NJ, Saks MJ (2013) Neuroimages in court: less biasing than feared. Trends Cogn Sci 17(3):99–101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schurger A, Uithol S (2015) Nowhere and everywhere: the causal origin of voluntary action. Rev Philos Psychol 6(4):761–778

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schurger A, Sitt JD, Dehaene S (2012) An accumulator model for spontaneous neural activity prior to self-initiated movement. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109(42):E2904

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schweitzer NJ, Saks MJ, Murphy ER et al (2011) Neuroimages as evidence in a mens rea defense: no impact. Psychol Public Policy Law 17(3):357–393

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sehon SR (2016) Free will and action explanation: a non-causal, compatibilist. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sifferd KL (2006) In defense of the use of commonsense psychology in the criminal law. Law Philos 25(6):571–612

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U (2011) False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol Sci 22(11):1359–1366

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone A (1984) Law, psychiatry, and morality. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC

    Google Scholar 

  • Szucs D, Ioannidis J (2017) Empirical assessment of published effect sizes and power in the recent cognitive neuroscience and psychology literature. PLoS Biol 15(3):e2000797

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • United States v Hinckley (1981) 525 F Supp DDC 1342

    Google Scholar 

  • Varoquaux G (2018) Cross-validation failure: small sample sizes lead to large error bars. NeuroImage 180(A):68–77

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vilares I, Wesley MJ, Ahn WH et al (2017) Predicting the knowledge–recklessness distinction in the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114(12):3222–3227

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P et al (2009) Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci 4(3):274–290

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1953) Philosophical investigations. The Macmillan Company, New York

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephen J. Morse .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Morse, S.J. (2020). Neuroscience and Law: Conceptual and Practical Issues. In: D’Aloia, A., Errigo, M.C. (eds) Neuroscience and Law. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38840-9_20

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38840-9_20

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-38839-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-38840-9

  • eBook Packages: Law and CriminologyLaw and Criminology (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics